Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-gvvz8 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-25T02:37:09.291Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Part III - Intentionality-Based Forms of the Imagination

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 May 2020

Anna Abraham
Affiliation:
University of Georgia
Get access

Summary

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2020

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

References

Addis, D. R. (2018). Are Episodic Memories Special? On the Sameness of Remembered and Imagined Event Simulation. Journal of the Royal Society of New Zealand, 48(2–3), 6488.Google Scholar
Arcangeli, M. (2018). Supposition and the Imaginative Realm: A Philosophical Inquiry. New York, NY: Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bernecker, S. (2008). The Metaphysics of Memory. New York, NY: Springer.Google Scholar
Bernecker, S. (2017). A Causal Theory of Mnemonic Confabulation. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1207.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bernecker, S., and Michaelian, K. (eds.) (2017). The Routledge Handbook of Philosophy of Memory. London, UK: Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Borghini, A., and Torrengo, G. (2013). The Metaphysics of the Thin Red Line. In Correia, F and Iacona, A (eds.), Around the Tree. Berlin, Germany: Springer, 105125.Google Scholar
Brewer, W. F. (1996). What is Recollective Memory? In Rubin, D. C. (ed.), Remembering Our Past: Studies in Autobiographical Memory. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, 1966.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Byrne, A., and Logue, H. (eds.) (2009). Disjunctivism: Contemporary Readings. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Coliva, A. (2006). Error Through Misidentification: Some Varieties. The Journal of Philosophy, 103(8), 403425.Google Scholar
Dalla Barba, G., Cappelletti, J. Y., Signorini, M., and Denes, G. (1997). Confabulation: Remembering “Another” Past, Planning “Another” Future. Neurocase, 3(6), 425436.Google Scholar
Debus, D. (2008). Experiencing the Past: A Relational Account of Recollective Memory. Dialectica, 62(4), 405432.Google Scholar
Debus, D. (2014). “Mental Time Travel”: Remembering the Past, Imagining the Future, and the Particularity of Events. Review of Philosophy and Psychology, 5, 333350.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
De Brigard, F. (2014a). Is Memory for Remembering? Recollection as a Form of Episodic Hypothetical Thinking. Synthese, 191(2), 155185.Google Scholar
De Brigard, F. (2014b). The Nature of Memory Traces. Philosophy Compass, 9(6), 402414.Google Scholar
Devitt, M., and Sterelny, K. (1999). Language and Reality: An Introduction to the Philosophy of Language. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Evans, J. (1982). The Varieties of Reference. Oxford, UK: Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
Fernández, J. (2014). Memory and Immunity to Error through Misidentification. Review of Philosophy and Psychology, 5(3), 373390.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fernández, J. (2017). The Intentional Objects of Memory. In Bernecker, S and Michaelian, K (eds.), The Routledge Handbook of Philosophy of Memory. London, UK: Routledge, 8899.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frege, G. (1892/1948). Sense and Reference. The Philosophical Review, 57(3), 209230.Google Scholar
Frise, M. (2015). Epistemology of Memory. In Fieser, J and Dowden, B (eds.), Internet Encyclopedia of Philosophy. www.iep.utm.edu/epis-mem/.Google Scholar
Gérardin-Laverge, L. (2017). Mémoire constructive, imagination et voyage mental dans le temps. Cahiers philosophiques, 2, 2340.Google Scholar
Goldman, A. I. (1967). A Causal Theory of Knowing. The Journal of Philosophy, 64(12), 357372.Google Scholar
Goldman, A. I. (2012). Reliabilism and Contemporary Epistemology: Essays. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hamilton, A. (2007). Memory and Self-Consciousness: Immunity to Error through Misidentification. Synthese, 171, 409417.Google Scholar
Hume, D. (1738/2011). A Treatise of Human Nature. Oxford, UK: Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
Hutto, D. D., and Peeters, A. (2018). The Roots of Remembering: Radically Enactive Recollecting. In Michaelian, K, Debus, D, and Perrin, D (eds.), New Directions in the Philosophy of Memory. London, UK: Routledge, 97118.Google Scholar
Kind, A., and Kung, P. (eds.) (2016). Knowledge Through Imagination. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Kneale, M. (1971). Our Knowledge of the Past and of the Future. Proceedings of the Aristotelian Society, 72:112.Google Scholar
Kripke, S. (1980). Naming and Necessity. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Locke, J. (1689/1975). An Essay Concerning Human Understanding. In Nidditch, P (ed.), The Clarendon Edition of the Works of John Locke: An Essay Concerning Human Understanding. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Lopes, D. (1996). Understanding Pictures. Oxford, UK: Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
Martin, C. B., and Deutscher, M. (1966). Remembering. The Philosophical Review, 75(2), 161196.Google Scholar
McDowell, J. (1997). Reductionism and the First Person. In Dancy, J (ed.), Reading Parfit. Oxford, UK: Blackwell, 230250.Google Scholar
Macpherson, F., and Dorsch, F. (eds.) (2018). Perceptual Imagination and Perceptual Memory. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Michaelian, K. (2016a). Against Discontinuism: Mental Time Travel and Our Knowledge of Past and Future Events. In Michaelian, K, Klein, S. B., and Szpunar, K. K. (eds.), Seeing the Future: Theoretical Perspectives on Future-Oriented Mental Time Travel. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press, 6992.Google Scholar
Michaelian, K. (2016b). Confabulating, Misremembering, Relearning: The Simulation Theory of Memory and Unsuccessful Remembering. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1857.Google Scholar
Michaelian, K. (2016c). Mental Time Travel: Episodic Memory and Our Knowledge of the Personal Past. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Michaelian, K. (Forthcoming). Confabulating as Unreliable Imagining: In Defence of the Simulationist Account of Unsuccessful Remembering. Topoi.Google Scholar
Michaelian, K., Klein, S. B., and Szpunar, K. K. (eds.) (2016). Seeing the Future: Theoretical Perspectives on Future-Oriented Mental Time Travel. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Michaelian, K., and Robins, S. (2018). Beyond the Causal Theory? Fifty Years after Martin and Deutscher. In Michaelian, K, Debus, D, and Perrin, D (eds.), New Directions in the Philosophy of Memory. London, UK: Routledge, 1232.Google Scholar
Nanay, B. (2015). Perceptual Content and the Content of Mental Imagery. Philosophical Studies, 172(7), 17231736.Google Scholar
Parfit, D. (1984). Reasons and Persons. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Perrin, D. (2016). Asymmetries in Subjective Time. In Michaelian, K, Klein, S. B., and Szpunar, K. K. (eds.), Seeing the Future: Theoretical Perspectives on Future-Oriented Mental Time Travel. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press, 3961.Google Scholar
Perrin, D. (2018). A Case for Procedural Causality in Episodic Recollection. In Michaelian, K, Debus, D, and Perrin, D (eds.), New Directions in the Philosophy of Memory. London, UK: Routledge, 3351.Google Scholar
Perrin, D., and Michaelian, K. (2017). Memory as Mental Time Travel. In Bernecker, S and Michaelian, K (eds.), The Routledge Handbook of Philosophy of Memory. London, UK: Routledge, 228239.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Recanati, F. (2007). Perspectival Thought: A Plea for (Moderate) Relativism. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Reid, T. (1764/2000). An Inquiry into the Human Mind on the Principles of Common Sense. Pennsylvania, PA: Pennsylvania State University Press.Google Scholar
Robins, S. K. (2016). Misremembering. Philosophical Psychology, 29(3), 432447.Google Scholar
Robins, S. K. (2018). Mnemonic Confabulation. Topoi. doi.org/10.1007/s11245-018-9613-x.Google Scholar
Robins, S. K. (2019). Confabulation and Constructive Memory. Synthese, 196(6), 21352151.Google Scholar
Russell, B. (1910). Knowledge by Acquaintance and Knowledge by Description. Proceedings of the Aristotelian Society, 11, 108128.Google Scholar
Russell, B. (1921/2005). Analysis of Mind. London, UK: Routledge.Google Scholar
Sant’Anna, A. (2018a). Mental Time Travel and the Philosophy of Memory. Unisinos Journal of Philosophy, 19(1), 5262.Google Scholar
Sant’Anna, A. (2018b). The Hybrid Contents of Memory. Synthese, March. doi:10.1007/s11229-018-1753-4.Google Scholar
