Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-gb8f7 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-26T01:59:51.319Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Chapter 4 - Critical Thinking and the Rejection of Unsubstantiated Claims

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  19 December 2019

Robert J. Sternberg
Affiliation:
Cornell University, New York
Diane F. Halpern
Affiliation:
Claremont McKenna College, California
Get access

Summary

This chapter examines how critical thinking and a dual-process theory of thinking are related to endorsement of unsubstantiated knowledge claims. Rejecting unsubstantiated claims, such as psychological misconceptions and pseudoscience, is associated with being more inclined and better able to think critically. In dual-process theory terms, people who endorse more unsubstantiated claims tend to rely more on intuitive thinking and less on rational-analytic thinking. Helping people to resist making a rapid, intuitive response to unsubstantiated claims and instead to take a slower, more rational, critical thinking approach can help them reject such claims.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2020

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Abrami, P.C., Bernard, R. M., Borokhovski, E., Wade, A., Suerkes, M. A., Tamim, R., & Zhang, D. (2008). Instructional interventions affecting critical thinking skills and dispositions: A stage 1 meta-analysis. Review of Educational Research, 4, 11021134.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Alter, A. L., Oppenheimer, D. M., Epley, N., & Eyre, R. N. (2007). Overcoming intuition: Metacognitive difficulty activates analytic reasoning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 136, 569576.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bailin, S., Case, R., Coombs, J. R., & Daniels, L. B. (1999). Conceptualizing critical thinking. Journal of Curriculum Studies, 31, 285302.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barberia, I., Tubuau, E., Matute, H., & Rodriguez-Ferreiro, J. (2018). A short educational intervention diminishes causal illusions and specific paranormal beliefs in undergraduates. PLoS ONE, 123, e0191907. Online. DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0191907Google Scholar
Baron, J. (2008). Thinking and Deciding. (4th ed.). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Bensley, D. A. (1998). Critical thinking in psychology: A unified skills approach. Pacific Grove, CA: Brooks/Cole.Google Scholar
Bensley, D. A. (2006). Why great thinkers sometimes fail to think critically. Skeptical Inquirer, 30, 4752.Google Scholar
Bensley, D. A. (2011). Rules for reasoning revisited: Toward a scientific conception of critical thinking. In Horvath, C. P. & Forte, J. M. (Eds.), Critical thinking (pp. 135). Hauppauge, NY: Nova Science Publishers.Google Scholar
Bensley, D. A. (2016). The critical thinking in psychology assessment battery. Manuscript.Google Scholar
Bensley, D. A. (2018). Critical thinking in psychology and everyday life: A guide to effective thinking. New York: Worth Publishers.Google Scholar
Bensley, D. A., & Lilienfeld, S. O. (2015). What is a psychological misconception? Moving towards an empirical answer. Teaching of Psychology, 42, 282292.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bensley, D. A., & Lilienfeld, S. O. (2017). Psychological misconceptions: Recent scientific advances and unresolved issues. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 27, 377382.Google Scholar
Bensley, D. A., Lilienfeld, S. O., & Powell, L. A. (2014). A new measure of psychological misconceptions: Relations with academic background, critical thinking, and acceptance of paranormal and pseudoscientific claims. Learning and Individual Differences, 36, 918.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bensley, D. A., Lilienfeld, S. O., Rowan, K. A., Masciocchi, C. M., & Grain, F. (2019a). The generality of belief in unsubstantiated claims. Applied Cognitive Psychology. DOI:10.1002/acp.3581Google Scholar
Bensley, D. A., Lilienfeld, S. O., Rowan, K. A., Masciocchi, C. M., & Watkins, C. T. (2019b). Dispositional predictors of the generality of belief in unsubstantiated claims. In preparation.Google Scholar
