Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-s2hrs Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-06T10:55:18.817Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

10 - Dealing with change: manifestations, measurements and methods

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  22 September 2009

Elena L. Grigorenko
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, PACE Center, Yale University, USA
Paul A. O'Keefe
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, PACE Center, Yale University, USA
Andreas Demetriou
Affiliation:
University of Cyprus
Athanassios Raftopoulos
Affiliation:
University of Cyprus
Get access

Summary

The main purpose of this chapter is to discuss briefly major methodologies used for the analysis of change. Throughout the development and maturation of approaches to measuring change, different types of change have been recognized and different methods of quantifying change have been developed (e.g., Collins and Horn 1991; Harris 1963). There is a colloquial reference to so-called old and new approaches to measuring change. ‘Old’ approaches refer to such conventional indicators of change as the differences between measures in a given time point and a subsequent time point. ‘New’ approaches refer to ever more complex methodologies for describing and quantifying development, whether spontaneous or occurring in response to intervention.

Clearly, the amount of information on a subject studied by many outstanding scientists that can be introduced in a single chapter is limited. Moreover, a single chapter cannot compete with the comprehensive volumes that have recently been written on the same topic (e.g. Collins and Sayer 2001; Gottman 1995; Moskowitz and Hershberger 2002; von Eye and Niedermeier 1999). Therefore, the strategy selected in this chapter for material presentation is to introduce briefly selected methodological approaches, illustrate them with specific examples, and provide the reader with a wealth of relevant references.

We decided to illustrate various change-related methodologies by reviewing a limited content area – the field of studies related to acquisition, both natural and in response to targeted intervention, of oral and written language. Such a decision is not random, of course. The selection was driven by two considerations.

Type
Chapter
Information
Cognitive Developmental Change
Theories, Models and Measurement
, pp. 318 - 353
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2005

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Abbott, S. P. and Berninger, V. W. (1999). It's never too late to remediate: teaching word recognition to students with reading disabilities in grades 4–7. Annals of Dyslexia, 49, 223–50CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Abbott, S. P., Reed, E., Abbott, R. D. and Berninger, V. W. (1997). Year-long balanced reading/writing tutorial: a design experiment used for dynamic assessment. Learning Disability Quarterly, 20(3), 249–63CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Alligood, K. T., Sauer, T. D. and Yorke, J. A. (1997). Chaos. An introduction to dynamic systems. New York: Springer
Anderson, R. (1999). Impact of first language loss on grammar in a bilingual child. Communication Disorders Quarterly, 21(1), 4–16CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Aro, M., Aro, T., Ahonen, T., Raesaenen, T., Hietala, A. and Lyytinen, H. (1999). The development of phonological abilities and their relation to reading acquisition: case studies of six Finnish children. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 32(5), 457–63, 478CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Arthur, W., Jr, Bennett, W., Jr and Huffcutt, A. I. (2001). Conducting meta-analysis using SAS. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum
Baltes, P. B., Reese, H. W. and Nesselrode, J. R. (1997). Life-span developmental psychology: introduction to research methods. Monterey, CA: Brooks/Cole
Benson, N. J., Lovett, M. W. and Kroeber, C. L. (1997). Training and transfer-of-learning effects in disabled and normal readers: evidence of specific deficits. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 64(3), 343–66CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bereiter, C. (1963). Some persisting dilemmas in the measurement of change. In C. W. Harris (ed.) Problems in measuring change (pp. 3–20). Madison: University of Wisconsin Press
Bergman, L. R., Magnusson, D. and El-Khouri, B. M. (2003). Studying individual development in an interindividual context. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum
Berninger, V. W. (2000). Dyslexia the invisible, treatable disorder: the story of Enstein's ninja turtles. Learning Disability Quarterly, 23(3), 175–95CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berninger, V. W., Abbott, R. D., Brooksher, R., Lemos, Z., Ogier, S., Zook, D.. (2000). A connectionist approach to making the predictability of English orthography explicit to at-risk beginning readers: evidence for alternative, effective strategies. Developmental Neuropsychology, 17(2), 241–71CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berninger, V. W., Abbott, R. D., Zook, D., Ogier, S., Lemos-Britton, Z. and Brooksher, R. (1999). Early intervention for reading disabilities: teaching the alphabet principle in a connectionist framework. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 32(6), 491–503CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blachman, B. A., Tangel, D. M., Ball, E. W., Black, R. and McGraw, C. K. (1999). Developing phonological awareness and word recognition skills: a two-year intervention with low-income, inner-city children. Reading and Writing, 11(3), 239–73CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bock, R. D. and Thissen, D. (1980). Statistical problems of fitting individual growth curves. In F. E. Johnston, A. F. Roche, C. Susanne (eds.) Human physical growth and maturation: methodologies and factors (pp. 265–90). New York, NY: PlenumCrossRef
