Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-q99xh Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-23T09:26:24.635Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Chapter 18 - Cognitive Rehabilitation in Healthy Aging

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  30 November 2019

Kenneth M. Heilman
Affiliation:
University of Florida
Stephen E. Nadeau
Affiliation:
University of Florida
Get access

Summary

Healthy aging is accompanied by decline in a broad range of functions, including episodic learning and memory, working memory, attention, processing speed, and executive functioning. Significant efforts have therefore been made to augment these functions in healthy older adults. Two principal rehabilitation approaches have been employed: restorative and compensatory. Restorative approaches aim to repair the affected cognitive processes by repeated, adaptive practice. The majority of these restorative approaches have proved to be efficacious, and there is considerable evidence for maintenance of training effects weeks or months after the intervention was concluded. Transfer of restorative training approaches has been more elusive, although recent work has shown training effects on aspects of everyday life such as driving and instrumental activities of daily living. Compensatory approaches strive to bypass the impairment by teaching people strategies to bolster performance. Multimodal compensatory approaches that combine strategy training with counseling about other factors that affect cognition have been shown to help older adults learn new strategies, implement them to the benefit of cognitive performance, and adjust their views and expectations to better cope with the changes that occur during healthy aging.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2019

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Nyberg, L, Lövden, M, Riklund, K, et al. Memory aging and brain maintenance. Trends Cogn Sci 2012; 16: 292305.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rabbitt, P. Speed of visual search in old age: 1950 to 2016. J Gerontol Psychol Sci 2017; 72: 5160.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
West, RL. An application of prefrontal cortex function theory to cognitive aging. Psychol Bull 1996; 120: 272–92.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kliegel, M, Ballhausen, N, Hering, A, et al. Prospective memory in older adults: Where are we now and what is next. Gerontology 2016; 62: 459–66.Google Scholar
Tulving, E. Memory and consciousness. Can Psychol 1985; 26: 112.Google Scholar
Jennings, JM, Jacoby, LL. Improving memory in older adults: Training recollection. Neuropsychol Rehabil 2003; 13: 417–40.Google Scholar
Koen, JD, Yonelinas, AP. The effects of healthy aging, amnestic mild cognitive impairment, and Alzheimer’s disease on recollection and familiarity: A meta-analytic review. Neuropsychol Rev 2014; 24: 332–54.Google Scholar
Anderson, ND, Ebert, PL, Grady, CL, et al. Repetition lag training eliminates age-related recollection deficits (and gains are maintained after three months) but does not transfer: Implications for the fractionation of recollection. Psychol Aging 2018; 33: 93108.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bailey, H, Dagenbach, D, Jennings, JM. The locus of the benefits of repetition-lag memory training. Aging Neuropsychol Cogn 2011; 18: 577–93.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bissig, D, Lustig, C. Who benefits from memory training? Psychol Sci 2007; 18: 720–6.Google Scholar
Jennings, JM, Webster, LM, Kleykamp, BA, et al. Recollection training and transfer effects in older adults: Successful use of a repetition-lag procedure. Aging Neuropsychol Cogn 2005; 12: 278–98.Google Scholar
Lustig, C, Flegal, KE. Targeting latent function: Encouraging effective encoding for successful memory training and transfer. Psychol Aging 2008; 23: 754–64.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stamenova, V, Jennings, JM, Cook, SP, et al. Training recollection in healthy older adults: Clear improvements on the training task, but little evidence of transfer. Front Hum Neurosci 2014; 8: 898.Google Scholar
Bellander, M, Eschen, A, Lövdén, M, et al. No evidence for improved associative memory performance following process-based associative memory training in older adults. Front Aging Neurosci 2017; 8: 326.Google Scholar
