Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-xbtfd Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-20T05:26:09.654Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

8 - Ability Determinants of Individual Differences in Skilled Performance

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  23 November 2009

Robert J. Sternberg
Affiliation:
Yale University, Connecticut
Jean E. Pretz
Affiliation:
Yale University, Connecticut
Get access

Summary

At the most fundamental level, the relationship between intelligence and learning is close and convincing. Indeed, the modern era of intelligence assessment is identified with the critical success of Binet and Simon (1905) in their development of a set of scales that provided valid predictions of school success. These scales, or similar assessments inspired by this approach (such as the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children; Wechsler, 1949), continue to represent the best predictors of school success. School success, at least for children and adolescents, is considered by many to be the indicator of learning achievement. While this analysis works quite well for global measures of learning, there is far less utility of omnibus IQ-type measures for predicting individual differences in narrower domains of learning. If we want to predict which students will excel in learning a musical instrument, mastering power tools, or becoming adept at a particular sport, or even which students will become the fastest typists, the relationship between intelligence and learning appears to be much more complicated.

Part of the reason why IQ-type measures are less valid for predicting individual differences in skilled performance has to do with the relative “bandwidth” of the assessment instrument and the breadth of the criterion, or what has been referred to as a lack of Brunswik symmetry (Wittmann & Süß, 1999). That is, IQ tests have high bandwidth — they are typically constructed from as many as a dozen different scales (e.g., memory, reasoning, vocabulary, math, etc.).

Type
Chapter
Information
Cognition and Intelligence
Identifying the Mechanisms of the Mind
, pp. 142 - 159
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2004

