Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-t7czq Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-21T19:48:13.376Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

6 - On Spanish Dvandva and Its Restrictions

from Part I - Lexico-Semantic Aspects of Complex Words

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  18 September 2020

Lívia Körtvélyessy
Affiliation:
P. J. Šafárik University, Košice, Slovakia
Pavol Štekauer
Affiliation:
P. J. Šafárik University, Košice, Slovakia
Get access

Summary

As noted by Bauer, real dvandva compounds – that is, coordinative compounds that properly express the aggregation of two different entities, not the intersection of properties in one entity – are extremely rare in English and Spanish. This article explores the empirical domain of dvandva compounding in Spanish, and notes that they are productive when not used as heads within their phrases. We propose that the explanation for this is that Spanish can only productively build dvandva compounds using flat structures without internal hierarchy. This causes the compound to look externally for a head noun that defines the interpretation of the relation established between the two members of the dvandva. The proposal also explains why proper names are preferred in dvandva compounding, given that they do not denote properties.

Type
Chapter
Information
Complex Words
Advances in Morphology
, pp. 100 - 116
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2020

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Allen, M. (1978). Morphological Investigations, PhD dissertation, Storrs, CT: University of Connecticut.Google Scholar
Baker, M. (2002). Lexical Categories, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Barwise, J. and Cooper, R. (1981). Generalized quantifiers and natural language. Linguistics and Philosophy, 4, 159219.Google Scholar
Bauer, L. (2008). Dvandva. Word Structure, 1, 120.Google Scholar
Bisetto, A. and Scalise, S. (2005). Classification of compounds. Lingue e linguaggio, 2, 319332.Google Scholar
Booij, G. (2010). Construction Morphology, Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Brekle, H. E. (1986). The production and interpretation of ad hoc nominal compounds in German: A realistic approach. Acta Linguistica Academiae Scientiarum Hungaricae, 36, 3952.Google Scholar
Donnellan, K. (1966). Reference and definite descriptions. Philosophical Review, 75, 281304.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Downing, P. (1977). On the creation and use of English compound nouns. Language, 53, 810842.Google Scholar
Evans, G. (1973). A causal theory of names. Proceedings of the Aristotelian Society, 47, 187208.Google Scholar
Fábregas, A. (2005). The Definition of the Grammatical Category in a Syntactically-Oriented Morphology, PhD dissertation, Madrid: Universidad Autónoma de Madrid.Google Scholar
Fernández-Leborans, M. J. (1999). El nombre propio. In Bosque, I. and Demonte, V., dirs., Gramática descriptiva de la lengua española, Madrid: Espasa, pp. 77129.Google Scholar
Frege, G. (1892) [1952]. On sense and reference. In Geach, P. and Black, M., eds., Translations from the Philosophical Writings of Gottlob Frege, Oxford: Blackwell, pp. 5679.Google Scholar
Gleitman, L. R. and Gleitman, H. (1970). Phrase and Paraphrase, New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Hale, K. and Keyser, S. J. (2002). Prolegomenon to a Theory of Argument Structure, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Halle, M. and Marantz, A. (1993). Distributed morphology and the pieces of inflection. In Hale, K. and Keyser, S. J., eds., The View from Building 20, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 111176.Google Scholar
Jackendoff, R. (2010). The ecology of English N-N compounds. In Jackendoff, R., ed., Meaning and the Lexicon, Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 413451.Google Scholar
Koptjevskaja-Tamm, M. (2009). Proper-name nominal compounds in Swedish between syntax and lexicon. Rivista di Linguistica, 21, 119148.Google Scholar
Kripke, S. (1980). Naming and Necessity, Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Lewis, D. (1986). On the Plurality of Worlds, Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Mill, J. S. (1843). A System of Logic, Ratiocinative and Inductive, London: John W. Parker, West Strand.Google Scholar
Pustejovsky, J. (1995). The Generative Lexicon, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Rainer, F. (2013). Can relational adjectives really express any relation? An onomasiological approach. SKASE Journal of Theoretical Linguistics, 10, 1240.Google Scholar
Recanati, F. (1983). La sémantique des noms propres: remarques sur la notion de désignateur rigide. Langue Française, 57, 106118.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Russell, B. (1905). On denoting. Mind, 14, 479493.Google Scholar
Sadock, J. M. (1998). On the autonomy of compounding morphology. In Lapointe, S. G., Brentari, D. K. and Farrell, P. M., eds., Morphology and Its Relation to Phonology and Syntax, Stanford: CSLI, pp. 161187.Google Scholar
Salmon, N. (1981). Reference and Essence, Princeton: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Ten Hacken, P. (2016). The Semantics of Compounding, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Varela, S. (1990). Fundamentos de morfología, Madrid: Síntesis.Google Scholar
Wackernagel, J. (1905). Altindische Grammatik. II, 1: Einleitung zur Wortlehre. Nominalkomposition, Göttingen: Vandenhoeck und Ruprecht.Google Scholar
Wälchli, B. (2005). Co-compounds and natural coordination, Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Wälchli, B. (2015). Co-compounds. In Müller, P. O., Ohnheiser, I., Olsen, S. and Rainer, F., eds., Word-Formation: An International Handbook of the Languages of Europe, Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter, pp. 707727.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Whitney, W. D. (1924). Sanskrit Grammar, Leipzig: Brockhaus.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×