Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-dh8gc Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-06T05:13:30.586Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

9 - Developmental Dyslexia in Japanese

from Part I - Developmental Dyslexia across Languages and Writing Systems

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  27 September 2019

Ludo Verhoeven
Affiliation:
Radboud Universiteit Nijmegen
Charles Perfetti
Affiliation:
University of Pittsburgh
Kenneth Pugh
Affiliation:
Yale University, Connecticut
Get access

Summary

In this chapter, language universality and language specificity of reading processes especially in Japanese are discussed with a special focus given to typical and atypical literacy acquisition/development by Japanese children. First, a brief introduction to the Japanese writing systems is provided: the syllabic Hiragana and Katakana and the morphographic Kanji. Second, research into the cognitive processes involved in reading Kana and Kanji is introduced, including a discussion on how whole-word-level and sub-word-level processes contribute to the computation of phonology from Kana and Kanji from a language-universality perspective. Third, the prevalence of reading impairments amongst Japanese children is discussed, including how their reading impairments (dyslexia) are manifested in reading Kana and Kanji, drawing attention to the fact that different orthographies manifest children’s reading difficulties differently, linking this to the language specificity. Finally, differences in the neural correlates of reading in different orthographies as well as in dyslexic and normal readers are discussed.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2019

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Amano, S., & Kondo, T. (1999). Nihongo no goitokusei [Lexical properties of Japanese] (Vols. 1–6, NTT Psycholinguistic Database Series). Tokyo: Sanseidō.Google Scholar
Andrews, S. (1982). Phonological recoding: Is the regularity effect consistent? Journal of Memory and Cognition, 10, 565575.Google Scholar
Aylward, E. H., Richards, T. L., Berninger, V. W. et al. (2003). Instructional treatment associated with changes in brain activation in children with dyslexia. Neurology, 61, 212219.Google Scholar
Besner, D., & Hildebrandt, N. (1987). Orthographic and phonological code in the oral reading of Japanese Kana. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 13, 335343.Google Scholar
Bowey, J. A., & Patel, R. K. (1988). Metalinguistic ability and early reading achievement. Applied Psycholinguistics, 9, 367383.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chan, D. W., Ho, C. S. H., Tsang, S. M., Lee, S. H., & Chung, K. K. H. (2006). Exploring the reading–writing connection in Chinese children with dyslexia in Hong Kong. Reading and Writing, 19, 543561.Google Scholar
Elbro, C., Møller, S., & Nielsen, E. M. (1995). Functional reading difficulties in Denmark. A study of adult reading of common text. Reading and Writing, 7, 257276.Google Scholar
Frost, R., Katz, L., & Bentin, S. (1987). Strategies for visual word recognition and orthographic depth: A multilingual comparison. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 13, 104115.Google Scholar
Fushimi, T., Ijuin, M., Patterson, K., & Tatsumi, I. F. (1999). Consistency, frequency, and lexicality effects in naming Japanese Kanji. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25, 382407.Google Scholar
Fushimi, T., Komori, K., & Ikeda, M. et al. (2003). Surface dyslexia in a Japanese patient with semantic dementia: Evidence for similarity-based orthography-to-phonology translation. Neuropsychologia, 41, 16441658.Google Scholar
Gibson, E. J., & Levin, H. (1975). Psychology of reading. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Glushko, R. J. (1979). The organization and activation of orthographic knowledge in reading aloud. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 5, 674691.Google Scholar
Halpern, D. F. (1992). Sex differences in cognitive abilities (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Harm, M. W., & Seidenberg, M. S. (1999). Phonology, reading acquisition, and dyslexia: Insights from connectionist models. Psychological Review, 106, 491528.Google Scholar
Haruhara, N., & Kaneko, M. (2003). The standardized comprehension test of abstract words. Edited by Uno, A.. Tokyo: Intelna-Shuppan,Google Scholar