Sant’Anna, A., and Michaelian, K. (2019). Thinking about Events: A Pragmatic Account of the Objects of Episodic Hypothetical Thought. Review of Philosophy and Psychology, 10(1), 187217.Google Scholar
Schellenberg, S. (2010). The Particularity and Phenomenology of Perceptual Experience. Philosophical Studies, 149(1), 1948.Google Scholar
Schnider, A. (2018). The Confabulating Mind: How the Brain Creates Reality. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Shoemaker, S. (1970). Persons and Their Pasts. American Philosophical Quarterly, 7(4), 269285.Google Scholar
Suddendorf, T., and Corballis, M. C. (2007). The Evolution of Foresight: What Is Mental Time Travel, and Is It Unique to Humans? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 30(3), 299313.Google Scholar
Travis, C. (2004). The Silence of the Senses. Mind, 113(449), 5794.Google Scholar
Tulving, E. (1972). Episodic and Semantic Memory. In Tulving, E and Donaldson, W (eds.), Organization of Memory. New York NY: Academic Press, 381402.Google Scholar
Tulving, E. (1985). Elements of Episodic Memory. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Werning, M., and Cheng, S. (2017). Taxonomy and Unity of Memory. In Bernecker, S and Michaelian, K (eds.), The Routledge Handbook of Philosophy of Memory. London, UK: Routledge, 720.Google Scholar
Zahavi, D. (2003). Husserl’s Phenomenology. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar

References

Addis, D. R., Wong, A. T., and Schacter, D. L. (2007). Remembering the Past and Imagining the Future: Common and Distinct Neural Substrates During Event Construction and Elaboration. Neuropsychologia, 45(7), 13631377.Google Scholar
Anelli, F., Avanzi, S., Arzy, S., Mancuso, M., and Frassinetti, F. (2018a). Effects of Spatial Attention on Mental Time Travel in Patients with Neglect. CORTEX, 101, 192205.Google Scholar
Anelli, F., Ciaramelli, E., Arzy, S., and Frassinetti, F. (2016a). Prisms to Travel in Time: Investigation of Time-Space Association through Prismatic Adaptation Effect on Mental Time Travel. Cognition, 156, 15.Google Scholar
Anelli, F., Ciaramelli, E., Arzy, S., and Frassinetti, F. (2016b). Age-Related Effects on Future Mental Time Travel, Neural Plasticity, 1(8), 125.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anelli, F., Peters-Founshtein, G., Shreibman, Y., et al. (2018b). Nature and Nurture Effects on the Spatiality of the Mental Time Line. Scientific Reports, 8(1), 11710.Google Scholar
Arzy, S., Adi-Japha, E., and Blanke, O. (2009a). The Mental Time Line: An Analogue of the Mental Number Line in the Mapping of Life Events. Consciousness and Cognition, 18(3), 781785.Google Scholar
Arzy, S., Bick, A., and Blanke, O. (2009b). Mental Time in Amnesia: Evidence from Bilateral Medial Temporal Damage Before and After Recovery. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 26(6), 503510.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Arzy, S., Collette, S., Ionta, S., Fornari, E., and Blanke, O. (2009c). Subjective Mental Time: The Functional Architecture of Projecting the Self to Past and Future. The European Journal of Neuroscience, 30(10), 20092017.Google Scholar
Arzy, S., Molnar-Szakacs, I., and Blanke, O. (2008). Self in Time: Imagined Self-Location Influences Neural Activity Related to Mental Time Travel. Journal of Neuroscience, 28(25), 65026507.Google Scholar
Arzy, S., and Schacter, D. L. (2019). Self-Agency and Self-Ownership in Cognitive Mapping. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 23(6), 476487.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Atkinson, R. C., and Shiffrin, R. M. (1968). Human Memory: A Proposed System and its Control Processes. In Spence, K. W. and Spence, J. T. (eds.), Psychology of Learning and Motivation: Advances in Research and Theory. Volume 2. New York, NY: Academic Press, 89195.Google Scholar
Banino, A., Barry, C., Uria, B., et al. (2018). Vector-Based Navigation Using Grid-Like Representations in Artificial Agents. Nature, 557(7705), 429433.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Banks, W. P., and Hill, D. K. (1974). The Apparent Magnitude of Number Scaled by Random Production. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 102(2), 353376.Google Scholar
Bartsch, T., Dohring, J., Rohr, A., Jansen, O., and Deuschl, G. (2011). CA1 Neurons in the Human Hippocampus Are Critical for Autobiographical Memory, Mental Time Travel, and Autonoetic Consciousness. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(42), 1756217567.Google Scholar
Behrens, T. E. J., Muller, T. H., Whittington, J. C. R., et al. (2018). What is a Cognitive Map? Organizing Knowledge for Flexible Behavior. Neuron, 100(2), 490509.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ben-Yakov, A., Honey, C. J., Lerner, Y., and Hasson, U. (2012). Loss of Reliable Temporal Structure in Event-Related Averaging of Naturalistic Stimuli. NeuroImage, 63(1), 501506.Google Scholar
Benoit, R. G., and Schacter, D. L. (2015). Specifying the Core Network Supporting Episodic Simulation and Episodic Memory by Activation Likelihood Estimation. Neuropsychologia, 75, 450457.Google Scholar
Berrios, G. E. (1982). Disorientation States and Psychiatry. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 23(5), 479491.Google Scholar
Buckner, R. L., Andrews-Hanna, J. R., and Schacter, D. L. (2008). The Brain’s Default Network: Anatomy, Function, and Relevance to Disease. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1124, 138.Google Scholar
Buckner, R. L., and Carroll, D. C. (2007). Self-Projection and the Brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(2), 4957.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buckner, R. L., and DiNicola, L. M. (2019). The Brain’s Default Network: Updated Anatomy, Physiology and Evolving Insights. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 20(10), 593608.Google Scholar
Cabeza, R., and St Jacques, P. (2007). Functional Neuroimaging of Autobiographical Memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(5), 219227.Google Scholar
Constantinescu, A. O., O’Reilly, J. X., and Behrens, T. E. (2016). Organizing Conceptual Knowledge in Humans with a Gridlike Code. Science, 1(6292), 19.Google Scholar
Coughlan, G., Laczó, J., Hort, J., Minihane, A. M., and Hornberger, M. (2018). Spatial Navigation Deficits – Overlooked Cognitive Marker for Preclinical Alzheimer Disease? Nature Reviews Neurology, 14(8), 111.Google Scholar
Craik, K. J. W. (1943). The nature of explanation. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK.Google Scholar
Dafni-Merom, A., Peters-Founshtein, G., Kahana-Merhavi, S., & Arzy, S. (2019). A Unified Brain System of Orientation and Its Disruption in Alzheimer’s Disease. Annals of Clinical and Translational Neurology, 6(12), 24682478.Google Scholar
Danjo, T., Toyoizumi, T., and Fujisawa, S. (2018). Spatial Representations of Self and Other in the Hippocampus. Science, 218(January), 213218.Google Scholar
Davelaar, E. J., Goshen-Gottstein, Y., Ashkenazi, A., Haarmann, H. J., and Usher, M. (2005). The Demise of Short-Term Memory Revisited: Empirical and Computational Investigations of Recency Effects. Psychological Review, 112(1), 342.Google Scholar
Dehaene, S., Bossini, S., and Giraux, P. (1993). The Mental Representation of Parity and Number Magnitude. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 122(3), 371396.Google Scholar
Dehaene, S., and Cohen, L. (1995). Towards an Anatomical and Functional Model of Number Processing. Mathematical Cognition, 1(1), 83120.Google Scholar
Dehaene, S., Spelke, E., Izard, V., and Pica, P. (2008). Log or Linear? Distinct Intuitions of the Number Scale in Western and Amazonian Indigene Cultures. Science, 1217(May), 12171220.Google Scholar
Dehaene, S., Spelke, E., Pinel, P., Stanescu, R., and Tsivkin, S. (1999). Sources of Mathematical Thinking. Science, 284(May), 970974.Google Scholar
DeVito, L. M., and Eichenbaum, H. (2011). Memory for the Order of Events in Specific Sequences: Contributions of the Hippocampus and Medial Prefrontal Cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 31(9), 31693175.Google Scholar
Doeller, C. F., Barry, C., and Burgess, N. (2010). Evidence for Grid Cells in a Human Memory Network. Nature, 463(7281), 657661.Google Scholar
Eichenbaum, H. (2014). Time Cells in the Hippocampus: A New Dimension for Mapping Memories. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 15(11), 732744.Google Scholar
Epstein, R. A., Parker, W. E., and Feiler, A. M. (2007). Where Am I Now? Distinct Roles for Parahippocampal and Retrosplenial Cortices in Place Recognition. Journal of Neuroscience, 27(23), 61416149.Google Scholar
Epstein, R. A., Patai, E. Z., Julian, J. B., and Spiers, H. J. (2017). The Cognitive Map in Humans: Spatial Navigation and Beyond. Nature Neuroscience, 20(11), 15041513.Google Scholar
Folkerts, S., Rutishauser, U., and Howard, M. W. (2018). Human Episodic Memory Retrieval Is Accompanied by a Neural Contiguity Effect. Journal of Neuroscience, 38(17), 42004211.Google Scholar
