Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-2plfb Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-28T23:20:19.105Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

6 - An Ecocultural Perspective on Cultural Transmission: The Family across Cultures

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 June 2012

Ute Schönpflug
Affiliation:
Freie Universität Berlin
Get access

Summary

INTRODUCTION

Cross-cultural psychology attempts to understand how human behavior is shaped by the cultural context in which it developed. One approach to understanding this relationship is by examining the ecological and cultural settings in which individuals live and the patterns of behavior that emerge in them (Berry, 1976). To explain how a group's culture becomes incorporated in individual behavior, some form of linkage between them needs to be postulated. This connection is to be found in the process of cultural transmission (Cavalli-Sforza & Feldman, 1981), which is seen as the main link among groups of people (i.e., cultures, communities, and families) and the development of individuals (including their overt behaviors and the inferred characteristics that underlie their behavior). Within this process of cultural transmission, three subprocesses have been distinguished: enculturation, socialization, and acculturation (Berry, Poortinga, & Segall, 2002). The first is a general enfolding of the developing individual by one's cultural group, often without specific instruction; the second also takes place within a person's own cultural group but is usually accomplished by way of deliberate shaping (i.e., child-rearing practices) and formal instruction (i.e., education); however, the third results from cultural influences arriving from outside a person's own culture, typically involving both enfolding as for enculturation (e.g., by mass telemedia) and deliberate changing as for socialization (e.g., by formal schooling).

Type
Chapter
Information
Cultural Transmission
Psychological, Developmental, Social, and Methodological Aspects
, pp. 95 - 125
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2008