Haruhara, N., Uno, A., Rankin, Q., & Wydell, T. N. (2014, March). Cognitive abilities and reading/writing attainment in Japanese of Japanese-English bilinguals with monolingual dyslexia in English. Paper presented at the British Dyslexia Association International Conference, Guildford, UK.Google Scholar
Hirose, T. & Hatta, T. (1985). Reading disabilities in Japan: Evidence against the myth of rarity. International Journal of Neuroscience, 26, 249252.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ho, C. S. H., Chan, D. W. O., Lee, S. H., Tsang, S. M., & Luan, V. H. (2004). Cognitive profiling and preliminary subtyping in Chinese developmental dyslexia. Cognition, 91, 4375.Google Scholar
Hulme, C. (1988). The implausibility of low-level visual deficits as a cause of children’s reading difficulties. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 5, 369374.Google Scholar
Ijuin, M., & Wydell, T. N. (2018). A reading model from the perspective of Japanese orthography: Connectionist approach to the hypothesis of granularity and transparency. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 51(5), 490498. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0022219417718200.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Japanese National Research Institute of Special Education. (1996). Report C-28: Categorization of primary school children with LD and a study on remediation methods. Tokyo: Japan.Google Scholar
Jared, D. (2002). Spelling-sound consistency and regularity effects in word naming. Journal of Memory and Language, 46, 723750.Google Scholar
Kaneko, M., Uno, A., Kaga, M. et al. (1997). Developmental dyslexia and dysgraphia: A case report [in Japanese]. No To Hattatsu [Brain and Child Development], 29, 249253.Google Scholar
Kitao, N. (1984). Reading Ability Test TK-1. Tokyo: Taken Press.Google Scholar
Kondo, T., & Wydell, T. N. (2011). Syllable effects during reading 1000 2-Character Kanji words: comparing ON-reading and KUN-reading words. Paper presented at the 14th Japanese Cognitive Neuropsychology Society Annual Conference (24th September 2011), Nagoya, Japan.Google Scholar
Koyama, M. S., Stein, J. F., Stoodley, C. J., & Hansen, P. C. (2011). Functional MRI evidence for the importance of visual short-term memory in logographic reading. European Journal of Neuroscience, 33, 539548.Google Scholar
Kusumi, T. (1992). Meta-memory. In Anzai, Y., Ishizaki, S., Otsu, Y., Hatano, G., & Mizogushi, H. (Eds.), Handbook of cognitive science. Tokyo, Japan: Kyoritsu Shuppan.Google Scholar
Landerl, K., Wimmer, H., & Frith, U. (1997). The impact of orthographic consistency on dyslexia: A German–English comparison. Cognition, 63, 315334.Google Scholar
Makita, K. (1968). The rarity of reading disability in Japanese children. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 38, 599614.Google Scholar
Mann, V. A. (1985). A cross-language perspective in the relation between temporary memory skills and early reading ability. Remedial and Special Education, 6, 3742.Google Scholar
Mattingley, I. G. (1972). Reading, the linguistic process, and linguistic awareness. In Kavanagh, J. F. & Mattingley, I. G. (Eds.), Language by ear and by eye. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Morton, J. & Sasanuma, S. (1984). Lexical access in Japanese. In Henderson, L. (Ed.), Orthographies and Reading: Perspectives from Cognitive Psychology, Neuropsychology and Linguistics (25–42). London: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Muraishi, S. (1972). Acquisition of reading Japanese syllabic characters in pre-school children in Japan. Paper presented at the Twentieth International Congress of Psychology, Tokyo.Google Scholar
Naka, M., & Naoi, H. (1995). The effect of repeated writing on memory. Journal of Memory and Cognition, 23, 201212.Google Scholar
Nakamura, K., Oga, T., Okada, T. et al. (2005). Hemispheric asymmetry emerges at distinct parts of the occipitotemporal cortex for objects, logograms and phonograms: A functional MRI study. Neuroimage, 28, 521528Google Scholar
National Language Research Institute. (1976). Gendai Shinbun no Kanji [A study of Chinese characters in modern newspapers], The National Language Research Institute Report, 56, Tokyo, Japan: Shuei-shuppan.Google Scholar
National Working Party on Dyslexia in Higher Education. (1999). Dyslexia in higher education: Policy, provision and practice. Hull, UK: University of Hull.Google Scholar
Onose, M. (1987). The effect of tracing and copying practice on handwriting skills of Japanese letters in preschool and first grade children. The Japanese Journal of Educational Psychology, 35, 916.Google Scholar
