Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-fbnjt Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-20T05:18:44.673Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

1 - Information Processing and Intelligence: Where We Are and Where We Are Going

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  23 November 2009

Robert J. Sternberg
Affiliation:
Yale University, Connecticut
Jean E. Pretz
Affiliation:
Yale University, Connecticut
Get access

Summary

INTRODUCTION

Intelligence tests are about one hundred years old. If you agree with Boring (1923) that intelligence is what the intelligence tests measure, then the science of intelligence is one hundred years old. I will call this psychometrically defined intelligence. Empirically the study of psychometric intelligence is a booming field, for it has led to a very large literature, impressive technological developments, and coherent relationships among test scores (Carroll, 1993). However, it has a weakness.

A purely psychometric approach to intelligence lets the technology of measurement define the concept, rather than the concept defining an appropriate measurement technology. Along with many others, I prefer a more conceptual, less boring approach. The conceptual definition of intelligence as individual variation in mental competence has a longer history. In the sixteenth century the Spanish philosopher Juan Huarte de San Juan (Huarte, 1575/1991) proposed a multifaceted theory of intelligence that was not too far from today's crystallized–fluid distinction. In the nineteenth century, Galton (1883) used laboratory techniques for measuring individual differences in basic mental processes that are recognizable ancestors of paradigms used in today's laboratories. And for that matter, Binet, the founder of modern testing, was not entirely atheoretic (Sternberg, 1990). All interesting theories of intelligence try to go beyond test scores to connect individual differences with a theory of how the mind works. Developing such a theory is the province of cognitive psychology.

Type
Chapter
Information
Cognition and Intelligence
Identifying the Mechanisms of the Mind
, pp. 1 - 25
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2004