Saksida, LM, Bussey, TJ. The representational-hierarchical view of amnesia: Translation from animal to human. Neuropsychologia 2010; 48: 2370–84.Google Scholar
Ranganath, C. A unified framework for the functional organization of the medial temporal lobes and the phenomenology of episodic memory. Hippocampus 2010; 20: 1263–90.Google Scholar
Troyer, AK, Murphy, KJ, Anderson, ND, et al. Associative recognition in mild cognitive impairment: Relationship to hippocampal volume and apolipoprotein E. Neuropsychologia 2012; 50: 3721–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zimmermann, K, von Bastian, CC, Röcke, C, et al. Transfer after process-based object-location memory training in healthy older adults. Psychol Aging 2016; 31: 798814.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dahlin, E, Neely, AS, Larsson, A, et al. Transfer of learning after updating training mediated by the striatum. Science 2008; 320, 1510–12.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Borella, E, Carretti, B, Cantarella, A, et al. Benefits of training visuospatial working memory in young-old and old-old. Dev Psychol 2014; 50: 714–27.Google Scholar
Borella, E, Carretti, B, Riboldi, F, et al. Working memory training in older adults: Evidence of transfer and maintenance effects. Psychol Aging 2010; 25: 767–78.Google Scholar
Borella, E, Carretti, B, Zanoni, G, et al. Working memory training in old age: An examination of transfer and maintenance effects. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 2013; 28: 331–47.Google Scholar
Borella, E, Carretti, B, Sciore, R, et al. Training working memory in older adults: Is there an advantage of using strategies? Psychol Aging 2017; 32: 178–91.Google Scholar
Brehmer, Y, Westerberg, H, Bäckman, L. Working-memory training in younger and older adults: Training gains, transfer, and maintenance. Front Hum Neurosci 2012; 6: 63.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buschkuehl, M, Jaeggi, SM, Hutchison, S, et al. Impact of working memory training on memory performance in old-old adults. Psychol Aging 2008; 23: 743–53.Google Scholar
Dahlin, E, Nyberg, L, Bäckman, L, et al. Plasticity of executive functioning in young and older adults: Immediate training gains, transfer, and long-term maintenance. Psychol Aging 2008; 23: 720–30.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goghari, VM, Lawlor-Savage, L. Comparison of cognitive change after working memory training and logic and planning training in healthy older adults. Front Aging Neurosci 2017; 9: 39.Google Scholar
Heinzel, S, Schulte, S, Onken, J, et al. Working memory training improvements and gains in non-trained cognitive tasks in young and older adults. Aging Neuropsychol Cogn 2014; 21: 146–73.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heinzel, S, Lorenz, RC, Pelz, P, et al. Neural correlates of training and transfer effects in working memory in older adults. NeuroImage 2016; 134: 236–49.Google Scholar
Payne, BR, Stine-Morrow, EAL. The effects of home-based cognitive training on verbal working memory and language comprehension in older adulthood. Front Aging Neurosci 2017: 9: 256.Google Scholar
Richmond, LL, Morrison, AB, Chein, JM, et al. Working memory training and transfer in older adults. Psychol Aging 2011; 26: 813–22.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Von Bastian, CC, Langer, N, Jäncke, L, et al. Effects of working memory training in young and old adults. Mem Cognit 2013; 41 : 611–24.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wayne, RV, Hamilton, C, Jones Huyck, J, et al. Working memory training and speech in noise comprehension in older adults. Front Aging Neurosci 2016; 8: 49.Google Scholar
Zinke, K, Zeintl, M, Eschen, A, et al. Potentials and limits of plasticity induced by working memory training in old-old age. Gerontology 2012; 58: 7987.Google Scholar
Zinke, K, Zeintl, M, Rose, NS, et al. Working memory training and transfer in older adults: Effects of age, baseline performance, and training gains. Dev Psychol 2014; 50: 304–15.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McAvinue, LP, Golemme, M, Castorina, M, et al. An evaluation of a working memory training scheme in older adults. Front Aging Neurosci 2013; 5: 20.Google Scholar