Boker, S. M. (2001). Differential structural equation modelling of intraindividual variability. In L. M. Collins and A. G. Sayer (eds.) New methods for the analysis of change (pp. 3–27). Washington, DC: APACrossRef
Bouldoukian, J., Wilkins, A. J. and Evans, B. J. (2002). Randomised controlled trial of the effect of coloured overlays on the rate of reading of people with specific learning difficulties. Ophthalmic and Physiological Optics, 22(1), 55–60CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boulet, D., Bryce, S., Bliss, L. S. and Linebaugh, C. (1998). Listener judgments of impaired narration at two age levels. Journal of Children's Communication Development, 20(1), 1–8CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brillinger, D. R. (2001). Time series: general. In N. J. Smelser and P. B. Baltes (eds.) Social and behavioral sciences (vol. 23, pp. 15724–31). Amsterdam, Holland: ElsevierCrossRef
Brock, W. A. (2001). Chaos theory. In N. J. Smelser & P. B. Baltes (eds.) International encyclopedia of the social and behavioral sciences (pp. 1643–6). Amsterdam, Holland: ElsevierCrossRef
Brooks, P. (1995). A comparison of the effectiveness of different teaching strategies in teaching spelling to a student with severe specific difficulties/dyslexia. Educational and Child Psychology, 12(1), 80–8Google Scholar
Browne, M. W. and Du Toit, S. H. C. (1991). Models for learning data. In L. M. Collins and J. L. Horn (eds.) Best methods for the analysis of change (pp. 47–68). Washington: American Psychological AssociationCrossRef
Brunsdon, R. K., Hannan, T. J., Coltheart, M. and Nickels, L. (2002). Treatment of lexical processing in mixed dyslexia: a case study. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 12(5), 385–418CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bryk, A. and Raudenbush, S. (1992). Hierarchical linear models in social and behavioral research: applications and data analysis methods. Newbury Park, CA: Sage
Burchinal, M. R., Peisner-Feinberg, E., Pianta, R. and Howes, C. (2002). Development of academic skills from preschool through second grade: family and classroom predictors of developmental trajectories. Journal of School Psychology, 40(5), 415–36CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Butler, D. L., Elaschuk, C. L. and Poole, S. (2000). Promoting strategic writing by postsecondary students with learning disabilities: a report of three case studies. Learning Disability Quarterly, 23(3), 196–213CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Camarata, S. M., Nelson, K. E. and Camarata, M. N. (1994). Comparison of conversational-recasting and imitative procedures for training grammatical structures in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 37(6), 1414–23CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Campbell, D. T. and Kenny, D. A. (1999). A primer on regression artifacts. New York: Guilford Press
Campbell, F. A., Pungello, E. P., Miller-Johnson, S., Burchinal, M. and Ramey, C. T. (2001). The development of cognitive and academic abilities: growth curves from an early childhood educational experiment. Developmental Psychology, 37(2), 231–42CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chambers, B., Abrami, P. C., Massue, F. M. and Morrison, S. (1998). Success for all: evaluating an early-intervention program for children at risk of school failure. Canadian Journal of Education, 23(4), 357–72CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chera, P. and Wood, C. (2003). Animated multimedia ‘talking books’ can promote phonological awareness in children beginning to read. Learning and Instruction, 13(1), 33–52CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Churches, M., Skuy, M. and Das, J. P. (2002). Identification and remediation of reading difficulties based on successive processing deficits and delay in general reading. Psychological Reports, 91(3, pt. 1), 813–24CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cipriani, P., Bottari, P., Chilosi, A. M. and Pfanner, L. (1998). A longitudinal perspective on the study of specific language impairment: the long-term follow-up of an Italian child. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 33(3), 245–80Google Scholar
Cohen, J. and Cohen, P. (1975). Applied multiple regression/correlation analysis for the behavioral sciences. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
Cohen, J., Cohen, P., West, S. G. and Aiken, L. S. (2003). Applied multiple regression/correlation analysis for the behavioral sciences. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
Collins, L. M. and Horn, J. L. (eds.) (1991). Best methods for the analysis of change. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association
Collins, L. M. and Horn, J. L. (2001). Best methods for the analysis of change. Washington, DC: APA
Collins, L. M. and Sayer, A. G. (eds.) (2001). New methods for analysis of change. Washington, DC: APA
Compton, D. L. (2000). Modeling the response of normally achieving and at-risk first grade children to word reading instruction. Annals of Dyslexia, 50, 53–84CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cronbach, L. and Furby, L. (1970). How should we measure change – or should we? Psychological Bulletin, 74, 68–70CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Daly, E. J. and Martens, B. K. (1994). A comparison of three interventions for increasing oral reading performance: application of the instructional hierarchy. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 27(3), 459–69CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Daly, E. J., Murdoch, A., Lillenstein, L., Webber, L. and Lentz, F. E. (2002). An examination of methods for testing treatments: conducting brief experimental analyses of the effects of instructional components on oral reading fluency. Education and Treatment of Children, 25(3), 288–316Google Scholar
