Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-r5fsc Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-22T15:51:43.708Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

20 - The Role of Working Memory in Language Comprehension and Production

Evidence from Neuropsychology

from Part IV - First Language Processing

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 July 2022

John W. Schwieter
Affiliation:
Wilfrid Laurier University
Zhisheng (Edward) Wen
Affiliation:
Hong Kong Shue Yan University
Get access

Summary

This chapter addresses the role of verbal working memory (WM) in language production and comprehension, focusing on data from brain-damaged individuals, while also drawing on related findings from healthy adults. The perspective on WM is the domain-specific model which includes WM buffers that are specific to phonological and semantic information and separate from long-term knowledge in these domains (Marti et al., 2020). Thus, the focus is on the separable contributions of these two buffers to language processes

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2022

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Alderete, J., & Davies, M. (2019). Investigating perceptual biases, data reliability, and data discovery in a methodology for collecting speech errors from audio recordings. Language and Speech, 62, 281317.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Allport, D. A. (1984). Speech production and comprehension: One lexicon or two? In Cognition and motor processes (pp. 209228). Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Allum, P. H., & Wheeldon, L. R. (2007). Planning scope in spoken sentence production: The role of grammatical units. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 33, 791.Google ScholarPubMed
Allum, P. H., & Wheeldon, L. (2009). Scope of lexical access in spoken sentence production: Implications for the conceptual–syntactic interface. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35, 1240.Google Scholar
Baddeley, A. D., Hitch, G. J., & Allen, R. J. (2021). A multicomponent model of working memory. In Working memory: The state of the science. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Baddeley, A. D., Thomson, N., & Buchanan, M. (1975). Word length and the structure of short-term memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 14, 575589.Google Scholar
Bemis, D. K., & Pylkkänen, L. (2013). Basic linguistic composition recruits the left anterior temporal lobe and left angular gyrus during both listening and reading. Cerebral Cortex, 23, 18591873.Google Scholar
Berndt, R. S., & Caramazza, A. (1980). A redefinition of the syndrome of Broca’s aphasia: Implications for a neuropsychological model of language. Applied Psycholinguistics, 1, 225278.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bock, K., & Levelt, W. (1994). Grammatical encoding. In Gernsbacher, M. A. (Ed.), Handbook of psycholinguistics (pp. 945984). Academic Press.Google Scholar
Burgess, N., & Hitch, G. J. (2006). A revised model of short-term memory and long-term learning of verbal sequences. Journal of Memory and Language, 55, 627652.Google Scholar
Butterworth, B., Campbell, R., & Howard, D. (1986). The uses of short-term memory: A case study. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 38, 705737.Google Scholar
Cecchetto, C., & Papagno, C. (2011). Bridging the gap between brain and syntax: A case for a role of the phonological loop. In Di Sciullo, A. M. & Boeckx, C. (Eds.), Biolinguistic approaches to language evolution and variation. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Cowan, N., Morey, C. C., & Naveh-Benjamin, M. (2021). An embedded-processes approach to working memory. In Working memory: The state of the science (p. 44). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Damian, M. F., & Dumay, N. (2007). Time pressure and phonological advance planning in spoken production. Journal of Memory and Language, 57, 195209.Google Scholar
Daneman, M., & Carpenter, P. A. (1980). Individual differences in working memory and reading. Journal of Memory and Language, 19, 450.Google Scholar
Dell, G. S. (1986). A spreading-activation theory of retrieval in sentence production. Psychological Review, 93, 283.Google Scholar
Engle, R. W. (2010). Role of working-memory capacity in cognitive control. Current Anthropology, 51, S17S26.Google Scholar
Friedmann, N., & Gvion, A. (2007). As far as individuals with conduction aphasia understood these sentences were ungrammatical: Garden path in conduction aphasia. Aphasiology, 21, 570586.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fromkin, V. A. (1971). The non-anomalous nature of anomalous utterances. Language, 27–52.Google Scholar
Garrett, M. (1980). Levels of processing in sentence production. In Language production, Vol. 1: Speech and talk (pp. 177220). Academic Press.Google Scholar
Griffin, Z. M., & Bock, K. (2000). What the eyes say about speaking. Psychological Science, 11, 274279.Google Scholar