Karbach, J, Verhaeghen, P. Making working memory work: A meta-analysis of executive-control and working memory training in older adults. Psychol Sci 2014; 25: 2027–37.Google Scholar
Melby-Lervåg, M, Hulme, C. There is no convincing evidence that working memory training is effective: A reply to Au et al. (2014) and Karbach and Verhaeghen (2014). Psychon Bull Rev 2016; 23: 324–30.Google Scholar
McDowd, JM. The effects of age and extended practice on divided attention performance. J Gerontol 1986; 41: 764–9.Google Scholar
Kramer, AF, Larish, JF, Strayer, DL. Training for attentional control in dual task settings: A comparison of young and old adults. J Exp Psychol: Applied 1995; 1 : 5076.Google Scholar
Belleville, S, Mellah, S, de Boysson, C, et al. The pattern and loci of training-induced brain changes in healthy older adults are predicted by the nature of the intervention. PLoS ONE 2014; 9: e102710.Google Scholar
Bier, B, de Boysson, C, Belleville, S. Identifying training modalities to improve multitasking in older adults. Age 2014; 36: 9688.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lussier, M, Bugaiska, A, Bherer, L. Specific transfer effects following variable priority dual-task training in older adults. Restor Neurol Neurosci 2017; 35: 237–50.Google Scholar
Bherer, L, Kramer, AF, Peterson, MS, et al. Training effects in dual-task performance: Are there age-related differences in plasticity of attentional control? Psychol Aging 2005; 20: 695709.Google Scholar
Ball, K, Beard, B, Roenker, D, et al. Visual search: Age and practice. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci 1988; 29: 448.Google Scholar
Ball, K, Berch, DB, Helmers, KF, et al. Effects of cognitive training interventions with older adults. JAMA 2002; 288: 2271.Google Scholar
Edwards, JD, Wadley, VG, Myers, RS, et al. Transfer of a speed of processing intervention to near and far cognitive functions. Gerontology 2002; 48: 329–40.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Edwards, JD, Wadley, VG, Vance, DE et al. The impact of speed of processing training on cognitive and everyday performance. Aging Ment Health 2005; 9: 262–71.Google Scholar
Roenker, DL, Cissell, GM, Ball, KK, et al. Speed-of-processing and driving simulator training results in improved driving performance. Hum Factors 2003; 45: 218–33.Google Scholar
Ross, LA, Edwards, JD, O’Conner, ML, et al. The transfer of cognitive speed of processing training to older adults’ driving mobility across 5 years. J Gerontol Psychol Sci 2016 ; 71: 8797.Google Scholar
Edwards, JD, Xu, H, Clark, DO, et al. Speed of processing training results in lower risk of dementia. Alzheimers Dement 2017; 3: 603–11.Google Scholar
Mishra, J, de Villers-Sidani, E, Merzenich, M, et al. Adaptive training diminishes distractibility in aging across species. Neuron 2014; 84: 1091–103.Google Scholar
Rolle, CE, Anguera, JA, Skinner, SN, et al. Enhancing spatial attention and working memory in younger and older adults. J Cogn Neurosci 2017; 29: 1483–97.Google Scholar
van Hooren, SAH, Valentijn, SAM, Bosma, H, et al. Effect of a structured course involving goal management training in older adults: A randomised controlled trial. Patient Educ Couns 2007; 65: 205–13.Google Scholar
Levine, B, Robertson, IH, Clare, L, et al. Rehabilitation of executive functioning: An experimental-clinical validation of goal management training. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2000; 6: 299312.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hertzog, C, Hultsch, DF. Metacognition in adulthood and old age. In: Craik, FIM, Salthouse, TA, eds. The handbook of aging and cognition (2nd ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum, 2000; 417–66.Google Scholar
Hultch, DF, Hertzog, C, Dixon, RA. Age differences in metamemory: Resolving the inconsistencies. Can J Psychol 1987; 41: 193208.Google Scholar
Craik, FIM. On the transfer of information from temporary to permanent memory. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 1983; 302: 341–59.Google Scholar
Craik, FIM. A functional account of age differences in memory. In: Klix, F, Hagendorf, H, eds. Human memory and cognitive capabilities, mechanisms, and performances. Amsterdam: Elsevier, 1986; 409–22.Google Scholar