Das, J. P., Mishra, R. K. and Pool, J. E. (1995). An experiment on cognitive remediation of word-reading difficulty. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 28(2), 66–79CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dryer, R., Beale, I. L. and Lambert, A. J. (1993). The balance model of dyslexia and remedial training: an evaluative study. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 32(2), 174–86CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elkind, J., Cohen, K. and Murray, C. (1993). Using computer-based readers to improve reading comprehension of students with dyslexia. Annals of Dyslexia, 43, 238–59CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Embretson, S. E. (1991). A multidimensional latent trait model for measuring learning and change. Psychometrika, 56, 495–516CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Embretson, S. E. (1994). Comparing changes between groups: Some perplexities arising from psychometrics. In D. Laveault, B. D. Zumbo, M. E. Gessaroli and M. W. Boss (eds.) Modern theories of measurement: problems and issues. Ottawa: Edumetric Research Group, University of Ottawa
Embretson, S. E. (1996). Item response theory models and inferential bias in multiple group comparisons. Applied Psychological Measurement, 20, 201–12CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Embretson, S. E. and Reise, S. R. (2000). Item response theory for psychologists. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum Lawrence
Eyer, J. A. and Leonard, L. B. (1995). Functional categories and specific language impairment: a case study. Language Acquisition: A Journal of Developmental Linguistics, 4(3), 177–203CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Facoetti, A., Lorusso, M. L., Paganoni, P., Umilta, C. and Mascetti, G. G. (2003). The role of visuospatial attention in developmental dyslexia: evidence from a rehabilitation study. Cognitive Brain Research, 15(2), 154–64CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fazio, B. B., Naremore, R. C. and Connell, P. J. (1996). Tracking children from poverty at risk for specific language impairment: a three-year longitudinal study. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 39(3), 611–24CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fischer, K. W. and Rose, S. P. (1994). Development of coordination of components in brain and behavior: a framework for theory and research. In G. Dawson and K. W. Fisher (eds.) Human behavior and the developing brain (pp. 3–66). New York: Guilford
Flowers, L., Meyer, M., Lovato, J., Wood, F. and Felton, R. (2000). Does third grade discrepancy status predict the course of reading development? Annals of Dyslexia, 51, 49–74CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Foorman, B. R., Francis, D. J., Winikates, D., Mehta, P., Schatschneider, C. and Fletcher, J. M. (1997). Early interventions for children with reading disabilities. Scientific Studies of Reading, 1(3), 255–76CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Francis, D. J., Shaywitz, S. E., Stuebing, K. K., Shaywitz, B. A. and Fletcher, J. M. (1996). Developmental lag versus deficit models of reading disability: a longitudinal, individual growth curves analysis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 88(1), 3–17CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gallagher, A., Frith, U. and Snowling, M. J. (2000). Precursors of literacy delay among children at genetic risk of dyslexia. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 41(2), 202–13CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gillon, Gail T. (2000). The efficacy of phonological awareness intervention for children with spoken language impairment. Language, Speech and Hearing Services in the Schools, 31(2), 126–41CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gillon, Gail T. (2002). Follow-up study investigating the benefits of phonological awareness intervention for children with spoken language impairment. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 37(4), 381–400CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gilmore, R. (1981). Catastrophe theory for scientists and engineers. New York: Wiley
Glass, L. and Mackey, M. (1988). From clocks to chaos. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press
Gogate, L. J. and Walker-Andrews, A. (2001). The intersensory origins of word comprehension: an ecological-dynamic systems view. Developmental Science, 4, 1–37CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldstein, B. H. and Obrzut, J. E. (2001). Neuropsychological treatment of dyslexia in the classroom setting. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 34(3), 276–85CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gottman, J. M. (ed.) (1995). The analysis of change. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum
Gottman, J. M. and Roy, A. K. (1990). Sequential analysis: a guide for behavioral researchers. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press
Graham, L. and Wong, B. Y. (1993). Comparing two modes of teaching a question-answering strategy for enhancing reading comprehension: didactic and self-instructional training. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 26(4), 270–9CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greaney, K. T., Tunmer, W. E. and Chapman, J. W. (1997). Effects of rime-based orthographic analogy training on the word recognition skills of children with reading disability. Journal of Educational Psychology, 89(4), 645–51CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Greenway, C. (2002). The process, pitfalls and benefits of implementing a reciprocal teaching intervention to improve the reading comprehension of a group of year 6 pupils. Educational Psychology in Practice, 18(2), 113–37CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Guyer, B. P., Banks, S. R. and Guyer, K. E. (1993). Spelling improvement for college students who are dyslexic. Annals of Dyslexia, 43, 186–93CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Habib, M., Rey, V., Daffaure, V., Camps, R., Espesser, R., Joly-Pottuz, B.. (2002). Phonological training in children with dyslexia using temporally modified speech: a three-step pilot investigation. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 37(3), 289–308CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hadley, P. A. (1998). Early verb-related vulnerability among children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research, 41(6), 1384–97CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hadley, P. A. and Rice, M. L. (1996). Emergent uses of BE and DO: evidence from children with specific language impairment. Language Acquisition: A Journal of Developmental Linguistics, 5(3), 209–43CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hambleton, R. K. and Slater, S. C. (1997). Item response theory models and testing practices: current international status and future directions. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 13, 21–8CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hambleton, R. K., Swaminathan, H. and Rogers, H. J. (1991). Fundamentals of item response theory. Newbury Park, CA: Sage
