Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-dlnhk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-24T22:37:14.244Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

12 - Measuring Individual Differences in Working Memory Capacity and Attention Control and Their Contribution to Language Comprehension

from Part II - Models and Measures

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 July 2022

John W. Schwieter
Affiliation:
Wilfrid Laurier University
Zhisheng (Edward) Wen
Affiliation:
Hong Kong Shue Yan University
Get access

Summary

In this chapter, we discuss the measurement of working memory capacity and attention control. We begin by examining the origins of complex span measures of working memory capacity, which were created to better understand the cognitive processes underpinning language comprehension. We then review evidence for the executive attention theory of working memory, which places attention control at the center of individual differences in working memory capacity and fluid intelligence. Next, we describe the relationship between working memory capacity, attention control, and language comprehension, and discuss how maintenance and disengagement – two functions supported by the control of attention – contribute to performance across a range of cognitive tasks. We then identify factors that threaten the construct and criterion validity of measures of working memory capacity and attention control and detail the steps our laboratory has taken to refine these measures. We close by providing practical recommendations and resources to researchers who wish to use our new measures of working memory capacity and attention control in their work.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2022

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Ackerman, P. L., & Hambrick, D. Z. (2020). A primer on assessing intelligence in laboratory studiesIntelligence80, 101440.Google Scholar
Baddeley, A. (1992). Working memoryScience255(5044), 556559.Google Scholar
Baddeley, A. (2002). Is working memory still working? European Psychologist7, 8597.Google Scholar
Baddeley, A., & Hitch, G. (1974). Working memory. In Psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 8, pp. 4789). Academic press.Google Scholar
Blankenship, T. L., Slough, M. A., Calkins, S. D., Deater‐Deckard, K., Kim‐Spoon, J., & Bell, M. A. (2019). Attention and executive functioning in infancy: Links to childhood executive function and reading achievementDevelopmental Science22, e12824.Google Scholar
Bock, J. K., & Miller, C. A. (1991). Broken agreementCognitive Psychology23, 4593.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Broadway, J. M., & Engle, R. W. (2010). Validating running memory span: Measurement of working memory capacity and links with fluid intelligenceBehavior Research Methods42, 563570.Google Scholar
Burgoyne, A. P., & Engle, R. (2020). Attention control: A cornerstone of higher-order cognitionCurrent Directions in Psychological Science, 29(6), 624630.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burgoyne, A. P., Hambrick, D. Z., & Altmann, E. M. (2019a). Is working memory capacity a causal factor in fluid intelligence? Psychonomic Bulletin & Review26, 13331339.Google Scholar
Burgoyne, A. P., Hambrick, D. Z., & Altmann, E. M. (2019b). Placekeeping ability as a component of fluid intelligence: Not just working memory capacityThe American Journal of Psychology132, 439449.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burgoyne, A. P., Tsukahara, J. S., Draheim, C., & Engle, R. W. (2020). Differential and experimental approaches to studying intelligence in humans and nonhuman animals. Learning & Motivation, 72.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Caplan, D., & Waters, G. S. (1999). Verbal working memory and sentence comprehensionBehavioral and Brain Sciences22, 77126.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chiou, J. S., & Spreng, R. A. (1996). The reliability of difference scores: A re-examinationJournal of Consumer Satisfaction Dissatisfaction and Complaining Behavior9, 158167.Google Scholar
Conway, A. R., Cowan, N., & Bunting, M. F. (2001). The cocktail party phenomenon revisited: The importance of working memory capacityPsychonomic Bulletin & Review8, 331335.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cronbach, L. J., & Furby, L. (1970). How should we measure “change” – or should we? Psychological Bulletin, 74, 6880.Google Scholar
Crowder, R. G. (1982). The demise of short-term memoryActa Psychologica50, 291323.Google Scholar
Daneman, M., & Carpenter, P. A. (1980). Individual differences in working memory and readingJournal of Memory and Language19, 450466.Google Scholar
Daneman, M., & Green, I. (1986). Individual differences in comprehending and producing words in contextJournal of Memory and Language25, 118.Google Scholar
Daneman, M., & Merikle, P. M. (1996). Working memory and language comprehension: A meta-analysisPsychonomic Bulletin & Review3, 422433.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Delaney, P. F. (2018). The role of long-term working memory and template theory in contemporary expertise researchJournal of Expertise, 3, 155161.Google Scholar