Folstein, M., Folstein, S., and McHugh, P. (1975). “Mini-Mental State”: A Practical Method for Grading the Cognitive State of Patients for the Clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 12(3), 189198.Google Scholar
Frith, U., and Frith, C. D. (2003). Development and Neurophysiology of Mentalizing. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 358(1431), 459473.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fuhrman, O., and Boroditsky, L. (2010). Cross-Cultural Differences in Mental Representations of Time: Evidence From an Implicit Nonlinguistic Task. Cognitive Science, 34(8), 14301451.Google Scholar
Garvert, M. M., Dolan, R. J., and Behrens, T. E. J. (2017). A Map of Abstract Relational Knowledge in the Human Hippocampal–Entorhinal Cortex. Elife, 6, 120.Google Scholar
Gauthier, B., and van Wassenhove, V. (2016). Cognitive Mapping in Mental Time Travel and Mental Space Navigation. Cognition, 154, 5568.Google Scholar
Gauthier, J. L., and Tank, D. W. (2018). A Dedicated Population for Reward Coding in the Hippocampus. Neuron, 99(1), 179193.e7.Google Scholar
Gershman, S. J., and Daw, N. D. (2017). Reinforcement Learning and Episodic Memory in Humans and Animals: An Integrative Framework. Annual Review of Psychology, 68, 101128.Google Scholar
Göbel, S. M., Shaki, S., and Fischer, M. H. (2011). The Cultural Number Line: A Review of Cultural and Linguistic Influences on the Development of Number Processing. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 42(4), 543565.Google Scholar
Hafting, T., Fyhn, M., Molden, S., Moser, M. B., and Moser, E. I. (2005). Microstructure of a Spatial Map in the Entorhinal Cortex. Nature, 436(7052), 801806.Google Scholar
Hassabis, D., and Maguire, E. A. (2007). Deconstructing Episodic Memory with Construction. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(7), 299306.Google Scholar
Holmes, K. J., and Lourenco, S. F. (2011). Common Spatial Organization of Number and Emotional Expression: A Mental Magnitude Line. Brain and Cognition, 77(2), 315323.Google Scholar
Howard, M. W. (2014). Mathematical Learning Theory through Time. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 59(1), 1829.Google Scholar
Høydal, Ø. A., Skytøen, E. R., Moser, M., and Moser, E. I. (2018). Object-Vector Coding in the Medial Entorhinal Cortex. Nature, 568, 400404.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Huijbers, W., Pennartz, C. M. A., Cabeza, R., and Daselaar, S. M. (2011). The Hippocampus Is Coupled with the Default Network during Memory Retrieval but Not during Memory Encoding. PloS ONE, 6(4).Google Scholar
Ishihara, M., Keller, P. E., Rossetti, Y., and Prinz, W. (2008). Horizontal Spatial Representations of Time: Evidence for the STEARC Effect. Cortex, 44, 454461.Google Scholar
Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1980). Mental Models in Cognitive Science. Cognitive Science, 4(1), 71115.Google Scholar
Julian, J. B., Keinath, A. T., Frazzetta, G., and Epstein, R. A. (2018). Human Entorhinal Cortex Represents Visual Space using a Boundary-Anchored Grid. Nature Neuroscience, 21(2), 191194.Google Scholar
Kahana, M. J. (1996). Associative Retrieval Processes in Free Recall. Memory & Cognition, 24(1), 103109.Google Scholar
Kraus, B. J., Brandon, M. P., Robinson, R. J., et al. (2015). During Running in Place, Grid Cells Integrate Elapsed Time and Distance Run. Neuron, 88(3), 578589.Google Scholar
Krupic, J., Bauza, M., Burton, S., Barry, C., and O’Keefe, J. (2015). Grid Cell Symmetry Is Shaped by Environmental Geometry. Nature, 518(7538), 232235.Google Scholar
Krupic, J., Bauza, M., Burton, S., and O’Keefe, J. (2018). Local Transformations of the Hippocampal Cognitive Map. Science, 359(6380), 11431146.Google Scholar
Kunz, L., Schroder, T. N., Lee, H., et al. (2015). Reduced Grid-Cell-Like Representations in Adults at Genetic Risk for Alzheimer’s Disease. Science, 350(6259), 430433.Google Scholar
Lever, C., Burton, S., Jeewajee, A., O’Keefe, J., and Burgess, N. (2009). Boundary Vector Cells in the Subiculum of the Hippocampal Formation. Journal of Neuroscience, 29(31), 97719777.Google Scholar
Liberman, N., and Trope, Y. (2008). The Psychology of Transcending the Here and Now. Science, 322(5905), 12011205.Google Scholar
MacDonald, C. J., Lepage, K. Q., Eden, U. T., and Eichenbaum, H. (2011). Hippocampal “Time Cells” Bridge the Gap in Memory for Discontiguous Events. Neuron, 71(4), 737749.Google Scholar
Maguire, E. A., Burgess, N., Donnett, J. G., et al. (1998). Knowing Where and Getting There: A Human Navigation Network. Science, 280(May), 921924.Google Scholar
Mason, M., Magee, J. C., and Fiske, S. T. (2014). Neural Substrates of Social Status Inference: Roles of Medial Prefrontal Cortex and Superior Temporal Sulcus. Journal Of Cognitive Neuroscience, 26(5), 11311140.Google Scholar
Murdock, B. B., Jr. (1962). The Serial Position Effect of Free Recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 64, 482488.Google Scholar
Murdock, В. B., and Metcalfe, J. (1978). Controlled Rehearsal in Single-Trial Free Recall. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 17, 309324.Google Scholar
Murdock, B. B., and Okada, R. (1970). Interresponse Times in Single-Trial Free Recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 86, 263267.Google Scholar
Nau, M., Navarro Schröder, T., Bellmund, J. L. S., and Doeller, C. F. (2018). Hexadirectional Coding of Visual Space in Human Entorhinal Cortex. Nature Neuroscience, 21(2), 188190.Google Scholar
O’Keefe, J., and Nadel, L. (1978). The Hippocampus as a Cognitive Map. Oxford, UK: Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
Omer, D. B., Maimon, S. R., Las, L., and Ulanovsky, N. (2018). Social Place-Cells in the Bat Hippocampus. Science, 224(January), 218224.Google Scholar
Parkinson, C., Kleinbaum, A. M., and Wheatley, T. (2017). Spontaneous Neural Encoding of Social Network Position. Nature Human Behaviour, 1(5), 17.Google Scholar
Peer, M., Lyon, R., and Arzy, S. (2014). Orientation and Disorientation: Lessons from Patients with Epilepsy. Epilepsy and Behavior, 41, 149157.Google Scholar
Peer, M., Prüss, H., Ben-Dayan, I., et al. (2017). Functional Connectivity of Large-Scale Brain Networks in Patients with Anti-NMDA Receptor Encephalitis: An Observational Study. Lancet Psychiatry, 4(10), 768774.Google Scholar
Peer, M., Salomon, R., Goldberg, I., Blanke, O., and Arzy, S. (2015). Brain System for Mental Orientation in Space, Time, and Person. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 112(35), 1107211077.Google Scholar
Peters-Founshtein, G., Peer, M., Rein, Y., et al. (2018). Mental-Orientation: A New Approach to Assessing Patients Across the Alzheimer’s Disease Spectrum. Neuropsychology, 32(6), 690699.Google Scholar
Raaijmakers, J. G., and Shiffrin, R. M. (1980). SAM: A Theory of Probabilistic Search of Associative Memory. In Bower, G (ed.), The Psychology of Learning and Motivation. Volume 14. New York, NY: Academic Press, 207262.Google Scholar
Raichle, M. E., Macleod, A. M., Snyder, A. Z., et al. (2001). A Default Mode of Brain Function. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 98(2), 676682.Google Scholar
Rapoport, B. I., and Rapoport, S. (2015). Orientation to Person, Orientation to Self. Neurology, 85(23), 20722074.Google Scholar
Rappaport, H., Enrich, K., and Wilson, A. (1985). Relation between Ego Identity and Temporal Perspective. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48(6), 16091620.Google Scholar
Rugani, R., Vallortigara, G., Priftis, K., and Regolin, L. (2015). Number-Space Mapping in the Newborn Chick Resembles Humans’ Mental Number Line. Science, 347(6221), 534537.Google Scholar
Santiago, J., Lupáñez, J., Pérez, E., and Funes, M. J. (2007). Time (Also) Flies from Left to Right. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14(3), 512516.Google Scholar
Sarel, A., Finkelstein, A., Las, L., and Ulanovsky, N. (2017). Vectorial Representation of Spatial Goals in the Hippocampus of Bats. Science, 355(6321), 176180.Google Scholar
Saxe, R., and Kanwisher, N. G. (2003). People Thinking about Thinking People: The Role of the Temporo-Parietal Junction in “Theory of Mind”. NeuroImage, 19, 18351842.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schacter, D. L., Addis, D. R., and Buckner, R. L. (2007). Remembering the Past to Imagine the Future: The Prospective Brain. Nature Neuroscience, 8(9), 657661.Google Scholar
Schacter, D. L., Addis, D. R., Hassabis, D., et al. (2012). The Future of Memory: Remembering, Imagining, and the Brain. Neuron, 76(4), 677694.Google Scholar
Schmajuk, N. A., and Buhusi, C. V. (1997). Spatial and Temporal Cognitive Mapping: A Neural Network Approach. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 1(3), 109114.Google Scholar