Harris, C. W. (ed.) (1963). Problems in measuring change. Madison; University of Wisconsin Press
Hart, T. M., Berninger, V. M. and Abbott, R. D. (1997). Comparison of teaching single or multiple orthographic-phonological connections for word recognition and spelling: implications for instructional consultation. School Psychology Review, 26(2), 279–97Google Scholar
Hartelman, P. A. I., Maas, H. L. J. and Molenaar, P. C. M. (1998). Detecting and modelling developmental transitions. The Journal of Developmental Psychology, 16, 97–122CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hatcher, P. J. (2000). Sound links in reading and spelling with discrepancy-defined dyslexics and children with moderate learning difficulties. Reading and Writing, 13(3–4), 257–72CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hatcher, P. J., Hulme, C. and Ellis, A. W. (1994). Ameliorating early reading failure by integrating the teaching of reading and phonological skills: the phonological linkage hypothesis. Child Development, 65(1), 41–57CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hecht, S. A., and Close, L. (2002). Emergent literary skills and training time uniquely predict variability in responses to phonemic awareness training in disadvantaged kindergarteners. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 82(2), 93–115CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hick, R. F., Joseph, K. L., Conti-Ramsden, G., Serratrice, L. and Faragher, B. (2002). Vocabulary profiles of children with specific language impairment. Child Language Teaching and Therapy, 18(2), 165–80CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hillis, A. E. (1993). The role of models of language processing in rehabilitation of language impairments. Aphasiology, 7(1), 5–26CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ho, C. S., Lam, E. Y. and Au, A. (2001). The effectiveness of multisensory training in improving reading and writing skills of Chinese dyslexic children. Psychologia, 44(4), 269–80CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hosenfeld, B., Maas, H. L. J. and Boom, D. C. (1997a). Detecting bimodality in the analogical reasoning performance of elementary schoolchildren. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 20, 529–47CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hosenfeld, B. (1997b). Indicators of discontinuous change in the development of analogical reasoning. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 64, 367–95CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hurford, D. P., Darrow, L. J., Edwards, T., Howerton, C. J., Mote, C. R., Schauf, J. D.. (1993). An examination of phonemic processing abilities in children during their first-grade year. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 26(3), 167–77CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hurford, D. P., Potter, T. S. and Hart, G. S. (2002). Examination of three techniques for identifying first-grade children at risk for difficulty in word identification with an emphasis on reducing the false negative error rate. Reading Psychology, 23(3) 2, 159–80CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hurford, D. P., Schauf, J. D., Blaich, T., Bunce, L. and Moore, K. (1994). Early identification of children at risk for reading disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 27, 371–82CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huttenlocher, J., Haight, W., Bryk, A. and Seltzer, M. (1991). Early vocabulary growth: relation to language input and gender. Developmental Psychology, 27, 236–49CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jacobson, C. (1999). How persistent is reading disability? Individual growth curves in reading. Dyslexia: The Journal of the British Dyslexia Association. 5(2), 78–933.0.CO;2-8>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnston, J. R., Miller, J. and Tallal, P. (2001). Use of cognitive state predicates by language-impaired children. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 36(3), 349–70CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Joseph, K. L., Serratrice, L. and Conti-Ramsden, G. (2002). Development of copula and auxiliary BE in children with specific language impairment and younger unaffected controls. First Language, 22(65, pt. 2), 137–72CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kappers, E. J. (1997). Outpatient treatment of dyslexia through stimulation of the cerebral hemispheres. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 30(1), 100–24CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kemper, S., Rice, K. and Chen, Y. (1995). Complexity metrics and growth curves for measuring grammatical development from five to ten. First Language, 15(44, pt. 2), 151–66CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Korhonen, T. T. (1995). The persistence of rapid naming problems in children with reading disabilities: a nine-year follow-up. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 28(4), 232–9CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Laakso, M., Poikkeus, A. and Lyytinen, P. (1999). Shared reading interaction in families with and without genetic risk for dyslexia: implications for toddlers' language development. Infant and Child Development, 8(4), 179–953.0.CO;2-G>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Landry, S. H., Smith, K. E., Miller-Loncar, C. L. and Swank, P. R. (1997). Predicting cognitive-language and social growth curves from early maternal behaviors in children at varying degrees of biological risk. Developmental Psychology, 33(6), 1040–53CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lewis, B. A., Freebairn, L. A. and Taylor, H. G. (2000). Academic outcomes in children with histories of speech sound disorders. Journal of Communication Disorders, 33(1), 11–30CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lipsey, M. W. and Wilson, D. B. (2001). Practical meta-analysis. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage
Lord, F. M. (1952). A theory of mental test scores. Psychometric Monograph No 7
Lord, F. M. (1980). Applications of item response theory to practical testing problems. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
Louis, M., Espesser, R., Rey, V., Daffaure, V., Di Cristo, A. and Habib, M. (2001). Intensive training of phonological skills in progressive aphasia: a model of brain plasticity in neurodegenerative disease. Brain and Cognition, 46(1–2), 197–201CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lovett, M. W. and Steinbach, K. A. (1997). The effectiveness of remedial programs for reading disabled children of different ages: does the benefit decrease for older children? Learning Disability Quarterly, 20(3), 189–210CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lovett, M. W., Lacerenza, L., Borden, S. L., Frijters, J. C., Steinbach, K. A. and Palma, M. (2000). Components of effective remediation for developmental reading disabilities: combining phonological and strategy-based instruction to improve outcomes. Journal of Educational Psychology, 92(2), 263–83CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lundberg, I. (1995). The computer as a tool of remediation in the education of students with reading disabilities: a theory-based approach. Learning Disability Quarterly, 18(2), 89–99CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lyytinen, P., Eklund, K. and Lyytinen, H. (2003). The play and language behavior of mothers with and without dyslexia and its association to their toddlers' language development. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 36(1), 74–86CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lyytinen, P., Poikkeus, A., Laakso, M., Eklund, K. and Lyytinen, H. (2001). Language development and symbolic play in children with and without familial risk for dyslexia. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research, 44(4), 873–85CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Manis, F. R., Custodio, R. and Szeszulski, P. A. (1993). Development of phonological and orthographic skill: a two-year longitudinal study of dyslexic children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 56(1), 64–86CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Manis, F. R., Seidenberg, M. S., Stallings, L., Joanisse, M., Bailey, C., Freedman, L.. (1999). Development of dyslexic subgroups: a one-year follow up. Annals of Dyslexia, 49, 105–34CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McArdle, J. (2001). Growth curve analysis. In N. J. Smelser and P. B. Baltes (eds.) Social and behavioral sciences (vol. 9, pp. 6439–45). Amsterdam, Holland: ElsevierCrossRef
McArdle, J. and Woodcock, J. (1997). Expanding test-retest designs to include developmental time-lag components. Psychological Methods, 2, 403–35CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCarthy, P., Newby, R. F. and Recht, D. R. (1995). Results of an early intervention program for first grade children at risk for reading disability. Reading Research and Instruction, 34(4), 273–94CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McGee, R., Prior, M., Williams, S., Smart, D. and Sanson, A. (2002). The long-term significance of teacher-rated hyperactivity and reading ability in childhood: findings from two longitudinal studies. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 43(8), 1004–16CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meredith, W. and Tisak, J. (1990). Latent curve analysis. Psychometrika, 55, 107–22CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meyer, M. S., Wood, F. B., Hart, L. A. and Felton, R. H. (1998). Longitudinal course of rapid naming in disabled and nondisabled readers. Annals of Dyslexia, 48, 91–114CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, L. L. and Felton, R. H. (2001). ‘It's one of them … I don't know’: case study of a student with phonological, rapid naming, and word-finding deficits. Journal of Special Education, 35(3), 125–33CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mirak, J., and Rescorla, L. (1998). Phonetic skills and vocabulary size in late talkers: concurrent and predictive relationships. Applied Psycholinguistics, 19(1), 1–17CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morris, D., Tyner, B., and Perney, J. (2000). Early steps: replicating the effects of a first-grade reading intervention program. Journal of Educational Psychology, 92(4), 681–93CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moskowitz, D. S. and Hershberger, S. L. (eds.) (2002). Modeling intraindividaul variability with repeated measures data: methods and applications. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum
Muthén, B., Khoo, S.-T., Francis, D. J. and Boscardin, C. K. (2003). Analysis of reading skills development from kindergarten through first grade: an application of growth mixture modeling to sequential process. In S. P. Reise and N. Duan (eds.) Multilevel modeling. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum
Nelson, K. E., Camarata, S. M., Welsh, J., Butkovsky, L. and Camarata, M. (1996). Effects of imitative and conversational recasting treatment on the acquisition of grammar in children with Specific Language Impairment and younger language-normal children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 39, 850–9CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Newell, K. M., and Molenaar, P. C. M. (1998). Applications of nonlinear dynamics to developmental process modeling. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