Bailey, HR, Dunlosky, J, Hertzog, C. Does strategy training reduce age-related deficits in working memory? Gerontology 2014; 60: 346–56.Google Scholar
Craik, FIM, Byrd, M. Aging and cognitive deficits: The role of attentional resources. In: Craik, FIM, Trehub, S. eds. Aging and cognitive processes. New York: Plenum, 1982; 191211.Google Scholar
Troyer, AK, Hafliger, A, Cadieux, MJ, et al. Name and face learning in older adults: Effects of level of processing, self-generation, and intention to learn. J Gerontol: Psychol Sci 2006; 61: 6774.Google Scholar
Kuhlmann, BG, Touron, DR. Aging and memory improvement through semantic clustering: The role of list-presentation format. Psychol Aging 2016; 31: 771–85.Google Scholar
Yesavage, JA. Imagery pretraining and memory training in the elderly. Gerontology 1983; 29: 271–5.Google Scholar
Yesavage, JA, Rose, TL, Bower, GH. Interactive imagery and affective judgments improve face–name learning in the elderly. J Gerontol 1983; 38: 197203.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kirchhoff, BA, Anderson, BA, Barch, DM, et al. Cognitive and neural effects of semantic encoding strategy training in older adults. Cereb Cortex 2012; 22: 788–99.Google Scholar
Kirchhoff, BA, Anderson, BA, Smith, SE, et al. Cognitive training-related changes in hippocampal activity associated with recollection in older adults. NeuroImage 2012; 62: 1956–64.Google Scholar
Gross, AL, Rebok, GW. Memory training and strategy use in older adults: Results from the ACTIVE study. Psychol Aging 2011; 26: 503–37.Google Scholar
Anschutz, L, Camp, CJ, Markley, RP, et al. Maintenance and generalization of mnemonics for grocery shopping by older adults. Exp Aging Res 1985; 11: 157–60.Google Scholar
Kliegl, R, Smith, J, Baltes, PB. Testing-the-limits and the study of adult age differences in cognitive plasticity of a mnemonic skill. Dev Psychol 1989; 25: 247–56.Google Scholar
Yesavage, JA, Rose, TL. Semantic elaboration and the method of loci: A new trip for older learners. Exp Aging Res 1984; 10: 155–9.Google Scholar
Anschutz, L, Camp, CJ, Markley, RP, et al. Remembering mnemonics: A three-year follow-up on the effects of mnemonics training in elderly adults. Experimental Aging Research 1987; 13: 141–3.Google Scholar
Gross, AL, Brandt, J, Bandeen-Roche, K, et al. Do older adults use the method of loci? Results from the ACTIVE study. Exp Aging Res 2014; 40: 140–63.Google Scholar
Brooks, JO 3rd, Friedman, L, Yesavage, JA. A study of the problems older adults encounter when using a mnemonic technique. Int Psychogeriatrs 1993; 5: 5765.Google Scholar
Gollwitzer, PM. Implementation intentions. Am Psychol 1999; 54: 493503.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gollwitzer, PM, Sheeran, P. Implementation intentions and goal achievement: A meta analysis of effects and processes. Adv Exp Soc Psychol 2006; 38: 69119.Google Scholar
Chasteen, AL, Park, DC, Schwarz, N. Implementation intentions and facilitation of prospective memory. Psychol Sci 2001; 12: 457–61.Google Scholar
Chen, X-J, Wang, Y, Liu, L-L, et al. The effect of implementation intentions on prospective memory: A systematic and meta-analytic review. Psychiatry Res 2015; 226: 1422.Google Scholar
Verhaeghen, P, Marcoen, A, Goossens, L. Improving memory performance in the aged through mnemonic training: A meta-analytic study. Psychol Aging 1992; 7: 242–51.Google Scholar
Gross, AL, Parisi, JM, Spira, AP, et al. Memory training interventions for older adults: A meta-analysis. Aging Ment Health 2012; 16: 722–34.Google Scholar
Brom, SS, Kliegel, M. Improving everyday prospective memory performance in older adults: Comparing cognitive process and strategy training. Psychol Aging 2014; 29: 744–55.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rebok, GW, Langbaum, JB, Jones, RN, et al. Memory training in the ACTIVE study: How much is needed and who benefits? Aging Ment Health 2013; 25 : 21S42S.Google Scholar
Rebok, GW, Ball, K, Guey, LT, et al. Ten-year effects of the advanced cognitive training for independent and vital elderly cognitive training trial on cognition and everyday functioning in older adults. J Am Geriatr Soc 2014; 62: 1624.Google Scholar