Bensley, D. A., Masciocchi, C. M., & Rowan, K. A. (2019). A comprehensive assessment of the impact of explicit critical thinking instruction on the recognition of thinking errors and psychological misconceptions. In preparation.Google Scholar
Bensley, D. A., Raney, K., Lilienfeld, S. O., & Kuehne, S. (2016). What do psychology students know about what they know in psychology? Scholarship of the Teaching and Learning of Psychology, 1, 283297.Google Scholar
Bensley, D. A., Raney, K., Masciocchi, C., Murtagh, M. P., Lilienfeld, S. O., Kuehne, S., & Bernhardt, P. C. (2015, May). All psychological misconceptions are not the same: The role of confidence, frequency, and experience. Presented at the 27th annual meeting of the Association for Psychological Science, New York City.Google Scholar
Bensley, D. A., & Spero, R. A. (2014). Improving critical thinking skills and metacognitive monitoring through direct infusion. Thinking Skills and Creativity, 12, 5568.Google Scholar
Bransford, J. D., & Johnson, M. K. (1972). Contextual prerequisites for understanding: Some investigations of comprehension and recall. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 11, 717726.Google Scholar
Brotherton, R. (2015). Suspicious Minds: Why we believe conspiracy theories. New York: Bloomsbury.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brotherton, R., & French, C. C. (2017). Conspiracy theories. In Groome, D. & Roberts, R., Parapsychology: The science of unusual experience (2nd ed.) (pp. 158176). London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Brotherton, R., French, C. C., & Pickering, A. D. (2013). Measuring belief in conspiracy theories: The generic conspiracist belief scale. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 115.Google Scholar
Bruder, M., Haffke, P., Neave, N., Nouripanah, N., & Imhoff, R. (2013). Measuring individual differences in generic beliefs in conspiracy theories across cultures: Conspiracy mentality questionnaire. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 225251.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burke, B. L., Sears, S. R., Kraus, S., & Roberts-Cady, S. (2014). Critical analysis: A comparison of critical thinking changes in psychology and philosophy classes. Teaching of Psychology, 41, 2836.Google Scholar
Byrnes, J. P., & Dunbar, K. N. (2014). The nature and development of critical-analytic thinking. Educational Psychology Review, 26(4), 477493. DOI:10.1007/s10648-014-9284-0Google Scholar
Cook, J., Oreskes, N., Doran, P. T., Anderegg, W. R. L., Verheggen, B., Maibach, E. W., Carlton, J. S., Lewandowsky, S., Skuce, A. G., Green, S. A., Nuccitelli, D., Jacobs, P., Richardson, M., Winkler, B., Painting, R., & Rice, K. (2016). Consensus on consensus: A synthesis of consensus estimates on human-caused global warming. Environmental Research Letters, 11(4). DOI:10.1088/1748-9326/11/4/048002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Darwin, H., Neave, N., & Holmes, J. (2011). Belief in conspiracy theories: The role of paranormal belief, paranoid ideation, and schizotypy. Personality and Individual Differences, 50, 12891293.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Della Sala, S. (2007). Tall tales about the mind & brain: Separating fact from fiction. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Douglas, K. M., Sutton, R. M., Callan, M. J., Dawtry, R. J., & Harvey, A. J. (2016). Someone is pulling the strings: Hypersensitive agency detection and belief in conspiracy theories. Thinking & Reasoning, 22, 5777.Google Scholar
Drinkwater, K., Dagnall, N., & Parker, A. (2012). Reality testing, conspiracy theories, and paranormal beliefs. Journal of Parapsychology, 76, 5778.Google Scholar
Ennis, R. H., (1987). A taxonomy of critical thinking dispositions and abilities. In Baron, J., & Sternberg, R. (Eds.), Teaching thinking skills: Theory and practice (pp. 926). New York: W. H. Freeman.Google Scholar
Ennis, R. H., & Millmann, J. (1985). Cornell critical thinking test. Pacific Grove, CA: Midwest Publications.Google Scholar