Oakland, T., Black, J. L., Stanford, G., Nussbaum, N. L. and Balise, R. R. (1998). An evaluation of the dyslexia training program: a multisensory method for promoting reading in students with reading disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 31(2), 140–7CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Olofsson, A. and Niedersoe, J. (1999). Early language development and kindergarten phonological awareness as predictors of reading problems: from 3 to 11 years of age. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 32(5), 464–72CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Osgood, D. W. (2001). Application of multilevel models to the analysis of change. In L. M. Collins and A. G. Sayer (eds.) New methods for the analysis of change (pp. 97–104). Washington, DC: APA
O'Shaugh nessy, T. E. and Swanson, H. L. (2000). A comparison of two reading interventions for children with reading disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 33(3), 257–77CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ottem, E. (2001). Use of pictographic-articulatory symbols to promote alphabetic reading in a language-impaired boy: case study. AAC: Augmentative and Alternative Communication, 17(1), 52–60Google Scholar
Oyama, S. (2000). The ontogeny of information: developmental systems and evolution. Durham, NC: Duke University Press
Pascual-Leone, J. (1995). Learning and development as dialectical factors in cognitive growth. Human Development, 38, 338–48CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pascual- Leone, J. and Baillargeon, R. (1994). Developmental measurement of mental attention. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 17, 161–200CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peach, R. K. (2002). Treatment for phonological dyslexia targeting regularity effects. Aphasiology, 16(8), 779–89CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pennington, B. F. and Lefly, D. L. (2001). Early reading development in children at family risk for dyslexia. Child Development, 72(3), 816–33CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pharr, A. B., Ratner, N. B. and Rescorla, L. (2000). Syllable structure development of toddlers with expressive specific language impairment. Applied Psycholinguistics, 21(4), 429–49CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Phelps, E., Furstenberg, F. F. and Colby, A. (eds.) (2002). Looking at lives: American longitudinal studies of the twentieth century. New York: Russell Sage Foundation
Piccinin, A. M. (2001). Differential structural equation modeling of intraindividual variability. In L. M. Collins and A. G. Sayer (eds.) New methods for the analysis of change (pp. 29–32). Washington, DC: APA
Pinherio, J. C. and Bates, D. M. (2000). Mixed-effect models in S and S-PLUS. New York, NY: Springer
Pogorzelski, S., and Wheldall, K. (2002). Do differences in phonological processing performance predict gains made by older low-progress readers following intensive literacy intervention? Educational Psychology, 22(4), 413–27CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Poskiparta, E., Niemi, P. and Vauras, M. (1999). Who benefits from training in linguistic awareness in the first grade, and what components show training effects? Journal of Learning Disabilities, 32(5), 437–46, 456CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Post, Y. V., Carreker, S. and Holland, G. (2001). The spelling of final letter patterns: a comparison of instruction at the phoneme and the rime. Annals of Dyslexia, 51, 121–46CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Preece, M. A. and Baines, M. K. (1978). A new family of mathematical models describing the human growth curve. Ann. Hum. Biol., 7, 507–28Google Scholar
Rasch, G. (1980). Probabilistic models for some intelligence and attainment tests. Chicago: University of Chicago Press (original work published in 1960: Rasch, G. Probabilistic models for some intelligence and attainment tests. Copenhagen: Danmarks Paedagogiske Institut)
Raudenbush, S. W. (2001). Toward a coherent framework for comparing trajectories of individual change. In L. M. Collins and A. G. Sayer (eds.) New methods for the analysis of change (pp. 33–64). Washington, DC: APACrossRef
Reise, S. P. and Duan, N. (2003). Multilevel modeling. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
Rescorla, L. (2000). Do late-talking toddlers turn out to have reading difficulties a decade later? Annals of Dyslexia, 50, 87–102CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rescorla, L. (2002). Language and reading outcomes to age nine in late-talking toddlers. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research. 45(2), 360–71CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rescorla, L. and Roberts, J. (2002). Nominal versus verbal morpheme use in late talkers at ages three and four. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research, 45(6), 1219–31CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rescorla, L., Mirak, J. and Singh, L. (2000). Vocabulary growth in late talkers: lexical development from 2;0 to 3;0. Journal of Child Language, 27(2), 293–311CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rice, M. L., Wexler, K. and Hershberger, S. (1998). Tense over time: the longitudinal course of tense acquisition in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research, 41(6), 1412–31CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rice, M. L., Wexler, K., Marquis, J. and Hershberger, S. (2000). Acquisition of irregular past tense by children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research, 43(5), 1126–45CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Richards, T. L., Berninger, V. W., Aylward, E. H., Richards, A. L., Thomson, J. B., Nagy, W. E.. (2002). Reproducibility of proton MR spectroscopic imaging (PEPSI): comparison of dyslexic and normal-reading children and effects of treatment on brain lactate levels during language tasks. American Journal of Neuroradiology, 23(10), 1678–85Google ScholarPubMed