Gvion, A., & Friedmann, N. (2012a). Does phonological working memory impairment affect sentence comprehension? A study of conduction aphasia. Aphasiology, 26, 494535.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gvion, A., & Friedmann, N. (2012b). Phonological short-term memory in conduction aphasia. Aphasiology, 26, 579614.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hamilton, A. C., Martin, R. C., & Burton, P. C. (2009). Converging functional magnetic resonance imaging evidence for a role of the left inferior frontal lobe in semantic retention during language comprehension. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 26, 685704.Google Scholar
Hickok, G., & Poeppel, D. (2007). The cortical organization of speech processing. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 8, 393402.Google Scholar
Levelt, W. J. (1989). Speaking: From intention to articulation. MIT Press Series in Natural-Language Processing. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Martin, A., & Chao, L. L. (2001). Semantic memory and the brain: Structure and processes. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 11, 194201.Google Scholar
Martin, N., & Saffran, E. M. (2002). The relationship of input and output phonological processing: An evaluation of models and evidence to support them. Aphasiology, 16(1–2), 107150. https://doi.org/10.1080/02687040143000447Google Scholar
Martin, R. C. (2021). The critical role of semantic working memory in language processing. Current Directions in Psychological Science.Google Scholar
Martin, R. C. (1987). Articulatory and phonological deficits in short-term memory and their relation to syntactic processing. Brain and Language, 32, 159192.Google Scholar
Martin, R. C., & Breedin, S. D. (1992). Dissociations between speech perception and phonological short-term memory deficits. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 9, 509534.Google Scholar
Martin, R. C., Crowther, J. E., Knight, M., Tamborello II, F. P., & Yang, C.-L. (2010). Planning in sentence production: Evidence for the phrase as a default planning scope. Cognition, 116, 177192.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martin, R. C., Ding, J., Hamilton, A. C., & Schnur, T. T. (2021). Working memory capacities neurally dissociate. Cerebral Cortex Communications.Google Scholar
Martin, R. C., & Freedman, M. L. (2001). Short-term retention of lexical-semantic representations: Implications for speech production. Memory, 9, 261280.Google Scholar
Martin, R. C., & He, T. (2004). Semantic short-term memory and its role in sentence processing: A replication. Brain and Language, 89, 7682.Google Scholar
Martin, R. C., Lesch, M. F., & Bartha, M. C. (1999). Independence of input and output phonology in word processing and short-term memory. Journal of Memory and Language, 41, 329.Google Scholar
Martin, R. C., Miller, M., & Vu, H. (2004). Lexical‐semantic retention and speech production: Further evidence from normal and brain‐damaged participants for a phrasal scope of planning. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 21, 625644.Google Scholar
Martin, R. C., Rapp, B., & Purcell, J. (2021). Domain-specific working memory. In Working memory: The state of the science. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Martin, R. C., & Romani, C. (1994). Verbal working memory and sentence comprehension: A multiple-components view. Neuropsychology, 8, 506523.Google Scholar
Martin, R. C., & Schnur, T. T. (2019). Independent contributions of semantic and phonological working memory to spontaneous speech in acute stroke. Cortex, 112, 5868.Google Scholar
Martin, R. C., Shelton, J. R., & Yaffee, L. S. (1994). Language processing and working memory: Neuropsychological evidence for separate phonological and semantic capacities. Journal of Memory and Language, 33, 83111.Google Scholar
Martin, R. C., Wu, D., Freedman, M., Jackson, E. F., & Lesch, M. (2003). An event-related fMRI investigation of phonological versus semantic short-term memory. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 16, 341360.Google Scholar
Meyer, A. S. (1996). Lexical access in phrase and sentence production: Results from picture–word interference experiments. Journal of Memory and Language, 35, 477496.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miyake, A., Just, M., & Carpenter, P. (1994). A capacity approach to syntactic comprehension disorders: making normal adults perform like aphasic patients. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 11, 671717.Google Scholar
Norris, D. (2017). Short-term memory and long-term memory are still different. Psychological Bulletin, 143, 992.Google Scholar
Okada, K., & Hickok, G. (2006). Identification of lexical-phonological networks in the superior temporal sulcus using functional magnetic resonance imaging. Neuroreport, 17, 12931296.Google Scholar
Papagno, C., Comi, A., Riva, M., Bizzi, A., Vernice, M., Casarotti, A., Fava, E., & Bello, L. (2017). Mapping the brain network of the phonological loop. Human Brain Mapping, 38, 30113024.Google Scholar