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Ackerman, P. L. (1987). Individual differences in skill learning: An integration of psychometric and information processing perspectives. Psychological Bulletin, 102, 3–27CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ackerman, P. L. (1988). Determinants of individual differences during skill acquisition: Cognitive abilities and information processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 117, 288–318CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ackerman, P. L. (1989). Within-task intercorrelations of skilled performance: Implications for predicting individual differences? Journal of Applied Psychology, 74, 360–364CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ackerman, P. L. (1990). A correlational analysis of skill specificity: Learning, abilities, and individual differences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 16, 883–901Google Scholar
Ackerman, P. L. (1992). Predicting individual differences in complex skill acquisition: Dynamics of ability determinants. Journal of Applied Psychology, 77, 598–614CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ackerman, P. L., & Cianciolo, A. T. (1999). Psychomotor abilities via touchpanel testing: Measurement innovations, construct, and criterion validity. Human Performance, 12, 231–273Google Scholar
Ackerman, P. L., & Cianciolo, A. T. (2000). Cognitive, perceptual speed, and psychomotor determinants of individual differences during skill acquisition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 6, 259–290Google ScholarPubMed
Ackerman, P. L., Kanfer, R., & Goff, M. (1995). Cognitive and noncognitive determinants and consequences of complex skill acquisition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 1, 270–304Google Scholar
Ackerman, P. L., & Kyllonen, P. C. (1991). Trainee characteristics. In J. E. Morrison (Ed.), Training for performance: Principles of applied human learning (pp. 193–229). West Sussex, UK: Wiley
Ackerman, P. L., & Woltz, D. J. (1994). Determinants of learning and performance in an associative memory/substitution task: Task constraints, individual differences, and volition. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86, 487–515CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Adams, J. A. (1957). The relationship between certain measures of ability and acquisition of a psychomotor response. Journal of General Psychology, 56, 121–134CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Adams, J. A. (1987). Historical review and appraisal of research on the learning, retention and transfer of human motor skills. Psychological Bulletin, 101, 41–74CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barrett, G. V., Alexander, R. A., & Doverspike, D. (1992). The implications for personnel selection of apparent declines in predictive validities over time: A critique of Hulin, Henry, and Noon. Personnel Psychology, 45, 601–617CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Binet, A., & Simon, T. (1905). New methods for the diagnosis of the intellectual level of subnormals. L'Année Psychologique, 11, 191–244. Translated by Elizabeth S. Kite and reprinted in J. J. Jenkins & D. G. Paterson (Eds.), Studies of individual differences: The search for intelligence (pp. 90–96). New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts
Bryan, W. L., & Harter, N. (1899). Studies on the telegraphic language: The acquisition of a hierarchy of habits. Psychological Review, 6, 345–375Google Scholar
Card, S. K., Moran, T. P., & Newell, A. (1983). The psychology of human–computer interaction. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum
Carroll, J. B. (1980). Individual difference relations in psychometric and experimental cognitive tasks. (Tech. Rep. No. 163). Chapel Hill: University of North Carolina, The L. L. Thurstone Psychometric Laboratory. (Also NTIS document AD-A 086057 and ERIC Doc. ED-191–891.)CrossRef
Chaiken, S. R., Kyllonen, P. C., & Tirre, W. C. (2000). Organization and components of psychomotor ability. Cognitive Psychology, 40, 198–226CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cronbach, L. J., & Furby, L. (1970). How we should measure “change” — or should we? Psychological Bulletin, 74, 68–80CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deary, I. J., & Stough, C. (1996). Intelligence and inspection time: Achievements, prospects, and problems. American Psychologist, 51, 599–608CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ericsson, K. A., & Lehmann, A. C. (1996). Expert and exceptional performance: Evidence of maximal adaptation to task constraints. Annual Review of Psychology, 47, 273–305CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ferguson, G. A. (1956). On transfer and the abilities of man. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 10, 121–131CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fitts, P., & Posner, M. I. (1967). Human performance. Belmont, CA: Brooks/Cole
Fleishman, E. A. (1956). Psychomotor selection tests: Research and application in the U.S. Air Force. Personnel Psychology, 9, 449–467CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fleishman, E. A. (1972). On the relation between abilities, learning, and human performance. American Psychologist, 27, 1017–1032CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fleishman, E. A., & Hempel, W. E. Jr. (1955). The relation between abilities and improvement with practice in a visual discrimination reaction task. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 49, 301–316CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fleishman, E. A., & Hempel, W. E. Jr. (1956). Factorial analysis of complex psychomotor performance and related skills. The Journal of Applied Psychology, 40, 96–104CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frederiksen, J. R., Warren, B. M., & Rosebery, A. S. (1985). A componential approach to training reading skills: Part 1. Perceptual units training. Cognition and Instruction, 2, 91–130CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Henry, R. A., & Hulin, C. L. (1987). Stability of skilled performance across time: Some generalizations and limitations on utilities. Journal of Applied Psychology, 72, 457–462CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hulin, C. L., Henry, R. A., & Noon, S. L. (1990). Adding a dimension: Time as a factor in the generalizability of predictive relationships. Psychological Bulletin, 107, 1–13CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Humphreys, L. G., & Taber, T. (1973). Postdiction study of the graduate record examination and eight semesters of college grades. Journal of Educational Measurement, 10, 179–184CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hunt, E., Frost, N., & Lunneborg, C. (1973). Individual differences in cognition: A new approach to intelligence. In G. Bower (Ed.), Advances in learning and motivation (Vol. 7, pp. 87–122). New York: Academic pressCrossRef
Jensen, A. R. (1998). The g factor: The science of mental ability. Westport, CT: Praeger
Kanfer, R., & Ackerman, P. L. (1989). Motivation and cognitive abilities: An integrative/aptitude-treatment interaction approach to skill acquisition. Journal of Applied Psychology — Monograph, 74, 657–690CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kyllonen, P. C., & Christal, R. E. (1989). Cognitive modeling of learning abilities: A status report of LAMP. In R. Dillon and J. W. Pellegrino (Eds.), Testing: Theoretical and applied issues, San Francisco: Freeman
Kyllonen, P. C., & Stephens, D. L. (1990). Cognitive abilities as determinants of success in acquiring logic skill. Learning and Individual Differences, 2, 129–160CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kyllonen, P. C., & Woltz, D. J. (1989). Role of cognitive factors in the acquisition of cognitive skill. In R. Kanfer, P. L. Ackerman, & R. Cudeck (Eds.), Abilities, motivation, and methodology: The Minnesota symposium on learning and individual differences (pp. 239–280). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum
Kyllonen, P. C., Tirre, W. C., & Christal, R. E. (1991). Knowledge and processing speed as determinants of associative learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 120(1), 57–79CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lakatos, I. (Ed.) (1978). The methodology of scientific research programmes: Philosophical papers (Vol. 1). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University PressCrossRef
Lohman, D. F. (1999). Minding our p's and q's: On finding relationships between learning and intelligence. In P. L. Ackerman, P. C. Kyllonen, & R. D. Roberts (Eds.), Learning and individual differences: Process, trait, and content determinants (pp. 55–76). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association
Melton, A. W. (Ed.) (1947). Army Air Forces Aviation Psychology Program Research Reports: Apparatus Tests. Report No. 4. Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office
Pena, C. M., & Tirre, W. C. (1992). Cognitive factors involved in the first stage of programming skill acquisition. Learning and Individual Differences, 4, 311–334CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Popper, K. R. (1963). Conjectures and refutations. New York: Harper & Row
Sternberg, R. J. (1977). Intelligence, information processing, and analogical reasoning: The componential analysis of human abilities. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum
Sternberg, R. J. (1985). Human abilities: An information-processing approach. New York: Freeman
Thorndike, E. L. (1924). Measurement of intelligence. Psychological Review, 31, 219–252CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tirre, W. C. (1992). Can reading ability be measured with tests of memory and processing speed. Journal of General Psychology, 119, 141–160CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wechsler, D. (1949). Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children. NY: Psychological Corporation
Wittmann, W. W., & Süß, H.-M. (1999). Investigating the paths between working memory, intelligence, knowledge, and complex problem-solving performances via Brunswik symmetry. In P. L. Ackerman, P. C. Kyllonen, & R. D. Roberts (Eds.), Learning and Individual Differences: Process, Trait, and Content Determinants (pp. 77–108). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association
Woltz, D. J., Bell, B. G., Kyllonen, P. C., & Gardner, M. K. (1996). Memory for order of operations in the acquisition and transfer of sequential cognitive skills. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22, 438–457Google Scholar
Woodrow, H. (1946). The ability to learn. Psychological Review, 53, 147–158CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zeaman, D., & House, B. J. (1967). The relation of IQ and learning. In R. M. Gagné (Ed.), Learning and individual differences (pp. 192–212). Columbus, OH: Charles Merrill

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×