Schurr, R., Nitzan, M., Eliahou, R., et al. (2018). Temporal Dissociation of Neocortical and Hippocampal Contributions to Mental Time Travel Using Intracranial Recordings in Humans. Frontiers in Computational Neuroscience, 12(February), 112.Google Scholar
Shaki, S., and Fischer, M. H. (2014). Random Walks on the Mental Number Line. Experimental Brain Research, 232(1), 4349.Google Scholar
Siegler, R. S., and Booth, J. L. (2004). Development of Numerical Estimation in Young Children. Child Development, 75(2), 428444.Google Scholar
Simons, J. S., Garrison, J. R., and Johnson, M. K. (2017). Brain Mechanisms of Reality Monitoring. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 21(6), 462473.Google Scholar
Simony, E., Honey, C. J., Chen, J., et al. (2016). Dynamical Reconfiguration of the Default Mode Network during Narrative Comprehension. Nature Communications, 7, 12141. doi:10.1038/ncomms12141.Google Scholar
Sirotin, Y. B., Kimball, D. R., and Kahana, M. J. (2005). Going Beyond a Single List: Modeling the Effects of Prior Experience on Episodic Free Recall. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 12(5), 787805.Google Scholar
Solstad, T., Boccara, C. N., Kropff, E., Moser, M., and Moser, E. I. (2008). Representation of Geometric Borders in the Entorhinal Cortex. Science, 322(5909), 18651868.Google Scholar
Spreng, R. N., and Grady, C. L. (2009). Patterns of Brain Activity Supporting Autobiographical Memory, Prospection, and Theory of Mind, and Their Relationship to the Default Mode Network. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22(6), 11121123.Google Scholar
St. Jacques, P. L., Carpenter, A. C., Szpunar, K. K., and Schacter, D. L. (2018). Remembering and Imagining Alternative Versions of the Personal Past. Neuropsychologia, 110, 170179.Google Scholar
St. Jacques, P., Rubin, D. C., LaBar, K. S., and Cabeza, R. (2008). The Short and Long of it: Neural Correlates of Temporal-Order Memory for Autobiographical Events. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20(7), 13271341.Google Scholar
Strange, B. A., Witter, M. P., Lein, E.S., and Moser, E. I. (2014). Functional Organization of the Hippocampal Longitudinal Axis. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 15(10), 655669.Google Scholar
Szpunar, K. K., Watson, J. M., and McDermott, K. B. (2007). Neural Substrates of Envisioning the Future. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104(2), 642647.Google Scholar
Taube, J. S., Muller, R. U., and Ranck, J. B. (1990). Head-Direction Cells Recorded from the Postsubiculum in Freely Moving Rats. I. Description and Quantitative Analysis. Journal of Neuroscience, 10(2), 420435.Google Scholar
Tavares, R. M., Mendelsohn, A., Grossman, Y., et al. (2015). A Map for Social Navigation in the Human Brain. Neuron, 87(1), 231243.Google Scholar
Tolman, E. C. (1948). Cognitive Maps in Rats and Men. The Psychological Review, 55(4), 189208.Google Scholar
Torralbo, A., Santiago, J., and Lupiáñez, J. (2006). Flexible Conceptual Projection of Time onto Spatial Frames of Reference. Cognitive Science, 30(4), 745757.Google Scholar
Tversky, B.(1993). Cognitive Maps, Cognitive Collages, and Spatial Mental Models. In European Conference on Spatial Information Theory (pp. 1424). Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg.Google Scholar
Uddin, L. Q., Iacoboni, M., Lange, C., and Keenan, J. P. (2007). The Self and Social Cognition: The Role of Cortical Midline Structures and Mirror Neurons. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(4), 153157.Google Scholar
Vallesi, A., Binns, M. A., and Shallice, T. (2008). An Effect of Spatial-Temporal Association of Response Codes: Understanding the Cognitive Representations of Time. Cognition, 107(2), 501527.Google Scholar
Wallace, D. G., and Whishaw, I. Q. (2003). NMDA Lesions of Ammon’s Horn and the Dentate Gyrus Disrupt the Direct and Temporally Paced Homing Displayed by Rats Exploring a Novel Environment: Evidence for a Role of the Hippocampus in Dead Reckoning. The European Journal of Neuroscience, 18(3), 513523.Google Scholar
Wilson, R. C., Takahashi, Y. K., Schoenbaum, G., and Niv, Y. (2014). Orbitofrontal Cortex as a Cognitive Map of Task Space. Neuron, 81(2), 267278.Google Scholar

References

Abraham, A. (2013). The World According to Me: Personal Relevance and the Medial Prefrontal Cortex. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 341.Google Scholar
Amit, E., Algom, D., Trope, Y., and Liberman, N. (2009). “Thou Shalt not Make Unto Thee Any Graven Image”: The Distance-Dependence of Representation. In Markman, K. D., Klein, W. M., and Suhr, J. A. (eds.), Handbook of Imagination and Mental Simulation. New York, NY: Psychology Press, 5368.Google Scholar
Amit, E., Hoeflin, C., Hamzah, N., and Fedorenko, E. (2017). An Asymmetrical Relationship between Verbal and Visual Thinking: Converging Evidence from Behavior and fMRI. NeuroImage, 152, 619627.Google Scholar
Andrews-Hanna, J. R. (2012). The Brain’s Default Network and Its Adaptive Role in Internal Mentation. The Neuroscientist, 18(3), 251270.Google Scholar
Andrews-Hanna, J. R., Christoff, K., and O’Connor, M. F. (in press). Dynamic Regulation of Internal Experience. In Lane, R, Ryan, L, and Nadel, L (eds.), The Neuroscience of Enduring Change: The Neural Basis of Talk Therapies. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Andrews-Hanna, J. R., Kaiser, R. H., Turner, A. E., et al. (2013). A Penny for Your Thoughts: Dimensions of Self-Generated Thought Content and Relationships with Individual Differences in Emotional Wellbeing. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 900.Google Scholar
Andrews-Hanna, J. R., Reidler, J. S., Sepulcre, J., Poulin, R., and Buckner, R. L. (2010). Functional-Anatomic Fractionation of the Brain’s Default Network. Neuron, 65(4), 550562.Google Scholar
Andrews-Hanna, J. R., Saxe, R., and Yarkoni, T. (2014). Contributions of Episodic Retrieval and Mentalizing to Autobiographical Thought: Evidence from Functional Neuroimaging, Resting-State Connectivity, and fMRI Meta-Analyses. Neuroimage, 91, 324335.Google Scholar
Andrews-Hanna, J. R., Smallwood, J., and Spreng, R. N. (2014). The Default Network and Self‐Generated Thought: Component Processes, Dynamic Control, and Clinical Relevance. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1316(1), 2952.Google Scholar
Baetens, K., Ma, N., Steen, J., and van Overwalle, F. (2014). Involvement of the Mentalizing Network in Social and Non-Social High Construal. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 9, 817824.Google Scholar
Baird, B., Smallwood, J., and Schooler, J. W. (2011). Back to the Future: Autobiographical Planning and the Functionality of Mind-Wandering. Consciousness and Cognition, 20(4), 16041611.Google Scholar
Bar., M., Aminoff, E., Mason, M., and Fenske, M. (2007). The Units of Thought. Hippocampus, 17(6), 420428.Google Scholar
Binder, J. R., and Desai, R. H. (2011). The Neurobiology of Semantic Memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15(11), 527536.Google Scholar
Binder, J. R., Desai, R. H., Graves, W. W., and Conant, L. L. (2009). Where is the Semantic System? A Critical Review and Meta-Analysis of 120 Functional Neuroimaging Studies. Cerebral Cortex, 19(12), 27672796.Google Scholar
Braga, R. M., and Buckner, R. L. (2017). Parallel Interdigitated Distributed Networks within the Individual Estimated by Intrinsic Functional Connectivity. Neuron, 95(2), 457–471.e5.Google Scholar
Braga, R. M., van Dijk, K. R., Polimeni, J. R., Eldaief, M. C., and Buckner, R. L. (2018). Parallel Distributed Networks Resolved at High Resolution Reveal Close Juxtaposition of Distinct Regions. Journal of Neurophysiology, 121(4), 15131534.Google Scholar
Brown, T. I., Rissman, J., Chow, T. E., Uncapher, M. R., and Wagner, A. D. (2018). Differential Medial Temporal Lobe and Parietal Cortical Contributions to Real-world Autobiographical Episodic and Autobiographical Semantic Memory. Scientific Reports, 8(1), 6190.Google Scholar
Buckner, R. L., and Carroll, D. C. (2007). Self-Projection and the Brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(2), 4957.Google Scholar
Buckner, R. L., Andrews-Hanna, J. R., and Schacter, D. L. (2008). The Brain’s Default Network: Anatomy, Function, and Relevance to Disease. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1124(1), 138.Google Scholar
Cabeza, R., and St. Jacques, P. (2007). Functional Neuroimaging of Autobiographical Memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(5), 219227.Google Scholar
Caspers, S., Zilles, K., Laird, A. R., and Eickhoff, S. B. (2010). ALE Meta-Analysis of Action Observation and Imitation in the Human Brain. Neuroimage, 50(3), 11481167.Google Scholar
Christoff, K., Irving, Z. C., Fox, K. C., Spreng, R. N., and Andrews-Hanna, J. R. (2016). Mind-Wandering as Spontaneous Thought: A Dynamic Framework. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 17(11), 718.Google Scholar
Conway, M. A., and Pleydell-Pearce, C. W. (2000). The Construction of Autobiographical Memories in the Self-Memory System. Psychological Review, 107(2), 261.Google Scholar