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Abad, F. J., Colom, R., Juan-Espinosa, M., & Garcia, L. F. (2003). Intelligence differentiation in adult samples. Intelligence, 31(2), 157–166CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baddeley, A. D. (1986). Working memory. Oxford: Oxford University Press
Baddeley, A. D. (1992). Working memory. Science, 255, 556–559CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boring, E. G. (1923). Intelligence as the tests test it. New Republic, 35(June 6), 35–37Google Scholar
Brody, N. (2003). Construct validation of the Sternberg Triarchic Abilities Test: Comment and reanalysis. Intelligence, 31(4), 319–330CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carpenter, P. A., Just, M. A., & Shell, P. (1990). What one intelligence test measures. A theoretical account of processing in the Raven Progressive Matrix Test. Psychological Review, 97(3), 404–431CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carroll, J. B. (1993). Human cognitive abilities. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press
Cattell, R. B. (1971). Abilities: Their structure, growth, and action. Boston: Houghton Mifflin
Cronbach, L. J. (1957). The two disciplines of scientific psychology. American Psychologist, 12, 671–684CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Daneman, M., & Merikle, P. M. (1996). Working memory and language comprehension: A meta-analysis. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 3(4), 422–433CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Deary, I. J., Egan, V., Gibson, G. J., Austin, E. J., Brand, C. R., & Kellaghan, T. (1996). Intelligence and the differentiation hypothesis. Intelligence, 23(2), 105–132CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Detterman, D. K., & Daniel, M. H. (1989). Correlations of mental tests with each other and with cognitive variables are highest in low IQ groups. Intelligence, 13, 349–360CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Galton, F. (1883). Inquiry into human faculty and its development. London: MacMillan
Gardner, H. (1983). Frames of mind: The theory of multiple intelligences. New York: Basic Books
Gardner, H., Kornhaber, M. L., & Wake, W. K. (1996). Intelligence: Multiple Perspectives. New York: Harcourt Brace
Garlick, D. (2002). Understanding the nature of the general factor of intelligence: The role of individual differences in neural plasticity as an explanatory mechanism. Psychological Review, 109, 116–136CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goleman, D. (1995). Emotional intelligence. New York: Bantam Books
Gottfredson, L. S. (1997). Why g matters: The complexity of everyday life. Intelligence, 24(1), 79–132CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grudnik, J. L., & Kranzler, J. H. (2001). Meta-analysis of the relationship between intelligence and inspection time. Intelligence, 29, 523–535CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Herrnstein, R. J., & Murray, C. (1994). The Bell Curve: Intelligence and Class Structure in American Life. New York: The Free Press
Hick, W. E. (1952). On the rate of gain of information. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 4, 11–26CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Horn, J. L. (1985). Remodeling old models of intelligence. In B. B. Wolman (Ed.), Handbook of intelligence. Theories, measurements, and applications (pp. 267–300). New York: Wiley
Horn J. L., & Noll, J. (1994). A system for understanding cognitive capabilities: A theory and the evidence on which it is based. In D. Detterman (Ed.), Current Topics in Human Intelligence: Vol. 4. Theories of Intelligence (pp. 151–204). Norwood, NJ: Ablex
Huarte de San Juan, J. (1575/1991). Examen de ingenios para las ciencias (Edicion Felisa Fresco Otero). Madrid: Espasa-Calpe S.A.
Hunt, E. (1974). Quote the Raven? Nevermore! In L. W. Gregg (Ed.), Knowledge and cognition (pp. 129–157). Potomac, MD: Erlbaum
Hunt, E. (1980). Intelligence as an information processing concept. British Journal of Psychology, 71(4), 449–474CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hunt, E. (1995a). Will we be smart enough? A cognitive analysis of the coming workforce. New York: Russell Sage
Hunt, E. (1995b). The role of intelligence in modern society. American Scientist 83, July-August, 356–368Google Scholar
Hunt, E. (2002). Thoughts on thought. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum
Hunt, E., Frost, N., & Lunneborg, C. E. (1973). Individual differences in cognition: A new approach to intelligence. In G. S. Bower (Ed.), Advances in learning and motivation (Vol. 7, pp. 87–123). New York: Academic PressCrossRef
Hunt, E., Lunneborg, C. E., & Lewis, J. (1975). What does it mean to be high verbal? Cognitive Psychology, 7, 194–227CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jensen, A. R. (1982). The chronometry of intelligence. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Advances in the psychology of human intelligence (Vol. 1, pp. 235–310). Hilldale, NJ: Erlbaum
Jensen, A. R. (1987). Individual differences in the Hick paradigm. In P. A. Vernon (Ed.), Speed of information processing and intelligence (pp. 101–176). Norwood, NJ: Ablex
Jensen, A. R. (1998). The g factor: The science of mental ability. Westport, CT: Praeger
Kane, M. J., & Engle, R. W. (2002). The role of prefrontal cortex in working memory capacity, executive attention, and general fluid intelligence: An individual differences perspective. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 9(4), 637–671CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kyllonen, P. C., & Christal, R. E. (1990). Reasoning ability is (little more than) working memory capacity?!Intelligence, 14, 389–433CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kyllonen, P. C., & Stephens, D. L. (1990). Cognitive abilities as determinants of success in acquiring logic skills. Learning and Individual Differences, 2(2), 129–160CrossRefGoogle Scholar
LeDoux, J. (2002). The synaptic self. How our brains become who we are. New York: Viking
Logie, R. H. (1995). Visuo-spatial working memory. Hove, UK: Erlbaum
Lopez, A., Atran, S., Coley, J. D., Medin, D. L., & Smith, E. E. (1997). The tree of life: Universal and cultural features of folkbiological taxonomies and inductions. Cognitive Psychology, 32(3), 251–295CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Luce, R. D. (1986). Response times: Their role in inferring elementary mental organization. New York: Oxford University Press
MacDonald, M. C., Just, M. A., & Carpenter, P. A. (1992). Working memory constraints on the processing of syntactic ambiguity. Cognitive Psychology, 24, 56–98CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McGill, W. J. (1963). Stochastic latency mechanisms. In R. D. Luce, R. R. Bush, & E. Galanter (Eds.), Handbook of mathematical psychology: Volume I (pp. 309–360). New York: Wiley
Nettlebeck, T. (1987). Inspection time and intelligence. In P. A. Vernon (Ed.), Speed of information processing and intelligence (pp. 295–346). Norwood, NJ: Ablex
Nettlebeck, T. (2001). Correlations between inspection time and psychometric ability: A personal interpretation. Intelligence, 29, 459–474CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Palmer, J., MacLeod, C. M., Hunt, E., & Davidson, J. (1985). Information processing correlates of reading. Journal of Memory and Language, 24, 59–88CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Posner, M. I., Boies, S. E., Eichelman, W. H., & Taylor, R. L. (1969). Retention of visual and name codes of single letters. Journal of Experimental Psychological Monographs 79, 1–16CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Posner, M. I., & Raichle, M. E. (1994). Images of mind. San Francisco: Freeman
Roberts, R. D., Goff, G. N., Anjoul, F., Kyllonen, P. C., Pallier, G., & Stankov, L. (2000). The Armed Services Vocational Aptitude Battery (ASVAB): Little more than acculturated learning (Gc)!? Learning and Individual Differences, 12(1), 81–103CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schacter, D. L. (1996). Searching for memory: The brain, the mind, and the past. New York: Basic Books
Schmidt, F. L., & Hunter, J. E. (1998). The validity and utility of selection methods in personnel psychology: Practical and theoretical implications of 85 years of research findings. Psychological Bulletin, 124(2), 262–274CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schretlen, D., Pearlson, G. D., Anthony, J. C., Aylward, E. H., Augustine, A. M., Davis, A., & Barta, P. (2000). Elucidating the contributions of processing speed, executive ability, and frontal lobe volume to normal age-related differences in fluid intelligence. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 6, 52–61CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sloman, S. A. (1996). The empirical case for two systems of reasoning. Psychological Bulletin, 119(1), 3–22CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, E. E., & Jonides, J. J. (1997). Working memory: A view from neuroimaging. Cognitive Psychology, 33(1), 5–42CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spearman, C. (1923). The nature of “intelligence” and the principles of cognition. London: Methuen
Sternberg, R. J. (1977). Component processes in analogical reasoning. Psychological Review, 84, 353–378CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sternberg, R. J. (1988). The triarchic mind: A new theory of human intelligence. New York: Viking
Sternberg, R. J. (1990). Metaphors of mind. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press
Sternberg, R. J. (1996). Successful intelligence: How practical and creative intelligence determines success in life. New York: Simon & Schuster
Sternberg, R. J. (2003). Issues in the theory and measurement of successful intelligence: A reply to Brody. Intelligence, 31(4), 331–338CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sternberg, R. J., Forsythe, G. B., Hedlund, J., Horvath, J. A., Wagner, R. K., Williams, W. M., Snook, S. A., & Grigorenko, E. L. (2000). Practical intelligence in everyday life. New York: Cambridge University Press

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×