Epstein, S. (2008). Intuition from the perspective of cognitive-experiential self-theory. In Plessner, H., Betsch, C., & Betsch, T (Eds.), Intuition in judgment and decision making (pp. 2337). New York: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Evans, J. St. B.T., & Stanovich, K. E. (2013). Dual-process theories of higher cognition: Advancing the debate. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 8(3), 223241. DOI:10.1177/1745691612460685CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Finegold, L., & Flamm, B. L. (2006). Magnet therapy: Extraordinary claims but no proved benefits. British Medical Journal, 332, 4.Google Scholar
Forsyth, D. R., Lawrence, N. K., Burnette, J. L., & Baumeister, R. F. (2007). Attempting to improve the academic performance of struggling college students by bolstering their self-esteem: An intervention that backfired. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 26(4), 447459. DOI:10.1521/jscp.2007.26.4.447Google Scholar
Frederick, S. (2005). Cognitive reflection and decision making. Journal of Economic Perspectives, 19, 2542.Google Scholar
French, C. C. (2018). Reflections on pseudoscience and parapsychology. In Kaufman, A. B. & Kaufman, J. C. (Eds.), Pseudoscience: The conspiracy against science (pp. 375391). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Gardner, R. M., & Hund, R. M. (1983). Misconceptions of psychology among academicians. Teaching of Psychology, 10, 2022.Google Scholar
Goertzel, T. (1994). Belief in conspiracy theories. Political Psychology, 15, 731742.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Guzzetti, B. J. (2000). Learning counter-intuitive science concepts: What have we learned from over a decade of research? Reading & Writing Quarterly, 16(2), 8998. DOI:10.1080/105735600277971Google Scholar
Halpern, D. F. (1998). Teaching critical thinking for transfer across domains: Dispositions, skills, structure training, and metacognitive monitoring. American Psychologist, 53, 449455.Google Scholar
Halpern, D. F. (2014). Thought and knowledge: An introduction to critical thinking (5th ed.). New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Harambam, J., & Aupers, S. (2016). ‘I am not a conspiracy theorist’: Relational identifications in the Dutch conspiracy milieu. Cultural Sociology, 11(1), 113129. DOI:10.1177/1749975516661959Google Scholar
Polls, Harris. (2013). Americans’ belief in God, miracles, and heaven decline. Online. http://www.harrisinteractive.com/Newsroom/HarrisPolls/tabid/447/ctl/ReadCustomGoogle Scholar
Herculano-Houzel, S. (2002). Do you know your brain? A survey on public neuroscience literacy at the closing of the decade of the brain. The Neuroscientist, 8, 98110.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hergovich, A., & Arendasy, M. (2005). Critical thinking ability and belief in the paranormal. Personality and Individual Differences, 38, 18051812.Google Scholar
Hines, T. (2003). Pseudoscience and the Paranormal (2nd ed.). Amherst, NY: Prometheus Books.Google Scholar
Hughes, S., Lyddy, F., & Lambe, S. (2013). Misconceptions about psychological science: A review. Psychology Learning & Teaching, 12, 2031.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson, S. P. (2013). The faces of Lee Harvey Oswald: The evolution of an alleged assassin. Lanham, UK: Lexington Books.Google Scholar
Kahneman, D. (2011). Thinking fast and slow. New York: Farrar, Straus, & Giroux.Google Scholar
Kowalski, P., & Taylor, A. (2009). The effect of refuting misconceptions in the introductory psychology class. Teaching of Psychology, 36, 153159.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kowalski, P., & Taylor, A. K. (2017). Reducing students’ misconceptions with refutational teaching: For long-term retention, comprehension matters. Scholarship of Teaching and Learning in Psychology, 3, 90100.Google Scholar
Leonard, C. A., & Williams, R. J. (2018). Fallacious beliefs: Gambling specific and belief in the paranormal. Canadian Journal of Behavioral Science, 51(1), 111. DOI:10.1037/cbs0000113Google Scholar
Lewandowsky, S., Ecker, U. K., Seifert, C. M., Schwartz, N., & Cook, J. (2012). Misinformation and its correction: Continued influence and successful debiasing. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 13, 106131.Google ScholarPubMed
