Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-fbnjt Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-19T03:49:06.707Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

26 - Wisdom and Emotion

from Part VI - Wisdom and Other Psychological Constructs

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  15 March 2019

Robert J. Sternberg
Affiliation:
Cornell University, New York
Judith Glück
Affiliation:
Universität Klagenfurt, Austria
Get access

Summary

Perhaps because the struggle of reason against emotion is an appealing image, emotion has rarely been considered a defining characteristic of wisdom. In this chapter, we argue that psychological wisdom research would benefit from a systematic investigation of the dynamic between wisdom and emotion for at least three related reasons. First, emotions provide important information to the self and others and can guide our behavior in situations that are complex, difficult, and uncertain, but require immediate action. Second, in many such situations it would be irrational to ignore one’s emotions because they are pretty much the only type of information that is available. Third, many, if not all, situations that require wisdom are inherently emotional and, thus, evoke emotions in the people concerned and require knowledge and skills to manage and regulate these emotions.
Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2019

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Achenbaum, W. A., & Orwoll, L. (1991). Becoming wise: A psycho-gerontological interpretation of the Book of Job. International Journal of Aging and Human Development, 32, 2139.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Allemand, M., Steiger, A. E., & Fend, H. A. (2015). Empathy development in adolescence predicts social competencies in adulthood. Journal of Personality, 83(2), 229–41.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ardelt, M. (2004). Wisdom as expert knowledge system: A critical review of a contemporary operationalization of an ancient concept. Human Development, 47, 257–85.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ardelt, M. (2003). Empirical assessment of a three-dimensional wisdom scale. Research on Aging, 25, 275324.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baltes, P. B., & Kunzmann, U. (2004). Two faces of wisdom: Wisdom as a general theory of knowledge and judgment about excellence in mind and virtue vs. wisdom as everyday realization in people and products. Human Development, 47, 290–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baltes, P. B., & Kunzmann, U. (2003). Wisdom: The peak of human excellence in the orchestration of mind and virtue. The Psychologist, 16, 131–3.Google Scholar
Baltes, P. B., & Smith, J. (1990). The psychology of wisdom and its ontogenesis. In Sternberg, R. J. (Ed.), Wisdom: Its nature, origins, and development (pp. 87120). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baltes, P. B., & Staudinger, U. M. (2000). Wisdom: A metaheuristic (pragmatic) to orchestrate mind and virtue toward excellence. American Psychologist, 55, 122–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baltes, P. B., Staudinger, U. M., Maercker, A., & Smith, J. (1995). People nominated as wise: A comparative study of wisdom-related knowledge. Psychology and Aging, 10, 155–66.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Basseches, M. (1984). Dialectical thinking and adult development. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Brienza, J., Kung, F., Santos, H. C., Bobocel, D. R., & Grossmann, I. (2017). Wisdom, bias, and balance: Toward a process-sensitive measurement of wisdom-related cognition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, online pre-publication.Google Scholar
Carstensen, L. L., Isaacowitz, D. M., & Charles, S. T. (1999). Taking time seriously: A theory of socioemotional selectivity. American Psychologist, 54, 165–81.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clayton, V. P., & Birren, J. E. (1980). The development of wisdom across the life span: A reexamination of an ancient topic. In Baltes, P. B. & Brim, O. G., Jr. (Eds.), Life-span development and behavior (Vol. 3, pp. 103–35). New York, NY: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Dittmann-Kohli, F., & Baltes, P. B. (1990). Toward a neofunctionalist conception of adult intellectual development: Wisdom as a prototypical case of intellectual growth. In C. Alexander & E. Langer (Eds.), Higher stages of human development: Perspectives on adult growth (pp. 5478). New York, NY: Oxford University.Google Scholar