Richards, T. L., Corina, D., Serafini, S., Steury, K., Echelard, D. R., Dager, S. R.. (2000). Effects of a phonologically driven treatment for dyslexia on lactate levels measured by proton MR spectroscopic imaging. American Journal of Neuroradiology, 21(5), 916–22Google ScholarPubMed
Robinson, B. F. and Mervis, C. B. (1998). Disentangling early language development: modeling lexical and grammatical acquisition using an extension of case-study methodology. Developmental Psychology, 34, 363–75CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Robinson, B. F. (1999). Comparing productive vocabulary measures from the CDI and a systematic diary study. Journal of Child Language, 26(1), 177–85CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rogosa, D. R. (1995). Myths and methods: ‘myth about longitudinal research’ plus supplemental questions. In J. M. Gottman (ed.) The analysis of change (pp. 3–66). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum
Rogosa, D., Brandt, D. and Zimowskyk, M. (1982). A growth curve approach to the measurement of change. Psychological Bulletin, 92, 726–48CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rovine, M. J. and Molenaar, P. C. M. (2001). A structural equations modeling approach to the general linear mixed model. In L. M. Collins and A. G. Sayer (eds.) New methods for the analysis of change (pp. 65–96). Washington, DC: APACrossRef
Ruhland, R. and Geert, P. (1998). Jumping into syntax: transitions in the development of closed class words. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 16, 65–95CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sayer, A. G. and Cumsille, P. R. (2001). Second-order latent growth models. In L. M. Collins and A. G. Sayer (eds.) New methods for the analysis of change (pp. 177–200). Washington, DC: APACrossRef
Scammon, R. E. (1927). The first seriatim study of human growth. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 10, 329–36CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Seber, G. A. F. and Wild, C. J. (1989). Nonlinear models. New York, NY; Wiley
Schmitz, B. (2001). Change: methods of studying. In N. J. Smelser and P. B. Baltes (eds.) International encyclopedia of the social and behavioral sciences, vol. 3 (pp. 1640–3). Amsterdam, Holland: ElsevierCrossRef
Schneider, W., Ennemoser, M., Roth, E. and Kuespert, P. (1999). Kindergarten prevention of dyslexia: does training in phonological awareness work for everybody? Journal of Learning Disabilities, 32(5), 429–36CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schneider, W., Kuespert, P., Roth, E., and Vise, M. (1997). Short- and long-term effects of training phonological awareness in kindergarten: evidence from two German studies. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 66(3), 311–40CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schneider, W., Roth, E. and Ennemoser, M. (2000). Training phonological skills and letter knowledge in children at risk for dyslexia: a comparison of three kindergarten intervention programs. Journal of Educational Psychology, 92(2), 284–95CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shaywitz, S., Fletcher, E., Holahan, J. M., Shneider, A. E., Marchione, K. E., Stuebing, K. K.. (1999). Persistence of dyslexia: the Connecticut Longitudinal Study at adolescence. Pediatrics, 104, 1351–9CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Silliman, E. R., Jimerson, T. L. and Wilkinson, L. C. (2000). A dynamic systems approach to writing assessment in students with language learning problems. Topics in Language Disorders, 20(4), 45–64CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Simos, P. G., Fletcher, J. M., Bergman, E., Breier, J. I., Foorman, B. R., Castillo, E. M.. (2002). Dyslexia-specific brain activation profile becomes normal following successful remedial training. Neurology, 58(8), 1203–13CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Snowling, M. J., Bishop, D. V. M. and Stothard, S. E. (2000). Is preschool language impairment a risk factor for dyslexia in adolescence? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 41(5), 587–600CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Snowling, M. J., Goulandris, N. and Defty, N. (1996). A longitudinal study of reading development in dyslexic children. Journal of Educational Psychology, 88(4), 653–69CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sprenger-Charolles, L., Cole, P., Lacert, P. and Serniclaes, W. (2000). On subtypes of developmental dyslexia: evidence from processing time and accuracy scores. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 54(2), 87–104CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stage, S. A. (2001). Program evaluation using hierarchical linear modeling with curriculum-based measurement reading probes. School Psychology Quarterly, 16(1), 91–112CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stage, S. A., Abbott, R. D., Jenkins, J. R., and Berninger, V. W. (2003). Predicting response to early reading intervention from verbal IQ, reading-related language abilities, attention ratings, and verbal IQ-word readings discrepancy: failure to validate discrepancy method. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 36(1), 24–33CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stein, J. F., Richardson, A. J. and Fowler, M. S. (2000). Monocular occlusion can improve binocular control and reading in dyslexics. Brain, 123, 164–70CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sternberg, R. J. and Grigorenko, E. L. (2002). Dynamic testing. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press