Cui, X., Jeter, C. B., Yang, D., Montague, P. R., and Eagleman, D. M. (2007). Vividness of Mental Imagery: Individual Variability Can Be Measured Objectively. Vision Research, 47(4), 474478.Google Scholar
D’Argembeau, A. (2013). On the Role of the Ventromedial Prefrontal Cortex in Self-Processing: The Valuation Hypothesis. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 372.Google Scholar
D’Argembeau, A., Cassol, H., Phillips, C., et al. (2013). Brains Creating Stories of Selves: The Neural Basis of Autobiographical Reasoning. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 9(5), 646652.Google Scholar
D’Argembeau, A., Lardi, C., and van der Linden, M. (2012). Self-Defining Future Projections: Exploring the Identity Function of Thinking about the Future. Memory, 20(2), 110120.Google Scholar
D’Argembeau, A., and van der Linden, M. (2006). Individual Differences in the Phenomenology of Mental Time Travel: The Effect of Vivid Visual Imagery and Emotion Regulation Strategies. Consciousness and Cognition, 15(2), 342350.Google Scholar
Delamillieure, P., Doucet, G., Mazoyer, B., et al. (2010). The Resting State Questionnaire: An Introspective Questionnaire for Evaluation of Inner Experience during the Conscious Resting State. Brain Research Bulletin, 81(6), 565573.Google Scholar
Denny, B. T., Kober, H., Wager, T. D., and Ochsner, K. N. (2012). A Meta-Analysis of Functional Neuroimaging Studies of Self- and Other Judgments Reveals a Spatial Gradient for Mentalizing in Medial Prefrontal Cortex. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 24(8), 17421752.Google Scholar
Ellamil, M., Dobson, C., Beeman, M., and Christoff, K. (2012). Evaluative and Generative Modes of Thought during the Creative Process. Neuroimage, 59(2), 17831794.Google Scholar
Ernst, A., and D’Argembeau, A. (2017). Make it Real: Belief in Occurrence within Episodic Future Thought. Memory and Cognition, 45(6), 10451061.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ferstl, E. C., and von Cramon, D. Y. (2002). What Does the Frontomedian Cortex Contribute to Language Processing: Coherence or Theory of Mind? Neuroimage, 17(3), 15991612.Google Scholar
Fox, K. C., Nijeboer, S., Solomonova, E., Domhoff, G. W., and Christoff, K. (2013). Dreaming as Mind Wandering: Evidence from Functional Neuroimaging and First-Person Content Reports. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 412.Google Scholar
Fox, K. C., Spreng, R. N., Ellamil, M., Andrews-Hanna, J. R., and Christoff, K. (2015). The Wandering Brain: Meta-Analysis of Functional Neuroimaging Studies of Mind-Wandering and Related Spontaneous Thought Processes. Neuroimage, 111, 611621.Google Scholar
Frith, C. D., and Frith, U. (2006). The Neural Basis of Mentalizing. Neuron, 50(4), 531534.Google Scholar
Grandin, T. (2006). Thinking in Pictures: And Other Reports from my Life with Autism. London, UK: Vintage.Google Scholar
Grilli, M. D., and Ryan, L. (Forthcoming). Autobiographical Memory and the Self-Concept. In Lane, R, Ryan, L, and Nadel, L (eds.), The Neuroscience of Enduring Change: The Neural Basis of Talk Therapies. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Hare, B. (2017). Survival of the Friendliest: Homo Sapiens Evolved via Selection for Prosociality. Annual Review of Psychology, 68, 155186.Google Scholar
Hassabis, D., Kumaran, D., and Maguire, E.A. (2007). Using Imagination to Understand the Neural Basis of Episodic Memory. Journal of Neuroscience, 27(52), 1436514374.Google Scholar
Hassabis, D., Kumaran, D., Vann, S. D., and Maguire, E. A. (2007). Patients with Hippocampal Amnesia Cannot Imagine New Experiences. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104(5), 17261731.Google Scholar
Hassabis, D., and Maguire, E. A. (2007). Deconstructing Episodic Memory with Construction. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11, 299306.Google Scholar
Holt-Lunstad, J., Smith, T. B., and Layton, J. B. (2010). Social Relationships and Mortality Risk: A Meta-Analytic Review. PloS Medicine, 7(7), e1000316.Google Scholar
Huth, A. G., de Heer, W. A., Griffiths, T. L., Theunissen, F. E., and Gallant, J. L. (2016). Natural Speech Reveals the Semantic Maps that Tile Human Cerebral Cortex. Nature, 532(7600), 453.Google Scholar
Irish, M., and Piolino, P. (2016). Impaired Capacity for Prospection in the Dementias – Theoretical and Clinical Implications. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 55(1), 4968.Google Scholar
Keogh, R., and Pearson, J. (2018). The Blind Mind: No Sensory Visual Imagery in Aphantasia. Cortex, 105, 5360.Google Scholar
Kim, H. (2010). Dissociating the Roles of the Default-Mode, Dorsal and Ventral Networks in Episodic Memory Retrieval. Neuroimage, 50(4), 16481657.Google Scholar
Klinger, E., (2009). Daydreaming and Fantasizing: Thought Flow and Motivation. In Markman, K. D., Klein, W. M. P., and Suhr, J. A. (eds.), Handbook of Imagination and Mental Simulation. New York, NY: Psychology Press, 225240.Google Scholar
Klinger, E., and Cox, W. M. (1987). Dimensions of Thought Flow in Everyday Life. Imagination, Cognition and Personality, 7(2), 105128.Google Scholar
Konishi, M., McLaren, D. G., Engen, H., and Smallwood, J. (2015). Shaped by the Past: The Default Network Supports Cognition that is Independent of Immediate Perceptual Input. PloS one, 10(6), e0132209.Google Scholar
Krienen, F. M., Tu, P. C., and Buckner, R. L. (2010). Clan Mentality: Evidence That the Medial Prefrontal Cortex Responds to Close Others. Journal of Neuroscience, 30(41), 1390613915.Google Scholar
Lardi, C., D’Argembeau, A., Chanal, J., Ghisletta, P., and van der Linden, M. (2010). Further Characterisation of Self-Defining Memories in Young Adults: A Study of a Swiss Sample. Memory, 18(3), 293309.Google Scholar
Lavenex, P., and Amaral, D. G. (2000). Hippocampal-Neocortical Interaction: A Hierarchy of Associativity. Hippocampus, 10(4), 420430.Google Scholar
Leary, M. R., Tipsord, J. M., and Tate, E. B. (2008). Allo-Inclusive Identity: Incorporating the Social and Natural Worlds into One’s Sense of Self. In Wayment, H. A. and Bauer, J. J. (eds.), Decade of Behavior Transcending Self-interest: Psychological Explorations of the Quiet Ego. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association, 137147.Google Scholar
Lieberman, M. D. (2007). Social Cognitive Neuroscience: A Review of Core Processes. Annual Review of Psychology, 58, 259289.Google Scholar
Mar, R. A. (2011). The Neural Bases of Social Cognition and Story Comprehension. Annual Review of Psychology, 62, 103134.Google Scholar
Mar, R. A., Mason, M. F., and Litvack, A. (2012). How Daydreaming Relates to Life Satisfaction, Loneliness, and Social Support: The Importance of Gender and Daydream Content. Consciousness and Cognition, 21(1), 401407.Google Scholar
Milligan, K., Astington, J. W., and Dack, L. A. (2007). Language and Theory of Mind: Meta‐Analysis of the Relation between Language Ability and False‐Belief Understanding. Child Development, 78(2), 622646.Google Scholar
Mills, C., Raffaelli, Q., Irving, Z. C., Stan, D., and Christoff, K. (2018). Is an Off-Task Mind a Freely-Moving Mind? Examining the Relationship between Different Dimensions of Thought. Consciousness and Cognition, 58, 2033.Google Scholar
Mooneyham, B. W., and Schooler, J. W. (2013). The Costs and Benefits of Mind-Wandering: A Review. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology/Revue canadienne de psychologie expérimentale, 67(1), 11.Google Scholar
Moran, J. M., Heatherton, T. F., and Kelley, W. M. (2009). Modulation of Cortical Midline Structures by Implicit and Explicit Self-Relevance Evaluation. Social Neuroscience, 4(3), 197211.Google Scholar
Mullally, S. L., and Maguire, E. A. (2014). Memory, Imagination, and Predicting the Future: A Common Brain Mechanism? The Neuroscientist, 20(3), 220234.Google Scholar
Paivio, A. (1986). Mental Representations: A Dual Coding Approach. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Parikh, N., Ruzic, L., Stewart, G. W., Spreng, R. N., and De Brigard, F. (2018). What if? Neural Activity Underlying Semantic and Episodic Counterfactual Thinking. Neuroimage, 178, 332345.Google Scholar
Pfeiffer, B. E. (2020). The Content of Hippocampal “Replay”. Hippocampus, 30(1), 618.Google Scholar
Prebble, S. C., Addis, D. R., and Tippett, L. J. (2013). Autobiographical Memory and Sense of Self. Psychological Bulletin, 139(4), 815.Google Scholar
Raffaelli, Q., Mills, C., and Christoff, K. (2017). The Knowns and Unknowns of Boredom: A Review of the Literature. Experimental Brain Research, 236(9), 24512462.Google Scholar
Rim, S., Amit, E., Fujita, K., et al. (2015). How Words Transcend and Pictures Immerse: On the Association between Medium and Level of Construal. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 6(2), 123130.Google Scholar