Lewandowsky, S., & Oberauer, K. (2016). Motivated rejection of science. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 25(4), 217222. DOI:10.1177/0963721416654436Google Scholar
Lilienfeld, S. O. (2007). Psychological treatments that cause harm. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 2(1), 5370.Google Scholar
Lilienfeld, S. O., Lynn, S. J., Namy, L. L., & Woolf, N. J. (2018). Psychology: From inquiry to understanding (4th ed.). Boston: Pearson.Google Scholar
Lilienfeld, S. O., Lynn, S. J., Ruscio, J., & Beyerstein, B. L. (2010). 50 great myths of popular psychology. Chichester, UK: Wiley-Blackwell.Google Scholar
Lobato, E., Mendoza, J., Sims, V., & Chin, M. (2014). Examining the relationship between conspiracy theories, paranormal beliefs, and pseudoscience acceptance among a university population. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 28, 617625.Google Scholar
Manza, L., Hilperts, K., Hindley, L., Marco, C., Santan, A., & Hawk, M. V. (2010). Exposure to science is not enough: The influence of classroom experiences on belief in paranormal phenomena. Teaching of Psychology, 37, 165171.Google Scholar
Marin, L. M., & Halpern, D. F. (2011). Pedagogy for developing critical thinking in adolescents: Explicit instruction produces the greatest gains. Thinking Skills and Creativity, 6, 113.Google Scholar
McCaffrey, P. (2012). Conspiracy theories: The reference shelf. Ipswich, MA: H. W. Wilson.Google Scholar
McCutcheon, L. E., Apperson, J. M., Hanson, E., & Wynn, V. (1992). Relationships among critical thinking skills, academic achievement, and misconceptions about psychology. Psychological Reports, 71, 635639.Google Scholar
McLean, C. P., & Miller, N. A. (2010). Changes in critical thinking skills following a course in science and pseudoscience: A quasi-experimental study. Teaching of Psychology, 37, 8590.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moscovici, S. (1987). The conspiracy mentality. In Graumann, C. F. & Moscovici, S. (Eds.). Changing conceptions of conspiracy (pp. 151169). New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Myers, D. G., & DeWall, C. N. (2018). Psychology in modules (12th ed.). New York: Worth Publishers.Google Scholar
Oliver, J. E., & Wood, T. J. (2014). Conspiracy theories and the paranoid style(s) of mass opinion. American Journal of Political Science, 58, 952966.Google Scholar
Orosz, G., Kreko, P., Paskuj, B., Toth-Kiraly, I., Bothe, B., & Roland-Levy, C. (2016). Changing conspiracy beliefs through rationality and ridiculing. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, Article 1525. DOI:10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01525Google Scholar
Owens, M., & McGowan, I. W. (2006). Madness and the moon: The lunar cycle and psychopathology. German Journal of Psychology, 2006(9), 123127. Online. http://www.gjpsy.uni-goettingen.de/gjp-article-owens.pdfGoogle Scholar
Pacini, R., & Epstein, S. (1999). The relation of rational and experiential information processing styles to personality, basic beliefs, and the ratio-bias phenomenon. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76(6), 972987. DOI:10.1037//0022-3514.76.6.972Google Scholar
Pennycook, G., Cheyne, J. A., Barr, N., Koehler, D. J., & Fugelsang, J. A. (2015). On the reception and detection of pseudo-profound bullshit. Judgment and Decision Making, 10, 549563.Google Scholar
Pigliucci, M., & Boudry, M. (Eds.). (2013). Philosophy of pseudoscience: Reconsidering the demarcation problem. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Roe, C. A. (1999). Critical thinking and belief in the paranormal: A re-evaluation. British Journal of Psychology, 90, 8598.Google Scholar
Rotton, J., & Kelly, I. (1985). Much ado about the full moon: A meta-analysis of lunar-lunacy research. Psychological Bulletin, 97, 286306.Google Scholar
Schick, T., & Vaughan, L. (2014). How to think about weird things: Critical thinking for a new age (7th ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Stahl, T., & Van Prooijen, J. (2018). Epistemic rationality: Skepticism toward unfounded beliefs requires sufficient cognitive ability and motivation to be rational. Personality and Individual Differences, 122, 155163.Google Scholar