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Arnett, J. (1995). Broad and narrow socialization: The family in the context of cultural theory. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 57, 617–628.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barry, H., Child, I., & Bacon, M. (1959). Relations of child training to subsistence economy. American Anthropologist, 61, 51–63.Google Scholar
Bennett, J. (1976). The ecological transition. London: Pergamon.Google Scholar
Berry, J. W. (1966). Temne and Eskimo perceptual skills. International Journal of Psychology, 1, 207–229.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berry, J. W. (1967). Independence and conformity in subsistence-level societies. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 7, 415–418.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berry, J. W. (1976). Human ecology and cognitive style: Comparative studies in cultural and psychological adaptation. Oxford, UK: Sage/Halsted.Google Scholar
Berry, J. W. (1979). A cultural ecology of social behaviour. In Berkowitz, L. (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology, Vol. 12 (pp. 177–206). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Berry, J. W. (1985). Cultural psychology and ethnic psychology. In Reyes, I., Lagunes, I. R., & Poortinga, Y. H. (Eds.), From a different perspective (pp. 3–15). Lisse, the Netherlands: Swets & Zeitlinger.Google Scholar
Berry, J. W. (1994a). Ecology of individualism and collectivism. In Kim, U., Triandis, H. C., Kagitcibasi, C., Choi, S-C., & Yoon, G. (Eds.), Individualism and collectivism (pp. 77–84). London: Sage Publications.Google Scholar
Berry, J. W. (1994b). An ecological approach to cultural and ethnic psychology. In Trickett, E. J., Watts, R. J., & Birman, D. (Eds.), Human diversity (pp. 115–141). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.Google Scholar
Berry, J. W. (2001). Contextual studies of cognitive adaptation. In Collis, J. & Messick, S.. (Eds.), Intelligence and personality (pp. 319–334). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Berry, J. W. (2003). Conceptual approaches to acculturation. In Chun, K., Balls-Organista, P., & Marin, G. (Eds.), Acculturation (pp. 17–37). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association Press.Google Scholar
Berry, J. W. (2007). Acculturation. In Grusec, J. E. & Hastings, P. D. (Eds.), Handbook of socialization: Theory and research (pp. 533–558) (2nd ed.). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Berry, J. W., & Cavalli-Sforza, L. L. (1986). Cultural and genetic influences on Inuit art. Report to Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada, Ottawa.Google Scholar
Berry, J. W., Dasen, P. R., & Witkin, H. A. (1983). Developmental theories in cross-cultural perspective. In Alder, L. (Ed.), Cross-cultural research at issue (pp. 13–21). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Berry, J. W., Phinney, J. S., Sam, D. L., & Vedder, P. (2006). Immigrant youth in cultural transition. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Berry, J. W., Poortinga, Y. H., Pandey, J., Dasen, P. R., Saraswathi, T. S., et al. (1997). Handbook of cross-cultural psychology, 3 volumes. Boston: Allyn & Bacon.Google Scholar
Berry, J. W., Poortinga, Y. H., & Segall, M. H. (2002). Cross-cultural psychology: Research and applications (2nd ed.). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Berry, J. W., & Sam, D. L. (1997). Acculturation and adaptation. In Berry, J. W., Segall, M. H., & Kagitcibasi, C. (Eds.), Handbook of cross-cultural psychology, Vol. 3, Social behavior and applications (pp. 291–326). Boston: Allyn & Bacon.Google Scholar
Berry, J. W., Koppel, J. M. H., Sénéchal, C., Annis, R. C., Bahuchet, S., et al. (1986). On the edge of the forest: Cultural adaptation and cognitive development in Central Africa. Lisse, the Netherlands: Swets & Zeitlinger.Google Scholar
Bogardus, E. S. (1925). Measuring social distance. Journal of Applied Sociology, 9, 299–308.Google Scholar
Bond, R., & Smith, P. (1996). Culture and conformity: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 119, 111–137.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. (1985). Why is culture adaptive? Quarterly Review of Biology, 58, 209–214.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Camillieri, C., & Malewska-Peyre, H. (1997). Socialization and identity strategies. In Berry, J. W., Dasen, P., & Saraswathi, T. S. (Eds.), Handbook of cross-cultural psychology, Vol. 2. Basic processes and human development (pp. 41–68). Boston: Allyn & Bacon.Google Scholar
Cavalli-Sforza, L. L., & Feldman, M. W. (1981). Cultural transmission and evolution: A quantitative approach. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google ScholarPubMed
Chun, K., Balls-Organista, P., & Marin, G. (2003). Acculturation: Advances in theory, measurement and applied research. Washington, DC: APA Books.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dasen, P. R. (1975). Concrete operational development in three cultures. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 6, 156–172.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Durkheim, E. (1888). Introduction à la sociologie de la famille. Annales de la Faculté des Lettres de Bordeaux, whole no. 10.
Durkheim, E. (1892). La famille conjugale. Revue Philosophique de la France et de l'Étranger, 90, 1–14.Google Scholar
Feldman, M. W. (1975). The history of the relationship between environment and culture in ethnological thought. Journal of the History of the Behavioural Sciences, 110, 67–81.3.0.CO;2-4>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Forde, D. (1934). Habitat, economy and society. New York: Dutton.Google Scholar
Georgas, J. (1988). An ecological and social cross-cultural model: The case of Greece. In Berry, J. W., Irvine, S. H., & Hunt, E. B. (Eds.), Indigenous cognition: Functioning in cultural context (pp. 105–123). Dordrecht, the Netherlands: Martinus Nijhoff.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Georgas, J. (1993). Ecological-social model of Greek psychology. In Kim, U. & Berry, J. W. (Eds.), Indigenous psychologies (pp. 56–78). Newbury Park, CA: Sage Publications.Google Scholar
Georgas, J. (1999). Family as a context variable in cross-cultural psychology. In Adamopoulos, J. & Kashima, Y. (Eds.), Social psychology and cultural context (pp. 163–175). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage Publications.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Georgas, J., & Berry, J. W. (1995). An ecocultural taxonomy for cross-cultural psychology. Cross-Cultural Research, 29, 121–157.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Georgas, J., Berry, J. W., Vijver, F. J. R., Kagitcibasi, C., & Poortinga, Y. H. (Eds.) (2006). Families across cultures: A 30-nation psychological study. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRef
Georgas, J., Mylonas, K., Bafiti, T., Christakopoulou, S., Poortinga, Y. H., Kagitçibasi, Ç., et al. (2001). Functional relationships in the nuclear and extended family: A 16-culture study. International Journal of Psychology, 36, 289–300.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Georgas, J., Vijver, F. J. R., & Berry, J. W. (2004). The ecocultural framework, ecosocial indicators and psychological variables in cross-cultural research. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 35, 74–96.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Herskovits, M. J. (1949). Man and his works: The science of cultural anthropology. New York: Knopf.Google Scholar
Huntington, E. (1945). Mainsprings of civilization. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Kroeber, A. (1939). Cultural and natural areas of native North America. Berkeley: University of California Press.Google Scholar
Malinowski, B. (2001). Scientific theory of culture and other essays. Selected works, Vol. 9. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Mishra, R. C., Dasen, P. R., & Niraula, S. (2003). Ecology, language, and performance on spatial cognitive tasks. International Journal of Psychology, 38(6), 366–383.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mishra, R. C., Sinha, D., & Berry, J. W. (1996). Ecology, acculturation and psychological adaptation: A study of Advasi in Bihar. Delhi: Sage Publications.Google Scholar
Murdock, G. P. (1969). Correlations of exploitive patterns. In Damas, D. (Ed.), Ecological essays. National Museum of Canada Bulletin No. 230, Anthropological Series No. 86.
Nimkoff, M. F., & Middleton, R. (1960). Types of family and types of economy. American Journal of Sociology, 66, 215–225.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nsamenang, B. (1992). Human development in cultural context. Newbury Park, CA: Sage Publications.Google Scholar
Parsons, T. (1943). The kinship system of the contemporary United States. American Anthropologist, 45, 22–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Parsons, T. (1949). The social structure of the family. In Anshen, R. N. (Ed.), The family: Its functions and destiny. New York: Harper.Google Scholar
Pelto, P. (1968). The difference between “tight” and “loose” societies. Transaction, April, 37–40.Google Scholar
Sam, D. L., & Berry, J. W. (Eds.) (2006). Cambridge handbook of acculturation psychology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M., Krüger, A., & Ratner, H. (1993). Culture learning. Behavioural and Brain Sciences, 16, 495–552.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tylor, E. B. (1871). Primitive culture. London: Murray.Google Scholar
,United Nations Statistics Division (2002a). Demographic, social & housing statistics. Retrieved September 2002 from http://unstats.un.org/unsd.
,United Nations Statistics Division (2002b). Energy statistics. Retrieved September 2002 from http://unstats.un.org/unsd.
Werner, C. M., Brown, B. B., & Altman, I. (1997). Environmental psychology. In Berry, J. W., Segall, M. H., & Kagitcibasi, C. (Eds.), Handbook of cross-cultural psychology, Vol. 3: Social behavior and applications (pp. 255–290). Boston: Allyn & Bacon.Google Scholar
Witkin, H., & Berry, J. W. (1975). Psychological differentiation in cross-cultural perspective. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 6, 4–87.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
World Development Report of the World Bank (2002a). Data & statistics. Retrieved September 2002 from http://www.worldbank.com/data/countrydata.
World Development Report of the World Bank (2002b). Data & statistics. Retrieved September 2002 from http://www.worldbank.com/data/countrydata.

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×