Paulesu, E., Frith, C. D., & Frackowiak, R. S. (1993). The neural correlates of the verbal component of working memory. Nature, 362, 342345.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pisoni, A., Mattavelli, G., Casarotti, A., Comi, A., Riva, M., Bello, L., & Papagno, C. (2019). The neural correlates of auditory-verbal short-term memory: A voxel-based lesion-symptom mapping study on 103 patients after glioma removal. Brain Structure and Function, 224, 21992211.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Potter, M. C., Kroll, J. F., Yachzel, B., Carpenter, E., & Sherman, J. (1986). Pictures in sentences: Understanding without words. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 115, 281.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Potter, M. C., & Lombardi, L. (1998). Syntactic priming in immediate recall of sentences. Journal of Memory and Language, 38, 265282.Google Scholar
Rapp, B., & Fischer-Baum, S. (2014). Representation of orthographic knowledge. In Goldrick, M., Ferreira, V. S., & Miozzo, M. (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of language production. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Romani, C. (1992). Are there distinct input and output buffers? Evidence from an aphasic patient with an impaired output buffer. Language and Cognitive Processes, 7, 131162.Google Scholar
Saffran, E. M., Berndt, R. S., & Schwartz, M. F. (1989). The quantitative analysis of agrammatic production: Procedure and data. Brain and Language, 37, 440479.Google Scholar
Schnur, T. T., Costa, A., & Caramazza, A. (2006). Planning at the phonological level during sentence production. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 35, 189213.Google Scholar
Schwartz, M. F., & Dell, G. S. (2010). Case series investigations in cognitive neuropsychology. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 27, 477494.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shallice, T., & Butterworth, B. (1977). Short-term memory impairment and spontaneous speech. Neuropsychologia, 15, 729735.Google Scholar
Shallice, T., Rumiati, R. I., & Zadini, A. (2000). The selective impairment of the phonological output buffer. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 17, 517546.Google Scholar
Shivde, G., & Thompson-Schill, S. L. (2004). Dissociating semantic and phonological maintenance using fMRI. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 4, 1019.Google Scholar
Smith, M., & Wheeldon, L. (1999). High level processing scope in spoken sentence production. Cognition, 73, 205246.Google Scholar
Snyder, H. R., & Munakata, Y. (2008). So many options, so little time. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15, 10831088.Google Scholar
Tan, Y., & Martin, R. C. (2018). Verbal short-term memory capacities and executive function in semantic and syntactic interference resolution during sentence comprehension: Evidence from aphasia. Neuropsychologia, 113, 111125.Google Scholar
Tan, Y., Martin, R. C., & Van Dyke, J. A. (2017). Semantic and syntactic interference in sentence comprehension: A comparison of working memory models. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 198.Google Scholar
Vallar, G. (2006). Memory systems: The case of the phonological store. A Festschrift for Cognitive Neuropsychology. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 23, 135-155.Google Scholar
Van Dyke, J. A., Johns, C. L., & Kukona, A. (2014). Low working memory capacity is only spuriously related to poor reading comprehension. Cognition, 131, 373403.Google Scholar
Van Dyke, J. A., & Lewis, R. L. (2003). Distinguishing effects of structure and decay on attachment and repair: A cue-based parsing account of recovery from misanalyzed ambiguities. Journal of Memory and Language, 49, 285316.Google Scholar
Van Dyke, J. A., & McElree, B. (2011). Cue-dependent interference in comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language, 65, 247263.Google Scholar
Varkanitsa, M., & Caplan, D. (2018). On the association between memory capacity and sentence comprehension: Insights from a systematic review and meta-analysis of the aphasia literature. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 48, 425.Google Scholar
Waters, G., Caplan, D., & Hildebrandt, N. (1991). On the structure of verbal short-term memory and its functional role in sentence comprehension: Evidence from neuropsychology. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 8, 81126.Google Scholar
Wheeldon, L. R., & Lahiri, A. (2002). The minimal unit of phonological encoding: Prosodic or lexical word. Cognition, 85, B31B41.Google Scholar
Yue, Q., & Martin, R. C. (2021). Maintaining verbal short-term memory representations in non-perceptual parietal regions. Cortex, 138, 7289.Google Scholar
Yue, Q., Martin, R. C., Hamilton, A. C., & Rose, N. S. (2019). Non-perceptual regions in the left inferior parietal lobe support phonological short-term memory: Evidence for a buffer account? Cerebral Cortex, 29, 13981413.Google Scholar
Zahn, R., Schnur, T. T., & Martin, R. C. (2019, October). Phonological retrieval mediates the relation of phonological short-term memory and narrative production. Academy of Aphasia Annual Meeting.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×