Onose, M. (1988). Effect of the combination of tracing and copying practices on handwriting skills of Japanese letters in preschool and first grade children. The Japanese Journal of Educational Psychology, 36, 129134.Google Scholar
Patterson, K., Suzuki, T., & Wydell, T. N. (1996). Interpreting a case of Japanese phonological alexia: The key is in phonology. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 13, 803822.Google Scholar
Patterson, K., Suzuki, T., Wydell, T. N., & Sasanuma, S. (1995). Progressive aphasia and surface alexia in Japanese. Neurocase, 1, 155165.Google Scholar
Paulesu, E., Demonet, J. F., Fazio, F. et al. (2001). Dyslexia – Cultural diversity and biological unity. Science, 291, 21652167.Google Scholar
Paulesu, E., McCrory, E., Fazio, F. et al. (2000). A cultural effect on brain function. Nature Neuroscience, 3, 9196.Google Scholar
Qian, Y., Deng, Y., Zhao, J., & Bi, H. Y. (2015). Magnocellular-dorsal pathway function is associated with orthographic but not phonological skill: fMRI evidence from skilled Chinese readers. Neuropsychologia, 71, 8490. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.03.024.Google Scholar
Ramus, F. (2003). Developmental dyslexia: Specific phonological deficit or general sensorimotor dysfunction? Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 13, 212218.Google Scholar
Ramus, F., Rosen, S., Dakin, S. C. et al. (2003). Theories of developmental dyslexia: insights from a multiple case study of dyslexic adults. Brain, 126, 841865.Google Scholar
Rastle, K., Havelka, J., Wydell, T. N., Coltheart, M., & Besner, D. (2009). The cross-script length effect: Further evidence challenging PDP models of reading aloud. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 35, 238246.Google Scholar
Raven, J. C. (1976). Coloured progressive matrices: SETS A, AB, B. Oxford, UK: OPP.Google Scholar
Richardson, J. T. E., & Wydell, T. N. (2003). The representation and attainment of students with dyslexia in UK higher education. Reading and Writing, 16, 475503.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Saito, H., Kawakami, M., & Matsuda, H. (1995). Kanji kousei ni okeru buhin (bushu)-on’in taiouhyou [Variety of phonetic components of radical types in complex left-right Kanji]. Jouhou Kagaku Kenkyu, 2, 89115.Google Scholar
Sakamoto, T., & Makita, K. (1973). Japan. In Downing, J. (Ed.), Comparative reading (pp. 440465). New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Sakuma, N., Sasanuma, S., Tatsumi, I. F., & Masaki, S. (1998). Orthography and phonology in reading Japanese Kanji words: Evidence from the semantic decision task with homophones. Journal of Memory and Cognition, 26, 7587.Google Scholar
Sampson, G. (1985). Writing systems. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Sasaki, M. (1987). Why do Japanese write characters in space? International Journal of Behavioural Development, 10, 135149.Google Scholar
Share, D. L., McGee, R., McKenzie, D., Williams, S. M., & Silva, P. A. (1987). Further evidence relating to the distinction between specific reading retardation and general reading backwardness. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 5, 3544.Google Scholar
Shaywitz, S. E., Shaywitz, B. A., Feltcher, J. M., & Escobar, M. D. (1990). Prevalence of reading disability in boys and girls: Results of the Connecticut longitudinal study. Journal of the American Medical Association, 264, 9981002.Google Scholar
Shibahara, N., Zorzi, M., Hill, M. P., Wydell, T. N., & Butterworth, B. (2003). Semantic effects in word naming: Evidence from English and Japanese Kanji. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 56A, 263286.Google Scholar
Siok, W. T., Perfetti, C. A., Jin, Z., & Tan, L. H. (2004). Biological abnormality of impaired reading is constrained by culture. Nature, 431, 7176.Google Scholar
Snowling, M. (2000). Dyslexia (2nd ed.). London: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Stanhope, N., & Parkin, A. J. (1987). Further explorations of the consistency effect in word and nonword pronunciation. Journal of Memory and Cognition, 15, 169179.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stein, J. (2001). The magnocellular theory of developmental dyslexia. Dyslexia, 7, 1236.Google Scholar
Stevenson, H. W., Stigler, J. W., Lucker, G. W. et al. (1982). Reading disabilities: The case of Chinese, Japanese, and English. Child Development, 53, 11641181.Google Scholar