Stone, C. A. and Connell, P. J. (1993). Induction of a visual symbolic rule in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 36(3), 599–608CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swanson, H. L. (1999). Reading research for students with LD: a meta-analysis of intervention outcomes. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 32, 504–32CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swisher, L., Restrepo, M. A., Plante, E. and Lowell, S. (1995). Effect of implicit and explicit ‘rule’ presentation on bound-morpheme generalization in specific language impairment. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 38(1), 168–73CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thelen, E. (1989). Self-organization in developmental process: can systems approaches work? In M. Gunnar and E. Thelen (eds.) Systems and development. The Minnesota Symposium on Child Psychology, 22 (pp. 77–117). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum
Thelen, E. and Smith, L. B. (1994). A dynamic systems approach to the development of cognitions and action. Cambridge, MA: Bradford Books, MIT
Thelen, E. (1997). Dynamic systems theories. In W. Damon (series ed.) and R. M. Lerner (vol. ed.), Handbook of child psychology, vol. 1: theoretical models of human development (5th edn, pp. 563–634). New York, NY: Wiley
Thom, R. (1975). Structural stability and morphogenesis. Reading, MA; Benjamin
Thomas, H. (1989). A binomial mixture model for classification performance: a commentary on Waxman, Chambers, Yntema, and Gelman. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 48, 423–30CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thomas, H. and Lohaus, A. (1993). Modeling growth and individual differences in spatial tasks. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 58, (9, serial no. 237)CrossRef
Thomas, H. and Turner, G. F. W. (1991). Individual differences and development in water-level task performance. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 51, 171–94CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tressoldi, P. E., Stella, G. and Faggella, M. (2001). The development of reading speed in Italians with dyslexia: a longitudinal study. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 34(5), 414–17CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Uhry, J. K. and Shepherd, M. J. (1997). Teaching phonological recoding to young children with phonological processing deficits: the effect on sight-vocabulary acquisition. Learning Disability Quarterly, 20(2), 104–25CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vadasy, P. F., Sanders, E. A., Peyton, J. A. and Jenkins, J. R. (2002). Timing and intensity of tutoring: a closer look at the conditions for effective early literacy tutoring. Learning Disabilities Research and Practice, 17(4), 227–41CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Daal, V. H. P. and Reitsma, P. (1999). Effects of outpatient treatment of dyslexia. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 32(5), 447–56CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maas, H. L. J. and Molenaar, P. M. C. (1992). Stagewise cognitive development: an application of catastrophe theory. Psychological Review, 99, 395–417CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Geert, P. (1991). A dynamic systems model of cognitive and language growth. Psychological Review, 98, 3–53CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van Geert, P. (1993). A dynamic systems model of cognitive growth: competition and support under limited resource conditions. In L. B. Smith and E. Thelen (eds.) A dynamic systems approach to development: applications. (pp. 265–331). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
van Geert, P. (1994). Dynamic systems of development: change between complexity and chaos. New York: Prentice Hall/Harvester Wheatsheaf
Geert, P. (1995). Dimensions of change: a semantic and mathematical analysis of learning and development. Human Development, 38, 322–31CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van Geert, P. (1997). Variability and fluctuation: a dynamic view. In E. Amsel and K. A. Renninger (eds.) Changes and development: issues of theory, method, and application (pp. 193–212). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum
Geert, P. and Dijk, M. (2002). Focus on variability: new tools to study intra-individual variability in developmental data. Infant Behavior and Development, 25, 340–74CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Strien, J. W., Stolk, B. D. and Zuiker, S. (1995). Hemisphere-specific treatment of dyslexia subtypes: better reading with anxiety-laden words? Journal of Learning Disabilities, 28(1), 30–4CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Verbeke, G. and Molenberghs, G. (2000). Linear mixed models for longitudinal data. New York, NY: Springer
Viholainen, H., Ahonen, T., Cantell, M., Lyytinen, P. and Lyytinen, H. (2002). Development of early motor skills and language in children at risk for familial dyslexia. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 44(11), 761–9CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Von Eye, A. and Niedermeier, K. E. (1999). Statistical analysis of longitudinal categorical data in the social and behavioral sciences. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
Vygotsky, L. S. (1982). Myshlenie i rech' [Thought and language]. Moskva: Pedagogika
Wessling, R. and Reitsma, P. (2001). Preschool phonological representations and development of reading skills. Annals of Dyslexia, 51, 203–29CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wheldall, K. (2000). Does Rainbow Repeated Reading add value to an intensive literacy intervention program for low-progress readers? An experimental evaluation. Educational Review, 52(1), 29–36CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wimmers, R. H., Beek, P. J., Savelsbergh, G. J. P. and Hopkins, B. (1998). Developmental changes in action: theoretical and methodological issues. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 16, 45–63CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wright, B. D. and Stone, M. H. (1979). Best test design. Chicago: MESA
Yampolsky, S. and Waters, G. (2002). Treatment of single word oral reading in an individual with deep dyslexia. Aphasiology, 16(4–6), 455–71CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zeger, S. L. and Harlow, S. D. (1987). Mathematical models from laws of growth to tools for biological analysis: fifty years of growth. Growth, 51, 1–21Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×