Dixon, R. A., & Baltes, P. B. (1986). Toward life-span research on the functions and pragmatics of intelligence. In Sternberg, R. J. & Wagner, R. K. (Eds.), Practical intelligence: Nature and origins of competence in the everyday world (pp. 203–35). Cambridge, UK Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Eisenberg, N., Spinrad, T. L., & Morris, A. (2014). Empathy-related responding in children. In Killen, M. & Smetana, J. G. (Eds.), Handbook of moral development, (2nd ed., pp. 184207). New York, NY: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Ekman, P. (1999). Basic emotions. In Dalgleish, T. & Power, M. J. (Eds.), Handbook of cognition and emotion (pp. 4560). New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons Ltd.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Erikson, E. H. (1959). Identity and the life cycle. New York, NY: International University Press.Google Scholar
Fleeson, W., & Furr, R. M. (2016). Do broad character traits exist? Repeated assessments of individuals, not group summaries from classic experiments, provide the relevant evidence. In Fileva, I. (Ed.), Questions of character (pp. 231–48). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Fredrickson, B. L. (2001). The role of positive emotions in positive psychology: The broaden-and build theory of positive emotions. American Psychologist, 56, 218–26.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fredrickson, B. L. (1998). What good are positive emotions? Review of General Psychology, 2, 300–19.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fredrickson, B. L., & Branigan, C. A. (2001). Positive emotions. In Mayne, T. J. & Bonnano, G. A. (Eds.), Emotion: Current issues and future developments (pp. 123–51). New York, NY: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Fredrickson, B. L., Waugh, C. E., Larkin, G. L., & Tugade, M. M. (2003). What good are positive emotions in crisis? A prospective study of resilience and emotions following the terrorist attacks on the United States on September 11th, 2001. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 365–76.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frijda, N. H., Kuipers, P., & ter Schure, E. (1989). Relations among emotion, appraisal, and emotional action readiness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 212–28.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gerstorf, D., Hoppmann, C. A., & Ram, N. (2014). The promise and challenges of integrating multiple time-scales in adult developmental inquiry. Research in Human Development, 11, 7590.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Glück, J. (2015). Wisdom, psychology of. In Wright, J. (Ed.), International encyclopedia of social and behavioral sciences (2nd ed., Vol. 25, pp. 590–7). London: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Glück, J. (2018). Measuring wisdom: existing approaches, continuing challenges, and new developments. The Journals of Gerontology: Series B, 73, 1393–403.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Glück, J. & Bluck, S. (2013). The MORE Life Experience Model: A theory of the development of personal wisdom. In Ferrari, M. & Weststrate, N. (Eds.), The scientific study of personal wisdom (pp. 7598). New York, NY: Springer.Google Scholar
Glück, J., Bluck, S., Baron, J. & McAdams, D. (2005). The wisdom of experience: Autobiographical narratives across adulthood. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 29, 197208.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Glück, J., Bluck, S., & Weststrate, N. M. (2018). More on the MORE Life Experience Model: what we have learned (so far). The Journal of Value Inquiry, online pre-publication.Google Scholar
Glück, J., König, S., Naschenweng, K., Redzanowski, U., Dorner, L., Strasser, I., & Wiedermann, W. (2013). How to measure wisdom: Content, reliability, and validity of five measures. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, article 405.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gough, H. G. & Heilbrun, A. B. (1983). The adjective check list manual. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologist's Press.Google Scholar
Gross, J. J. (2015). Emotion regulation: Current status and future prospects. Psychological Inquiry, 26, 126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grossmann, I. (2017). Wisdom in context. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 12(2), 233–57.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grossmann, I. & Kross, E. (2014). Exploring Solomon's paradox: Self-distancing eliminates the self-other asymmetry in wise reasoning about close relations in younger and older adults. Psychological Science, 25(8), 1571–80.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grossmann, I., Gerlach, T. M., & Denissen, J. J. (2016). Wise reasoning in the face of everyday life challenges. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 7(7), 611–22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grossmann, I., Kung, F. Y. H., & Santos, H. C. (this volume). Wisdom as state vs. trait. In Sternberg, R. J. & Glück, J. (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of wisdom. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Grossmann, I., Na, J., Varnum, M. E. W., Kitayama, S., & Nisbett, R. E. (2013). A route to well-being: Intelligence versus wise reasoning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 142, 944–53.Google ScholarPubMed
