Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-j824f Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-20T05:04:55.540Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

7 - Cognition and tasks

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 October 2012

Peter Robinson
Affiliation:
Aoyama Gakuin University, Japan
Get access

Summary

Introduction

Unlike any other skill that you could care to mention (algebra, tennis, driving, playing the bagpipes), language can work well despite poor execution. Its meaning is recoverable even when its form is incorrect. ‘Me Tarzan, you Jane’, or ‘ET phone home’ are not grammatical, nor particularly subtle, but they get the job done. This useful characteristic of language means that learners can decode and encode the semantic contents of a message without attending to all of its syntactic packaging. Anderson and Lynch (1988), for example, show how contextual and discourse knowledge-sources enable comprehension to proceed for native and non-native speakers without exhaustive bottom-up analysis of a message. Schematic knowledge enables the redundancy in language to be exploited for meaning-extraction. Productively, it is clear that second language (L2) users draw upon a range of communication strategies (Bygate, 1988; Faerch & Kasper, 1983; Kasper & Kellerman, 1997) enabling them to communicate meanings despite limited resources. Individual case studies, e.g., of Wes (Schmidt, 1983) and Schmidt (Schmidt & Frota, 1986), as well as larger scale research such as that evaluating immersion education in Canada (Harley, Allen, Cummins & Swain, 1990; Swain & Lapkin, 1982) also indicates how the need to engage in output does not automatically lead to acquisitional progress. In evolutionary terms, it is entirely plausible that the language system should be robust in this way – requiring crystalline precision in all communication could well have been decidedly dangerous for our ancestors' health and survival.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2001

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×