Stahl, T., Zaal, M. P., & Skitka, L. J. (2016). Moralized rationality: Relying on logic and evidence in the formation and evaluation of belief can be seen as a moral issue. PLoS ONE, 11(11). DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0166332Google Scholar
Stanovich, K. E. (2013). How to think straight about psychology (10th ed.). Boston: Allyn & Bacon.Google Scholar
Stanovich, K. E., & West, R. F. (2007). Natural myside bias is independent of cognitive ability. Thinking & Reasoning, 13, 225247.Google Scholar
Swami, V., Coles, R., Steiger, S., Pietschnig, J., Furnham, A., Rehim, S., & Voracek, M. (2011). Conspiracist ideation, in Britain and Austria: Evidence of a monological belief system and associations between individual psychological differences and real-world and fictitious conspiracy theories. British Journal of Psychology, 102, 443463.Google Scholar
Swami, V., Voracek, M., Steiger, S., Tran, U. S., & Furnham, A. (2014). Analytic thinking reduces belief in conspiracy theories. Cognition, 33, 572575.Google Scholar
Svedholm, A. M., & Lindeman, M. (2013). The separate roles of the reflective mind and involuntary inhibitory control in gatekeeping paranormal beliefs and the underlying intuitive confusions. British Journal of Psychology, 13, 303319.Google Scholar
Taylor, A. K., & Kowalski, P. (2004). Ability and critical thinking as predictors of change in students’ psychological misconceptions. Journal of Instructional Psychology, 31, 297303.Google Scholar
Thomson, K. S., & Oppenheimer, D. M. (2016). Investigating an alternate form of the cognitive reflection test. Judgment and Decision Making, 11, 99113.Google Scholar
Tobacyk, J. J. (2004). A revised paranormal belief scale. The International Journal of Transpersonal Studies, 23, 9498.Google Scholar
Tobacyk, J. J., & Milford, G. (1983). Belief in paranormal phenomena: Assessment instrument development and implications for personality functioning. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44, 1029–37.Google Scholar
Van Prooijen, J. (2017). Why education predicts decreased belief in conspiracy theories. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 31, 5058.Google Scholar
Van Prooijen, J., & Van Vugt, M. (2018). Conspiracy theories: Evolved functions and psychological mechanisms. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 13, 770788.Google Scholar
Van der Linden, S. (2015). The conspiracy-effect: Exposure to conspiracy theories (about global warming) leads to decreases pro-social behavior and science acceptance. Personality and Individual Differences, 87, 173175.Google Scholar
Wason, P. C. (1960). On the failure to eliminate hypotheses in a conceptual task, Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 12, 129140.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Watson, G., & Glaser, E. (1985). Critical thinking appraisal. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Wesp, R., & Montgomery, K. (1998). Developing critical thinking through the study of paranormal phenomena. Teaching of Psychology, 25, 275278.Google Scholar
West, R. F., Toplak, M. E., & Stanovich, K. E. (2008). Heuristics and biases as measures of critical thinking: Associations with cognitive ability and thinking dispositions. Journal of Educational Psychology, 100, 930941.Google Scholar
Wood, M. J. (2016). Some dare call it conspiracy: Labeling something a conspiracy theory does not reduce belief in it. Political Psychology, 37, 695705.Google Scholar
Wood, M. J. (2017). Conspiracy suspicions as a proxy for beliefs in conspiracy theories: Implications for theory and measurement. British Journal of Psychology, 108, 507527.Google Scholar
Wood, M. J., Douglas, K. M., & Sutton, R. M. (2011). Dead and alive: Beliefs in contradictory conspiracy theories. Social Psychological & Personality Science, 3, 767773.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zarka, P. (2011). Astronomy and astrology. Proceedings of the International Astronomical Union, 5, 420425.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×