Grossmann, I., Na, J., Varnum, M. E. W., Park, D. C, Kitayama, S., & Nisbett, R. E. (2010). Reasoning about social conflicts improves into old age. PNAS Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 107, 7246–50.Google ScholarPubMed
Heckhausen, J., Dixon, R. A., & Baltes, P. B. (1989). Gains and losses in development throughout adulthood as perceived by different adult age groups. Developmental Psychology, 25, 109–21.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Helson, R., & Srivastava, S. (2002). Creative and wise people: Similarities, differences, and how they develop. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28, 1430–40.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Isen, A. M. (1987). Positive affect, cognitive processes, and social behavior. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 20, 203–53.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Isen, A. M. (1999). Positive affect. In Dalgleish, T. & Power, M. (Eds.), Handbook of emotion and cognition (pp. 521–39). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Izard, C. E. (1993). Four systems for emotion activation: Cognitive and noncognitive processes. Psychological Review, 100, 6890.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jones, E. E. & Harris, V. A. (1967). The attribution of attitudes. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 3(1), 124. doi: 10.1016/0022-1031(67)90034-0.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kahneman, D. (2011). Thinking, fast and slow. London: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Kahneman, D., Krueger, A. B., Schkade, D. A., Schwarz, N., & Stone, A. A. (2004). A survey method for characterizing daily life experience: The day reconstruction method. Science, 306, 1776–80.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kramer, D. A. (2000). Wisdom as a classical source of human strength: Conceptualization and empirical inquiry. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 19, 83101.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kross, E., & Grossmann, I. (2012). Boosting wisdom: Distance from the self enhances wise reasoning, attitudes, and behavior. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 141, 43–8.Google ScholarPubMed
Kunzmann, U. (2004). Approaches to a good life: The emotional-motivational side to wisdom. In Linley, P. A. & Joseph, S. (Eds.), Positive psychology in practice (pp. 504–17). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley and Sons.Google Scholar
Kunzmann, U., & Baltes, P. B. (2003a). Wisdom-related knowledge: Affective, motivational, and interpersonal correlates. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29, 1104–19.Google ScholarPubMed
Kunzmann, U., & Baltes, P. B. (2003b). Beyond the traditional scope of intelligence: Wisdom in action. In Sternberg, R. J., Lautry, J. & Lubart, T. I. (Eds.), Models of intelligence for the next millennium (pp. 329–43). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Kunzmann, U., & Grühn, D. (2005). Age differences in emotional reactivity: The sample case of sadness. Psychology and Aging, 20, 4759.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kunzmann, U., Stange, A. & Jordan, J. (2005). Positive emotionality and lifestyle in adulthood: Do you do what you feel? Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31, 574–88.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kunzmann, U. & von Salisch, M. (2009). Die Entwicklung von Emotionen und emotionalen Kompetenzen über die Lebensspanne. In Stemmler, G. (Eds.), Enzyklopädie der Psychologie: Psychologie der Emotion (pp. 528–69). Göttingen: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Kunzmann, U. & Wrosch, C. (2017). Emotional development in old age. In Pachana, N. (Ed.), Encyclopedia of geropsychology (pp. 752–62). New York, NY: Springer.Google Scholar
Labouvie-Vief, G. (2003). Dynamic integration: Affect, cognition, and the self in adulthood. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 12, 201–6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Labouvie-Vief, G. (1990). Wisdom as integrated thought: Historical and developmental perspectives. In Sternberg, R. J. (Ed.), Wisdom: Its nature, origins, and development (pp. 5283). Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Larson, R., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1983). The experience sampling method. New Directions for Methodology of Social & Behavioral Science, 15, 4156.Google Scholar