Draheim, C., Harrison, T. L., Embretson, S. E., & Engle, R. W. (2018). What item response theory can tell us about the complex span tasks. Psychological Assessment, 30, 116129.Google Scholar
Draheim, C., Hicks, K. L., & Engle, R. W. (2016). Combining reaction time and accuracy: The relationship between working memory capacity and task-switching as a case example. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 11, 133155.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Draheim, C., Mashburn, C. A., Martin, J. D., & Engle, R. W. (2019). Reaction time in differential and development research: A review and commentary on problems and alternatives. Psychological Bulletin, 145, 508535.Google Scholar
Draheim, C., Tsukahara, J. S., Martin, J. D., Mashburn, C. A., & Engle, R. W. (2021). A toolbox approach to improving the measurement of attention controlJournal of Experimental Psychology: General, 150(2), 242-275.Google Scholar
Engle, R. W., Carullo, J. J., & Collins, K. W. (1991). Individual differences in working memory for comprehension and following directionsThe Journal of Educational Research84, 253262.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Engle, R. W., Tuholski, S. W., Laughlin, J. E., & Conway, A. R. (1999). Working memory, short-term memory, and general fluid intelligence: A latent-variable approachJournal of Experimental Psychology: General128, 309331.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Engel de Abreu, P. M. J. E., Gathercole, S. E., & Martin, R. (2011). Disentangling the relationship between working memory and language: The roles of short-term storage and cognitive controlLearning and Individual Differences21, 569574.Google Scholar
Farnham-Diggory, S., & Gregg, L. W. (1975). Short-term memory function in young readersJournal of Experimental Child Psychology19, 279298.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Foster, J. L., Shipstead, Z., Harrison, T. L., Hicks, K. L., Redick, T. S., & Engle, R. W. (2015). Shortened complex span tasks can reliably measure working memory capacityMemory & Cognition43, 226236.Google Scholar
Friedman, N. P., & Miyake, A. (2004). The relations among inhibition and interference control functions: A latent-variable analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133, 101135.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gathercole, S. E., & Baddeley, A. D. (1993). Working memory and language. Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Gernsbacher, M. A. (1990). Language comprehension as structure building. Psychology Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hale, S., Myerson, J., Rhee, S. H., Weiss, C. S., & Abrams, R. A. (1996). Selective interference with the maintenance of location information in working memoryNeuropsychology10, 228240.Google Scholar
Hambrick, D. Z., & Altmann, E. M. (2015). The role of placekeeping ability in fluid intelligencePsychonomic Bulletin & Review22, 11041110.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harrison, T. L. (2017). N-back as a measure of working memory capacity. (Ph.D. dissertation, Georgia Institute of Technology). http://hdl.handle.net/1853/58762Google Scholar
Hedge, C., Powell, G., Bompas, A., & Sumner, P. (2020, February 1). Strategy and processing speed eclipse individual differences in control ability in conflict tasks. PsyArXiv. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/vgpxqGoogle Scholar
Hedge, C., Powell, G., & Sumner, P. (2018). The reliability paradox: Why robust cognitive tasks do not produce reliable individual differences. Behavior Research Methods, 50, 11661186.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heitz, R. P., & Engle, R. W. (2007). Focusing the spotlight: Individual differences in visual attention control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 136, 217240.Google Scholar
Hunt, E, Frost, N, Lunneborg, C. (1973). Individual differences in cognition: A new approach to intelligence. In Bower, G. H., (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation: Advances in research and theory (pp. 87122). Academic PressGoogle Scholar
Jackson, M. D., & McClelland, J. L. (1979). Processing determinants of reading speedJournal of Experimental Psychology: General108, 151181.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Just, M. A., & Carpenter, P. A. (1992). A capacity theory of comprehension: Individual differences in working memoryPsychological Review99, 122149.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kane, M. J., Bleckley, M. K., Conway, A. R., & Engle, R. W. (2001). A controlled-attention view of working-memory capacityJournal of Experimental Psychology: General130, 169183.Google Scholar
Kane, M. J., & Engle, R. W. (2002). The role of prefrontal cortex in working-memory capacity, executive attention, and general fluid intelligence: An individual-differences perspectivePsychonomic Bulletin & Review9, 637671.Google Scholar
Kane, M. J., & Engle, R. W. (2003). Working-memory capacity and the control of attention: The contributions of goal neglect, response competition, and task set to Stroop interference. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 132, 4770.Google Scholar