Ross, B. (2016). Imagine a Dog. Got it? I Don’t. Here’s What it’s Like to be Unable to Visualize Anything. Vox. May 19. www.vox.com/2016/5/19/11683274/aphantasia.Google Scholar
Ruby, F. J. M., Smallwood, J., Engen, H., and Singer, T. (2013). How Self-Generated Thought Shapes Mood – The Relation between Mind-Wandering and Mood Depends on the Socio-Temporal Content of Thoughts. PloS ONE, 8, e77554.Google Scholar
Saxe, R. (2009). The Happiness of the Fish: Evidence for a Common Theory of One’s Own and Others’ Actions. In Markman, K. D., Klein, W. M., and Suhr, J. A., (eds.), Handbook of Imagination and Mental Simulation. New York, NY: Psychology Press, 257266.Google Scholar
Schacter, D. L., and Addis, D. R. (2007). The Cognitive Neuroscience of Constructive Memory: Remembering the Past and Imagining the Future. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 362(1481), 773786.Google Scholar
Schacter, D. L., Addis, D. R., and Buckner, R. L. (2007). Remembering the Past to Imagine the Future: The Prospective Brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 8(9), 657.Google Scholar
Schacter, D. L., Addis, D. R., Hassabis, D., et al. (2012). The Future of Memory: Remembering, Imagining, and the Brain. Neuron, 76(4), 677694.Google Scholar
Seligman, M. E., Railton, P., Baumeister, R. F., and Sripada, C. (2016). Homo Prospectus. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Seyfarth, R. M., and Cheney, D. L. (2014). The Evolution of Language from Social Cognition. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 28, 59.Google Scholar
Sheldon, S., Farb, N., Palombo, D. J., and Levine, B. (2016). Intrinsic Medial Temporal Lobe Connectivity Relates to Individual Differences in Episodic Autobiographical Remembering. Cortex, 74, 206216.Google Scholar
Smallwood, J., and Andrews-Hanna, J. (2013). Not All Minds That Wander Are Lost: The Importance of a Balanced Perspective on the Mind-Wandering State. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 441.Google Scholar
Smallwood, J., and Schooler, J. W. (2015). The Science of Mind Wandering: Empirically Navigating the Stream of Consciousness. Annual Review of Psychology, 66, 487518.Google Scholar
Spreng, R. N., and Andrews-Hanna, J. R. (2015). The Default Network and Social Cognition. Brain Mapping: An Encyclopedic Reference, 1316, 165169.Google Scholar
Spreng, R. N., DuPre, E., Selarka, D., et al. (2014). Goal-Congruent Default Network Activity Facilitates Cognitive Control. Journal of Neuroscience, 34, 1410814114.Google Scholar
Spreng, R. N., Gerlach, K. D., Turner, G. R., and Schacter, D. L. (2015). Autobiographical Planning and the Brain: Activation and its Modulation by Qualitative Features. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 27(11), 21472157.Google Scholar
Spreng, R. N., Mar, R. A., and Kim, A. S. (2009). The Common Neural Basis of Autobiographical Memory, Prospection, Navigation, Theory of Mind, and the Default Mode: A Quantitative Meta-Analysis. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21(3), 489510.Google Scholar
Spreng, R. N., Stevens, W. D., Chamberlain, J. P., Gilmore, A. W., and Schacter, D. L. (2010). Default Network Activity, Coupled with the Frontoparietal Control Network, Supports Goal-Directed Cognition. NeuroImage, 53, 303–317.Google Scholar
Spunt, R. P., Kemmerer, D., and Adolphs, R. (2015). The Neural Basis of Conceptualizing the Same Action at Different Levels of Abstraction. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 11(7), 11411151.Google Scholar
Spunt, R. P., Meyer, M. L., and Lieberman, M. D. (2015). The Default Mode of Human Brain Function Primes the Intentional Stance. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 27(6), 11161124.Google Scholar
Sripada, C. S. (2018). An Exploration/Exploitation Trade-off Between Mind-Wandering and Goal-Directed Thinking. In Fox, K. C. and Christoff, K (eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Spontaneous Thought: Mind-Wandering, Creativity, and Dreaming. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press, 2334.Google Scholar
Stawarczyk, D., and D’Argembeau, A. (2015). Neural Correlates of Personal Goal Processing during Episodic Future Thinking and Mind‐Wandering: An ALE Meta‐Analysis. Human Brain Mapping, 36(8), 29282947.Google Scholar
Stawarczyk, D., Cassol, H., and D’Argembeau, A. (2013). Phenomenology of Future-Oriented Mind-Wandering Episodes. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 425.Google Scholar
Stawarczyk, D., Majerus, S., Maj, M., van der Linden, M., and D’Argembeau, A. (2011). Mind-Wandering: Phenomenology and Function as Assessed with a Novel Experience Sampling Method. Acta Psychologica, 136(3), 370–381.Google Scholar
Suddendorf, T., and Corballis, M. C. (2007). The Evolution of Foresight: What Is Mental Time Travel, and Is It Unique to Humans? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 30(3), 299313.Google Scholar
Tamir, D. I., Bricker, A. B., Dodell-Feder, D., and Mitchell, J. P. (2015). Reading Fiction and Reading Minds: The Role of Simulation in the Default Network. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 11(2), 215224.Google Scholar
Trope, Y., and Liberman, N. (2010). Construal-Level Theory of Psychological Distance. Psychological Review, 117(2), 440.Google Scholar
Tulving, E. (1985). Memory and Consciousness. Canadian Psychology/Psychologie canadienne, 26(1), 1.Google Scholar
van Veluw, S. J., and Chance, S. A. (2014). Differentiating between Self and Others: An ALE Meta-Analysis of fMRI Studies of Self-Recognition and Theory of Mind. Brain Imaging and Behavior, 8(1), 2438.Google Scholar
von Helmholtz, H. (1925). Treatise on Physiological Optics. Translated from the 3rd German edition and edited by Southall, J. Volume 3: The Perceptions of Vision. Ithaca, NY: Secretary, Optical Society of America, Rockefeller Hall.Google Scholar
Wang, H. T., Poerio, G., Murphy, C., et al. (2018). Dimensions of Experience: Exploring the Heterogeneity of the Wandering Mind. Psychological Science, 29(1), 5671.Google Scholar
Yeo, B. T. T., Krienen, F. M., Sepulcre, J., et al. (2011). The Organization of the Human Cerebral Cortex Estimated by Intrinsic Functional Connectivity. Journal of Neurophysiology, 106(3), 11251165.Google Scholar
Zaki, J., and Ochsner, K. N. (2012). The Neuroscience of Empathy: Progress, Pitfalls and Promise. Nature Neuroscience, 15(5), 675.Google Scholar

References

Addis, D. R., Sacchetti, D. C., Ally, B. A., Budson, A. E., and Schacter, D. L. (2009). Episodic Simulation of Future Events Is Impaired in Mild Alzheimer’s Disease. Neuropsychologia, 47, 26602671.Google Scholar
Anderson, R. J. (2012). Imagining Novel Futures: The Roles of Event Plausibility and Familiarity. Memory, 20, 443451.Google Scholar
Anderson, R. J., and Dewhurst, S. A. (2009). Remembering the Past and Imagining the Future: Differences in Event Specificity of Spontaneously Generated Thought. Memory, 17, 367373.Google Scholar
Andrews-Hanna, J. R., Reidler, J. S., Sepulcre, J., Poulin, R., and Buckner, R. L. (2010). Functional-Anatomic Fractionation of the Brain’s Default Network. Neuron, 65, 550562.Google Scholar
Araujo, H. F., Kaplan, J., and Damasio, A. (2013). Cortical Midline Structures and Autobiographical-Self Processes: An Activation-Likelihood Estimation Meta-Analysis. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 548.Google Scholar
Atance, C. M., and O’Neill, D. K. (2001). Episodic Future Thinking. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 5(12), 533539.Google Scholar
Austin, J. T., and Vancouver, J. B. (1996). Goal Constructs in Psychology: Structure, Process, and Content. Psychological Bulletin, 120, 338375.Google Scholar
Barsics, C., van der Linden, M., and D’Argembeau, A. (2016). Frequency, Characteristics, and Perceived Functions of Emotional Future Thinking in Daily Life. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 69, 217233. Doi/10.1080/17470218.2015.1051560.Google Scholar
Ben Malek, H., Berna, F., and D’Argembeau, A. (2017). Reconstructing the Times of Past and Future Personal Events. Memory, 25, 14021411.Google Scholar
Benoit, R. G., and Schacter, D. L. (2015). Specifying the Core Network Supporting Episodic Simulation and Episodic Memory by Activation Likelihood Estimation. Neuropsychologia, 75, 450457.Google Scholar
Berntsen, D., and Bohn, A. (2010). Remembering and Forecasting: The Relation between Autobiographical Memory and Episodic Future Thinking. Memory & Cognition, 38, 265278.Google Scholar
Berntsen, D., and Jacobsen, A. S. (2008). Involuntary (Spontaneous) Mental Time Travel into the Past and Future. Consciousnes and Cognition, 17, 10931104.Google Scholar
Blagov, P. S., and Singer, J. A. (2004). Four Dimensions of Self-Defining Memories (Specificity, Meaning, Content, and Affect) and Their Relationships to Self-Restraint, Distress, and Repressive Defensiveness. Journal of Personality, 72, 481511.Google Scholar