Stoeckel, C., Gough, P. M., Watkins, K. E., Devlin, J. T. (2009). Supramarginal gyrus involvement in visual word recognition. Cortex, 45(9), 10911096.Google Scholar
Strain, E., Patterson, K., & Seidenberg, M. (1995). Semantic effects in single-word naming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21, 11401154.Google Scholar
Takebe, T. (1979). Nihongo no hyoki [The Japanese orthography]. Tokyo, Japan: Kadokawa.Google Scholar
Taraban, R., & McClelland, J. L. (1987). Conspiracy effects in word pronunciation. Journal of Memory and Language, 26, 608631.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Treiman, R., Mullennix, J., Bijeljac-Babic, R., & Richmond-Welty, E. D. (1995). The special role of rimes in the description, use and acquisition of English orthography. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 124, 107136.Google Scholar
Uno, A., Haruhara, N., Kaneko, M. & Wydell, T. N. (2006–2016). Shougakusei no Yomi kaki Screening Kensa – Screening test of reading and writing for Japanese primary school children (STRAW). Tokyo, Japan: Interuna Shuppan.Google Scholar
Uno, A., Kaga, M., & Inagaki, M. (1995). A specific disorder of Kanji writing observed in a learning-disabled child: Cognitive psychological and neuropsychological analysis. No To Hattatsu [The Brain and Child Development], 27, 395400.Google Scholar
Uno, A., & Kamibayashi, Y. (1998). Learning disabled child showing writing disorders with ADHD: Cognitive neuropsychological investigation about writing disorders. Shouji No Seishin To Shinkei [Developmental Neuropsychiatry], 38(2), 117123.Google Scholar
Uno, A., Wydell, T. N., Haruhara, N., Kaneko, M., & Shinya, N. (2009). Relationship between reading/writing skills and cognitive abilities among Japanese primary-school children: Normal readers versus poor readers (dyslexics). Reading & Writing, 22, 755789.Google Scholar
Wang, J.-J., Bi, H.-Y., Gao, L.-Q., & Wydell, T. N. (2010). The visual magnocellular pathway in Chinese-speaking children with developmental dyslexia. Neuropsychologia, 48, 36273633.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wei, T.-Q., Bi, H.-Y., Chen, B.-G. et al. (2014). Developmental changes in the role of different metalinguistic awareness skills in Chinese reading acquisition from preschool to third grade. PLoS One, 9(5), e96240.Google Scholar
Wimmer, H., Mayringer, H., & Landerl, K. (2000). The double-deficit hypothesis and difficulties in learning to read a regular orthography. Journal of Educational Psychology, 92, 668680.Google Scholar
Wydell, T. N. (2003). Dyslexia in Japanese and the ‘Hypothesis of Granularity and Transparency’. In Goulandris, A. (Ed.), Dyslexia in different languages: Cross-linguistic comparisons. London: Whurr Publishers.Google Scholar
Wydell, T. N., & Butterworth, B. (1999). A case study of an English–Japanese bilingual with monolingual dyslexia. Cognition, 70, 273305.Google Scholar
Wydell, T. N., Butterworth, B., & Patterson, K. (1995). The inconsistency of consistency effects in reading: The case of Japanese Kanji. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21, 11551168.Google Scholar
Wydell, T. N., & Kondo, T. (2003). Phonological deficit and the reliance on orthographic approximation for reading: A follow-up study on an English–Japanese bilingual with monolingual dyslexia. Journal of Research in Reading, 26, 3348.Google Scholar
Wydell, T.N. & Kondo, T. (2015). Behavioral and neuroimaging research of reading: a case of Japanese. Current Developmental Disorders Reports, 2(4), 339345.Google Scholar
Wydell, T., Patterson, K., & Humphreys, G. (1993). Phonologically mediated access to meaning for Kanji: Is a ROWS still a ROSE in Japanese Kanji? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 19, 491–514.Google Scholar
Wydell, T. N., Vuorinen, T., Helenius, P., & Salmelin, R. (2003). Neural correlates of letter string length and lexicality during reading in a regular orthography. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 15, 7, 10521062.Google Scholar
Yokosawa, K., & Umeda, M. (1988). Process in human Kanji word recognition. Proceedings of the 1988 IEEE International Conference on Systems, Man, and Cybernetics (pp. 377380). Beijing: IEEE.Google Scholar
Ziegler, J. C., & Goswami, U. (2005). Reading acquisition, developmental dyslexia, and skilled reading across languages: A psycholinguistic grain size theory. Psychological Bulletin, 131, 329.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×