Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-dh8gc Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-06T10:53:06.735Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

7 - Practices of quantification from a socio-cultural perspective

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  22 September 2009

Geoffrey B. Saxe
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of California, Berkeley, USA
Andreas Demetriou
Affiliation:
University of Cyprus
Athanassios Raftopoulos
Affiliation:
University of Cyprus
Get access

Summary

Children are engaged with mathematics in their everyday activities. Look around you – notice the children chanting numbers as they jump rope or haggling over their scores in handball. Children in urban centres in Brazil buy and sell goods (Carraher, Carraher and Schliemann 1985; Saxe 1991); toddlers in working and middle-class homes in the US play number games and sing number songs with their mothers (Saxe, Guberman and Gearhart 1987); inner-city teenage boys keep track of their statistics in league basketball play (Nasir 2002). Mathematics is interwoven in children's everyday collective activities, and yet the cognitive-developmental study of children's mathematics has often overlooked such activities as sites for analysis. The result is that treatments of development often do not capture adequately the role of children's participation in collective activities, nor the way that children themselves contribute to the mathematical norms, values and conventions that take form in collective life.

The purpose of the chapter is to present a conceptual framework for analysing the interplay between individual and collective activity in cognitive development, using mathematical cognition as an illustrative case. The framework is rooted in an assumption common to psychogenetic treatments, whether structural-developmental (Piaget 1970) or activity-theoretic (e.g. Leontiev 1981): new cognitive developments emerge as individuals create and accomplish goals in daily activities. In this chapter I present an exposition of the framework with particular attention to the ‘what?’, ‘how?’ and ‘why?’ of development.

Throughout the chapter, I limit my discussion of cognitive development to practices of quantification.

Type
Chapter
Information
Cognitive Developmental Change
Theories, Models and Measurement
, pp. 241 - 263
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2005

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Butterworth, B. (1999). What counts: how every brain is hardwired for math. New York: The Free Press
Carraher, T. N., Carraher, D. W. and Schliemann, A. D. (1985). Mathematics in the streets and in schools. British Journal of Developmental Psychology 3, 21–9CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Case, R. (1985). Intellectual development: birth to adulthood. New York: Academic Press
Case, R. (1992). The mind's staircase: exploring the conceptual underpinnings of children's thought and knowledge. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum
Case, R., Okamoto, Y., Henderson, B. and McKeough, A. (1993). Analyzing diversity in developmental pathways: Methods and concepts. In R. Case and W. Edelstein (eds.) The new structuralism in cognitive development: theory and research on individual pathways (pp. 71–100). Contributions to Human Development, vol. 23. Basel: Karger
Case, R., Okamoto, Y., Griffin, S., McKeough, A., Bleiker, C., Henderson, B. and Stephenson, K. M. (1996). The role of central conceptual structures in the development of children's thought. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 61 (1–2), serial no. 246CrossRef
Cole, M. (1996). Cultural psychology: a once and future discipline. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press
Dasen, P. R. and de Ribaupierre, A. (1988). Neo-Piagetian theories: cross-cultural and differential perspectives. In A. Demetriou (ed.) The neo-Piagetian theories of cognitive development: toward an integration. North Holland: Elsevier
Dehaene, S. (1997). The number sense: how the mind creates mathematics. New York: Oxford
Demetriou, A., Christou, C., Spanoudis, G. and Platsidou, M. (2002). The development of mental processing: efficiency, working memory, and thinking. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 67(1), serial no. 263
Demetriou, A., Kui, Z. X., Spanoudis, G., Christou, C., Kyriakides, L. and Platsidou, M. (2003). The architecture and development of mental processing: Greek, Chinese, or universal? Unpublished manuscript. University of Cyprus
Gearhart, M., Saxe, G. B., Seltzer, M., Schlackman, J., Ching, C., Nasir, N., Fall, R., Bennett, T., Rhine, S. and Sloan, T. F. (1999). Opportunities to learn fractions in elementary mathematics classrooms. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education 30 (3): 286–315CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1995). Beyond modularity: a developmental perspective on cognitive science. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
Leontiev, A. N. (1981). The problem of activity in psychology. In J. V. Wertsch (ed.) The concept of activity in Soviet psychology (pp. 37–71). New York: Sharpe
Modell, J. (1996). The uneasy engagement of ethnography and human development. In R. Jessor, A. Colby and R. A. Shweder (eds.) Ethnography and human development: context and meaning in social inquiry (pp. 479–504). Chicago: University of Chicago Press
Nasir, N. S. (2002). Identity, goals, and learning: mathematics in cultural practice. Mathematical Thinking and Learning, 4(2 and 3), 213–47CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pascual-Leone, J. (1970). A mathematical model for the transition rule in Piaget's developmental stages. Acta Psychologica, 32, 301–45CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pascual-Leone, J. (1988). Organismic processes for neo-Piagetian theories: a dialectical causal account of cognitive development. In A. Demetriou (ed.) The neo-Piagetian theories of cognitive development: towards an integration (pp. 25–64). Amsterdam: North-Holland
Piaget, J. (1965). The child's conception of number. New York, Norton
Piaget, J. (1970). Piaget's theory. In P. H. Mussen (ed.) Carmichael's manual of child psychology (pp. 703–31). New York: Wiley
Piaget, J. (1972). Intellectual evolution from adolescence to adulthood. Human Development, 15(1), 1–12CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Saxe, G. B. (1991). Culture and cognitive development: studies in mathematical understanding. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates
Saxe, G. B. (1999). Cognition, development, and cultural practices. In E. Turiel (ed.) Culture and development: new directions in child psychology (pp. 19–35). San Francisco: Jossey-BassCrossRef
Saxe, G. B., Guberman, S. R. and Gearhart, M. (1987). Social processes in early number development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 52(2), serial no. 216CrossRef
Saxe, G. B. and Esmonde, I. (in press). Making change in Oksapmin tradestores: studies in shifting practices of quantification under conditions of rapid social change. South Pacific Journal of Psychology
Saxe, G. B., Gearhart, M. and Seltzer, M. (1999). Relations between classroom practices and student learning in the domain of fractions. Cognition and Instruction, 17 (1), 1–24CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Saxe, G. B. and Guberman, S. R. (1998). Studying mathematics learning in collective activity. Learning and Instruction, 8(6), 489–501CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Siegler, R. S. and Crowley, K. (1991). The microgenetic method: a direct means for studying cognitive development. American Psychologist, 46, 606–20CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press
Vygotsky, L. S. (1986). Thought and language. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
Vygotsky, L. S. (1987). Thinking and speech. New York: Plenum
Werner, H. and Kaplan, B. (1963). Symbol formation: an organismic-developmental approach to language and the expression of thought. New York: Wiley
Werner, H. and Kaplan, B. (1984). Symbol formation: an organismic-developmental approach to the psychology of language. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates
Wertsch, J. V. (1985). Vygotsky and the social formation of mind. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press
Wertsch, J. V. (1991). Voices of the mind. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×