Kintsch, W., & Van Dijk, T. A. (1978). Toward a model of text comprehension and productionPsychological Review85, 363394.Google Scholar
Kline, R. B. (2015). Principles and practice of structural equation modeling. Guilford.Google Scholar
Kramer, A. F., Humphrey, D. G., Larish, J. F., & Logan, G. D. (1994). Aging and inhibition: Beyond a unitary view of inhibitory processing in attentionPsychology and Aging9, 491512.Google Scholar
MacLeod, C. M. (1991). Half a century of research on the Stroop effect: An integrative review. Psychological Bulletin, 109, 163203CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martin, J. D., Shipstead, Z., Harrison, T. L., Redick, T. S., Bunting, M., & Engle, R. W. (2020). The role of maintenance and disengagement in predicting reading comprehension and vocabulary learningJournal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition46, 140154.Google ScholarPubMed
McVay, J. C., & Kane, M. J. (2012). Why does working memory capacity predict variation in reading comprehension? On the influence of mind wandering and executive attentionJournal of Experimental Psychology: General141, 302320.Google Scholar
Moray, N. (1959). Attention in dichotic listening: Affective cues and the influence of instructionsQuarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology11, 5660.Google Scholar
Oswald, F. L., McAbee, S. T., Redick, T. S., & Hambrick, D. Z. (2015). The development of a short domain–general measure of working memory capacityBehavior Research Methods47, 13431355.Google Scholar
Paap, K. R., & Sawi, O. (2016). The role of test-retest reliability in measuring individual and group differences in executive functioning. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 274, 8193.Google Scholar
Perfetti, C. A., & Goldman, S. R. (1976). Discourse memory and reading comprehension skillJournal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior15, 3342.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perfetti, C. A., & Lesgold, A. M. (1977). Discourse comprehension and sources of individual differences. In Just, M. A. and Carpenter, P. A. (Eds.), Cognitive processes in comprehension. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Raven, J. C., & Court, J. H. (1998). Raven’s progressive matrices and vocabulary scales. Oxford Pyschologists Press.Google Scholar
Redick, T. S., & Lindsey, D. R. (2013). Complex span and n-back measures of working memory: A meta-analysisPsychonomic Bulletin & Review20, 11021113.Google Scholar
Rey-Mermet, A., Gade, M., & Oberauer, K. (2018). Should we stop thinking about inhibition? Searching for individual and age differences in inhibition ability. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 44 (4), 501.Google Scholar
Rizzo, N. D. (1939). Studies in visual and auditory memory span with special reference to reading disabilityThe Journal of Experimental Education8, 208244.Google Scholar
Rouder, J. N., & Haaf, J. M. (2019). A psychometrics of individual differences in experimental tasks. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 26, 772789.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rouder, J., Kumar, A., & Haaf, J. M. (2019, March 25). Why most studies of individual differences with inhibition tasks are bound to fail. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/3cjr5CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schönbrodt, F. D., & Perugini, M. (2013). At what sample size do correlations stabilize? Journal of Research in Personality, 47, 609612.Google Scholar
Shah, P., & Miyake, A. (1996). The separability of working memory resources for spatial thinking and language processing: An individual differences approachJournal of Experimental Psychology: General125, 427.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shipstead, Z., Harrison, T. L., & Engle, R. W. (2016). Working memory capacity and fluid intelligence: Maintenance and disengagementPerspectives on Psychological Science11, 771799.Google Scholar
Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18, 643662.Google Scholar
Swanson, H. L., & Ashbaker, M. H. (2000). Working memory, short-term memory, speech rate, word recognition and reading comprehension in learning disabled readers: Does the executive system have a role? Intelligence28, 130.Google Scholar
Thurstone, L. L. (1938). Primary mental abilitiesPsychometric Monographs, 1, 270275.Google Scholar
Turner, M. L., & Engle, R. W. (1989). Is working memory capacity task dependent? Journal of Memory and Language28, 127154.Google Scholar
Unsworth, N., Heitz, R. P., Schrock, J. C., & Engle, R. W. (2005). An automated version of the operation span taskBehavior Research Methods37, 498505.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Unsworth, N., Redick, T. S., Heitz, R. P., Broadway, J. M., & Engle, R. W. (2009). Complex working memory span tasks and higher-order cognition: A latent-variable analysis of the relationship between processing and storageMemory17, 635654.Google Scholar
Waters, G., Caplan, D. & Hildebrandt, N. (1987). Working memory and written sentence comprehension. In Coltheart, M. (Ed.), Attention and performance XII: The psychology of reading (pp. 531555). Erlbaum.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×