Boyer, P. (2008). Evolutionary Economics of Mental Time Travel? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12, 219224.Google Scholar
Brod, G., Werkle-Bergner, M., and Shing, Y. L. (2013). The Influence of Prior Knowledge on Memory: A Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience Perspective. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 7, 139.Google Scholar
Buckner, R. L., Andrews-Hanna, J. R., and Schacter, D. L. (2008). The Brain’s Default Network – Anatomy, Function, and Relevance to Disease. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1124, 138.Google Scholar
Bulley, A., Henry, J., and Suddendorf, T. (2016). Prospection and the Present Moment: The Role of Episodic Foresight in Intertemporal Choices between Immediate and Delayed Rewards. Review of General Psychology, 20, 2947.Google Scholar
Cabeza, R., and St Jacques, P. (2007). Functional Neuroimaging of Autobiographical Memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11, 219227.Google Scholar
Chessell, Z. J., Rathbone, C. J., Souchay, C., Charlesworth, L., and Moulin, C. J. A. (2014). Autobiographical Memory, Past and Future Events, and Self-images in Younger and Older Adults. Self and Identity, 13, 380397.Google Scholar
Christian, B. M., Miles, L. K., Fung, F. H. K., Best, S., and Macrae, C. N. (2013). The Shape of Things to Come: Exploring Goal-Directed Prospection. Consciousness and Cognition, 22, 471478.Google Scholar
Cole, S. N., and Berntsen, D. (2016). Do Future Thoughts Reflect Personal Goals? Current Concerns and Mental Time Travel into the Past and Future. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 69, 273284.Google Scholar
Conway, M. A. (2005). Memory and the Self. Journal of Memory and Language, 53, 594628.Google Scholar
Conway, M. A. (2009). Episodic Memories. Neuropsychologia, 47, 23052313.Google Scholar
Conway, M. A., Justice, L., and D’Argembeau, A. (2019). The Self-Memory System Revisited: Past, Present, and Future. In Mace, J. H. (ed.), The Organization and Structure of Autobiographical Memory. New York, NY: Oxford University Press, 28–51.Google Scholar
Conway, M. A., and Pleydell-Pearce, C. W. (2000). The Construction of Autobiographical Memories in the Self-Memory System. Psychological Review 107, 261288.Google Scholar
Conway, M. A., Singer, J. A., and Tagini, A. (2004). The Self and Autobiographical Memory: Correspondence and Coherence. Social Cognition, 22, 495537.Google Scholar
D’Argembeau, A. (2013). On the Role of the Ventromedial Prefrontal Cortex in Self-Processing: The Valuation Hypothesis. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 372.Google Scholar
D’Argembeau, A. (2016). The Role of Personal Goals in Future-Oriented Mental Time Travel. In Michaelian, K, Klein, S. B, and Szpunar, K. K. (eds.), Seeing the Future: Theoretical Perspectives on Future-Oriented Mental Time Travel. New York, NY: Oxford University Press, 199214.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
D’Argembeau, A., and Demblon, J. (2012). On the Representational Systems Underlying Prospection: Evidence from the Event-Cueing Paradigm. Cognition, 125, 160167.Google Scholar
D’Argembeau, A., Jedidi, H., Balteau, E., et al. (2012). Valuing One’s Self: Medial Prefrontal Involvement in Epistemic and Emotive Investments in Self-Views. Cerebral Cortex, 22, 659667.Google Scholar
D’Argembeau, A., Lardi, C., and van der Linden, M. (2012). Self-Defining Future Projections: Exploring the Identity Function of Thinking about the Future. Memory, 20, 110120.Google Scholar
D’Argembeau, A., and Mathy, A. (2011). Tracking the Construction of Episodic Future Thoughts. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 140, 258271.Google Scholar
D’Argembeau, A., Raffard, S., and van der Linden, M. (2008). Remembering the Past and Imagining the Future in Schizophrenia. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 117, 247251.Google Scholar
D’Argembeau, A., Renaud, O., and van der Linden, M. (2011). Frequency, Characteristics, and Functions of Future-Oriented Thoughts in Daily Life. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 25, 96103.Google Scholar
D’Argembeau, A., Stawarczyk, D., Majerus, S., et al. (2010). The Neural Basis of Personal Goal Processing when Envisioning Future Events. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22, 17011713.Google Scholar
D’Argembeau, A., and van der Linden, M. (2004). Phenomenal Characteristics Associated with Projecting Oneself Back into the Past and Forward into the Future: Influence of Valence and Temporal Distance. Consciousness and Cognition, 13, 844858.Google Scholar
D’Argembeau, A., and van der Linden, M. (2006). Individual Differences in the Phenomenology of Mental Time Travel: The Effect of Vivid Visual Imagery and Emotion Regulation Strategies. Consciousness and Cognition, 15, 342350.Google Scholar
D’Argembeau, A., and van der Linden, M. (2012). Predicting the Phenomenology of Episodic Future Thoughts. Consciousness and Cognition, 21, 11981206.Google Scholar
De Brigard, F., Giovanello, K. S., Stewart, G. W., et al. (2016). Characterizing the Subjective Experience of Episodic Past, Future, and Counterfactual Thinking in Healthy Younger and Older Adults. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 69, 23582375.Google Scholar
Demblon, J., Bahri, M. A., and D’Argembeau, A. (2016). Neural Correlates of Event Clusters in Past and Future Thoughts: How the Brain Integrates Specific Episodes with Autobiographical Knowledge. NeuroImage, 127, 257266.Google Scholar
Demblon, J., and D’Argembeau, A. (2014). The Organization of Prospective Thinking: Evidence of Event Clusters in Freely Generated Future Thoughts. Consciousness and Cognition, 24, 7583.Google Scholar
Demblon, J., and D’Argembeau, A. (2017). Contribution of Past and Future Self-Defining Event Networks to Personal Identity. Memory, 25, 656665.Google Scholar
Emmons, R. A. (1986). Personal Strivings: An Approach to Personality and Subjective Well-Being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 10581068.Google Scholar
Ernst, A., and D’Argembeau, A. (2017). Make It Real: Belief in Occurrence within Episodic Future Thought. Memory & Cognition, 45(6), 10451061.Google Scholar
Ferrante, D., Girotto, V., Straga, M., and Walsh, C. (2013). Improving the Past and the Future: A Temporal Asymmetry in Hypothetical Thinking. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 142, 2327.Google Scholar
Friedman, W. J. (1993). Memory for the Time of Past Events. Psychological Bulletin, 113, 4466.Google Scholar
Gilboa, A., and Marlatte, H. (2017). Neurobiology of Schemas and Schema-Mediated Memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 21, 618631.Google Scholar
Grilli, M. D. (2017). The Association of Personal Semantic Memory to Identity Representations: Insight into Higher-Order Networks of Autobiographical Contents. Memory, 25, 14351443.Google Scholar
Habermas, T., and Bluck, S. (2000). Getting a Life: The Emergence of the Life Story in Adolescence. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 748769.Google Scholar
Haque, S., and Conway, M. A. (2001). Sampling the Process of Autobiographical Memory Construction. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 13, 529547.Google Scholar
Hassabis, D., Kumaran, D., Vann, S. D., and Maguire, E. A. (2007). Patients with Hippocampal Amnesia Cannot Imagine New Experiences. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 104, 17261731.Google Scholar
Irish, M., Addis, D. R., Hodges, J. R., and Piguet, O. (2012). Considering the Role of Semantic Memory in Episodic Future Thinking: Evidence from Semantic Dementia. Brain, 135(Pt 7), 21782191.Google Scholar
Irish, M., and Piguet, O. (2013). The Pivotal Role of Semantic Memory in Remembering the Past and Imagining the Future. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 7, 27.Google Scholar
Jeunehomme, O., and D’Argembeau, A. (2016). Prevalence and Determinants of Direct and Generative Modes of Production of Episodic Future Thoughts in the Word Cueing Paradigm. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 69, 254272.Google Scholar
Kim, H. (2012). A Dual-Subsystem Model of the Brain’s Default Network: Self-Referential Processing, Memory Retrieval Processes, and Autobiographical Memory Retrieval. Neuroimage, 61, 966977.Google Scholar
Klein, S. B. (2013). The Complex Act of Projecting Oneself into the Future. Wiley Interdiscipinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 4, 6379.Google Scholar
Klein, S. B. (2016). Autonoetic Consciousness: Reconsidering the Role of Episodic Memory in Future-Oriented Self-Projection. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 69, 381401.Google Scholar
Klein, S. B., and Gangi, C. E. (2010). The Multiplicity of Self: Neuropsychological Evidence and its Implications for the Self as a Construct in Psychological Research. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1191, 115.Google Scholar