Wolinsky, FD, Unverzagt, FW, Smith, DM, et al. The ACTIVE cognitive training trial and health-related quality of life: Protection that lasts 5 years. J Gerontol: Med Sci 2006; 61: 1324–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stigsdotter, A, Bäckman, L. Multifactorial memory training with older adults: How to foster maintenance of improved performance. Gerontology 1989; 35: 260–7.Google Scholar
Stigsdotter Neely, A, Bäckman, L. Maintenance of gains following multifactorial and unifactorial memory training in late adulthood. Educ Gerontol 1993; 19 : 105–17.Google Scholar
Stigsdotter Neely, A, Bäckman, L. Long-term maintenance of gains from memory training in older adults: Two 3 1/2-year follow-up studies. J Gerontol 1993; 48: P233–7.Google Scholar
Belleville, S, Gilbert, B, Fontaine, F, et al. Improvement of episodic memory in persons with mild cognitive impairment and healthy older adults: Evidence from a cognitive intervention program. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 2006; 22: 486–99.Google Scholar
Best, DL, Hamlett, KW, Davis, SW. Memory complaint and memory performance in the elderly: The effects of memory-skills training and expectancy change. Appl Cogn Psychol 1992; 6: 405–16.Google Scholar
Dellefield, KS, McDougall, GJ. Increasing metamemory in older adults. Nurs Res 1996; 45: 284–90.Google Scholar
Fabre, C, Massé-Biron, J, Chamari, K, et al. Evaluation of quality of life in elderly healthy subjects after aerobic and/or mental training. Arch Gerontol Geriatr 1999; 28: 922.Google Scholar
Fairchild, JK, Scogin, FR. Training to enhance adult memory (TEAM): An investigation of the effectiveness of a memory training program with older adults. Aging Mentl Health 2010; 14: 364–73.Google Scholar
Hastings, EC, West, RL. The relative success of a self-help and a group-based memory training program for older adults. Psychol Aging 2009; 24: 586–94.Google Scholar
Kinsella, GJ, Ames, D, Storey, E, et al. Strategies for improving memory: A randomized trial of memory groups for older people, including those with mild cognitive impairment. J Alzheimer Dis 2016; 49: 3143.Google Scholar
Lima-Silva, TB, Ordonez, TN, Santos, GD, et al. Effects of cognitive training based on metamemory and mental images. Dement Neuropsychol 2010; 4, 114–19.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mohs, RC, Ashman, TA, Jantzen, K, et al. A study of the efficacy of a comprehensive memory enhancement program in healthy elderly persons. Psychiatry Res 1998; 77: 183–95.Google Scholar
West, RL, Bagwell, DK, Dark-Freudeman, A. Self-efficacy and memory aging: The impact of a memory intervention based on self-efficacy. Aging Neuropsychol Cogn 2008; 15: 302–29.Google Scholar
McDougall, GJ, Becker, H, Pituch, K, et al. The SeniorWISE study: Improving everyday memory in older adults. Arch Psychiatr Nurs 2010; 24: 291306.Google Scholar
Troyer, AK. Improving memory knowledge, satisfaction, and functioning via an education and intervention program for older adults. Aging Neuropsychol Cogn 2001; 8: 256–68.Google Scholar
Wiegand, MA, Troyer, AK, Gojmerac, C, et al. Facilitating change in health-related behaviors and intentions: A randomized controlled trial of a multidimensional memory program for older adults. Aging Ment Health 2013; 17: 806–15.Google Scholar
Vandermorris, S, Davidson, S, Au, A, et al. “Accepting where I’m at”: A qualitative study of the mechanisms, benefits, and impact of a behavioral memory intervention for community-dwelling older adults. Aging Ment Health 2017; 21: 895901.Google Scholar
Stuss, DT, Robertson, IH, Craik, FIM, et al. Cognitive rehabilitation in the elderly: A randomized trial to evaluate a new protocol. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2007; 13: 120–31.Google Scholar
Levine, B, Stuss, DT, Winocur, G, et al. Cognitive rehabilitation in the elderly: Effects on strategic behavior in relation to goal management. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2007; 13: 143–52.Google Scholar
Craik, FIM, Winocur, G, Palmer, H, et al. Cognitive rehabilitation in the elderly: Effects on memory. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2007; 13: 132–42.Google Scholar
Winocur, G, Palmer, H, Dawson, DR, et al. Cognitive rehabilitation in the elderly: An evaluation of psychosocial factors. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2007; 13: 153–65.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×