Lazarus, R. S. (1991). Emotion and adaptation. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levenson, R. W. (2000). Expressive, physiological, and subjective changes in emotion across adulthood. In Qualls, S. H. & Abeles, N. (Eds.), Psychology and the aging revolution: How we adapt to longer life (pp. 123–40). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Mickler, C. & Staudinger, U. M. (2008). Personal wisdom: Validation and age-related differences of a performance measure. Psychology and Aging, 23(4), 787–99.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nesselroade, J. R. (1984). Concepts of intraindividual variability and change: Impressions of Cattell's influence on lifespan developmental psychology. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 19, 269–86.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oakes, H., Brienza, J. P., Elnakouri, A., & Grossmann, I. (this volume). Wise reasoning: Unpacking the psychology of sound judgment. In Sternberg, R. J. & Glück, J. (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of wisdom. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Paxton, J. M., Ungar, L., & Greene, J. D. (2012). Reflection and reasoning in moral judgment. Cognitive Science, 36, 163–77.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ruhl, H., & Chow, C. M. (2016). Empathy in adolescence: Familial determinants and peer relationship outcomes. In Watt, D. F. & Panksepp, J. (Eds.), Psychology and neurobiology of empathy (pp. 171–86). Hauppauge, NY: Nova Biomedical Books.Google Scholar
Salovey, P., Kokkonen, M., Lopes, P. N., & Mayer, J. D. (2004). Emotional intelligence: What do we know? In Manstead, A. S. R., Frijda, N. H. & Fischer, A. H. (Eds.), Feelings and emotions: The Amsterdam Symposium (pp. 319–38). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Staudinger, U. M., & Baltes, P. B. (1996). Interactive minds: A facilitative setting for wisdom-related performance? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 746–62.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Staudinger, U. M., & Glück, J. (2011). Psychological wisdom research: Commonalities and differences in a growing field. Annual Review of Psychology, 62, 215–41.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Staudinger, U. M., Lopez, D. F., & Baltes, P. B. (1997). The psychometric location of wisdom-related performance: Intelligence, personality, and more? Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 23, 1200–14.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Staudinger, U. M., Smith, J., & Baltes, P. B. (1994). Manual for the assessment of wisdom-related knowledge. Berlin: Max Planck Institute for Human Development and Education.Google Scholar
Sternberg, R. J. (Ed.). (1990). Wisdom: Its nature, origins, and development. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sternberg, R. J. (1998). A balance theory of wisdom. Review of General Psychology, 2, 347–65.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sternberg, R. J. (this volume). Why people often prefer wise guys to guys who are wise: An augmented balance theory of the production and reception of wisdom. In Sternberg, R. J. & Glück, J. (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of wisdom. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thomas, S., & Kunzmann, U. (2014). Age differences in wisdom-related knowledge: Does the age-relevance of the task matter? The Journals of Gerontology, Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 69, 897905.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Voelkle, M. C., Brose, A., Schmiedek, F. & Lindenberger, U. (2014). Towards a unified framework for the study of between-person and within-person structures: Building a bridge between two research paradigms. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 49, 193213.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Webster, J. D. (2003). An exploratory analysis of a self-assessed wisdom scale. Journal of Adult Development, 10(1), 1322.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Webster, J. D. (2007). Measuring the character strength of wisdom. International Journal of Aging & Human Development, 65, 163–83.Google ScholarPubMed
Weststrate, N. M., & Glück, J. (2017). Hard-earned wisdom: Exploratory processing of difficult life experience is positively associated with wisdom. Developmental Psychology, 53, 800–14.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wieck, C., & Kunzmann, U. (2015). Age differences in empathy: Multidirectional and context-dependent. Psychology and Aging, 30, 407–19.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wieck, C. & Kunzmann, U. (2017). Age differences in emotion recognition: A question of modality? Psychology and Aging, 32, 401–11.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wink, P., & Helson, R. (1997). Practical and transcendent wisdom: Their nature and some longitudinal findings. Journal of Adult Development, 4, 115.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wink, P., & Staudinger, U. M. (2016). Wisdom and psychosocial functioning in later life. Journal of Personality, 84, 306–18.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×