Klein, S. B., and Lax, M. L. (2010). The Unanticipated Resilience of Trait Self-Knowledge in the Face of Neural Damage. Memory, 18, 918948.Google Scholar
Klein, S. B., and Loftus, J. (1993). The Mental Representation of Trait and Autobiographical Knowledge about the Self. In Sr, T. K.ull and Wyer, R. S. (eds.), Advances in Social Cognition. Volume 5. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 149.Google Scholar
Klein, S. B., Loftus, J., and Kihlstrom, J. F. (1996). Self-Knowledge of an Amnesic Patient: Toward a Neuropsychology of Personality and Social Psychology. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 125, 250260.Google Scholar
Klein, S. B., Loftus, J., and Kihlstrom, J. F. (2002). Memory and Temporal Experience: The Effects of Episodic Memory Loss on an Amnesic Patient’s Ability to Remember the Past and Imagine the Future. Social Cognition, 20, 353379.Google Scholar
Klinger, E. (2013). Goal Commitments and the Content of Thoughts and Dreams: Basic Principles. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 415.Google Scholar
Leary, M. R. (2007). Motivational and Emotional Aspects of the Self. Annual Review of Psychology, 58, 317344.Google Scholar
Lehner, E., and D’Argembeau, A. (2016). The Role of Personal Goals in Autonoetic Experience when Imagining Future Events. Consciousness and Cognition, 42, 267276.Google Scholar
Little, B. R. (1983). Personal Projects: A Rationale and Method for Investigation. Environment and Behavior, 15, 273309.Google Scholar
MacLeod, A. K., and Byrne, A. (1996). Anxiety, Depression, and the Anticipation of Future Positive and Negative Experiences. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 105, 286289.Google Scholar
Markus, H. (1977). Self-Schemata and Processing Information about the Self. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 35, 6378.Google Scholar
Markus, H., and Nurius, P. (1986). Possible Selves. American Psychologist, 41, 954969.Google Scholar
Markus, H., and Wurf, E. (1987). The Dynamic Self-Concept: A Social Psychological Perspective. Annual Review of Psychology, 38, 299337.Google Scholar
Martinelli, P., Sperduti, M., and Piolino, P. (2013). Neural Substrates of the Self-Memory System: New Insights from a Meta-Analysis. Human Brain Mapping, 34, 15151529.Google Scholar
McAdams, D. P. (2013). The Psychological Self as Actor, Agent, and Author. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 8(3), 272295.Google Scholar
Murray, R. J., Schaer, M., and Debbane, M. (2012). Degrees of Separation: A Quantitative Neuroimaging Meta-Analysis Investigating Self-Specificity and Shared Neural Activation between Self- and Other-Reflection. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 36, 10431059.Google Scholar
Newby-Clark, I. R., and Ross, M. (2003). Conceiving the Past and Future. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29, 807818.Google Scholar
O’Donnell, S., Oluyomi Daniel, T., and Epstein, L. H. (2017). Does Goal Relevant Episodic Future Thinking Amplify the Effect on Delay Discounting? Consciousness and Cognition, 51, 1016.Google Scholar
Oyserman, D., and James, L. (2009). Possible Selves: From Content to Process. In Markman, K. D., Klein, M. P., and Suhr, J. A. (eds.), Handbook of Imagination and Mental Simulation. New York, NY: Psychology Press, 373394.Google Scholar
Packard, B. W.-L., and Conway, P. F. (2006). Methodological Choice and its Consequences for Possible Selves Research. Identity: An International Journal of Theory and Research, 6, 251271.Google Scholar
Peters, J., and Buchel, C. (2010). Episodic Future Thinking Reduces Reward Delay Discounting through an Enhancement of Prefrontal-Mediotemporal Interactions. Neuron, 66, 138148.Google Scholar
Prebble, S. C., Addis, D. R., and Tippett, L. J. (2013). Autobiographical Memory and Sense of Self. Psychological Bulletin, 139, 815840.Google Scholar
Quoidbach, J., Gilbert, D. T., and Wilson, T. D. (2013). The End of History Illusion. Science, 339, 9698.Google Scholar
Radvansky, G. A., and Zacks, J. M. (2011). Event Perception. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 2(6), 608620.Google Scholar
Rasmussen, A. S., and Berntsen, D. (2013). The Reality of the Past versus the Ideality of the Future: Emotional Valence and Functional Differences between Past and Future Mental Time Travel. Memory & Cognition, 41, 187200.Google Scholar
Rathbone, C. J., Conway, M. A., and Moulin, C. J. (2011). Remembering and Imagining: The Role of the Self. Consciousness and Cognition, 20, 11751182.Google Scholar
Rathbone, C. J., Moulin, C. J., and Conway, M. A. (2009). Autobiographical Memory and Amnesia: Using Conceptual Knowledge to Ground the Self. Neurocase, 15, 405418.Google Scholar
Renoult, L., Davidson, P. S., Palombo, D. J., Moscovitch, M., and Levine, B. (2012). Personal Semantics: At the Crossroads of Semantic and Episodic Memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16, 550558.Google Scholar
Roese, N. J. (1997). Counterfactual Thinking. Psychological Bulletin, 121(1), 133148.Google Scholar
Roy, M., Shohamy, D., and Wager, T. D. (2012). Ventromedial Prefrontal-Subcortical Systems and the Generation of Affective Meaning. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16, 147156.Google Scholar
Schacter, D. L., and Addis, D. R. (2007). The Cognitive Neuroscience of Constructive Memory: Remembering the Past and Imagining the Future. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 362, 773786.Google Scholar
Schacter, D. L., Addis, D. R., Hassabis, D., et al. (2012). The Future of Memory: Remembering, Imagining, and the Brain. Neuron, 76, 677694.Google Scholar
Schacter, D. L., Benoit, R. G., De Brigard, F., and Szpunar, K. K. (2015). Episodic Future Thinking and Episodic Counterfactual Thinking: Intersections between Memory and Decisions. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 117, 1421.Google Scholar
Schacter, D. L., Benoit, R. G., and Szpunar, K. K. (2017). Episodic Future Thinking: Mechanisms and Functions. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 17, 4150.Google Scholar
Sedikides, C., and Gregg, A. P. (2008). Self-Enhancement: Food for Thought. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 3, 102116.Google Scholar
Singer, J. A., Blagov, P., Berry, M., and Oost, K. M. (2013). Self-Defining Memories, Scripts, and the Life Story: Narrative Identity in Personality and Psychotherapy. Journal of Personality, 81, 569582.Google Scholar
Spreng, R. N., Mar, R. A., and Kim, A. S. (2009). The Common Neural Basis of Autobiographical Memory, Prospection, Navigation, Theory of Mind, and the Default Mode: A Quantitative Meta-Analysis. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21, 489510.Google Scholar
Stawarczyk, D., Cassol, H., and D’Argembeau, A. (2013). Phenomenology of Future-Oriented Mind-Wandering Episodes. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 425.Google Scholar
Stawarczyk, D., and D’Argembeau, A. (2015). Neural Correlates of Personal Goal Processing during Episodic Future Thinking and Mind-Wandering: An ALE Meta-Analysis. Human Brain Mapping, 36(8), 29282947.Google Scholar
Suddendorf, T., and Corballis, M. C. (2007). The Evolution of Foresight: What is Mental Time Travel and is it Unique to Humans? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 30, 299351.Google Scholar
Svoboda, E., McKinnon, M. C., and Levine, B. (2006). The Functional Neuroanatomy of Autobiographical Memory: A Meta-Analysis. Neuropsychologia, 44, 21892208.Google Scholar
Szpunar, K. K. (2010). Episodic Future Thought: An Emerging Concept. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 5, 142162.Google Scholar
Taylor, S. E., and Brown, J. D. (1988). Illusion and Well-Being: A Social Psychological Perspective on Mental Health. Psychological Bulletin, 103, 193210.Google Scholar
Taylor, S. E., Pham, L. B., Rivkin, I. D., and Armor, D. A. (1998). Harnessing the Imagination: Mental Simulation, Self-Regulation, and Coping. American Psychologist, 53, 429439.Google Scholar
Thompson, C. P., Skowronski, J. J., and Betz, A. L. (1993). The Use of Partial Temporal Information in Dating Personal Events. Memory & Cognition, 21, 352360.Google Scholar
Thomsen, D. K. (2015). Autobiographical Periods: A Review and Central Components of a Theory. Review of General Psychology, 19, 294310.Google Scholar
Thomsen, D. K., Steiner, K. L., and Pillemer, D. B. (2016). Life Story Chapters: Past and Future, You and Me. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 5, 143149.Google Scholar
Tulving, E. (1985). Memory and Consciousness. Canadian Psychologist, 26, 112.Google Scholar
Tulving, E. (2005). Episodic Memory and Autonoesis: Uniquely Human? In Terrace, H. S. and Metcalfe, J (eds.), The Missing Link in Cognition: Origins of Self-Reflective Consciousness. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press, 356.Google Scholar
Uzer, T., Lee, P. J., and Brown, N. R. (2012). On the Prevalence of Directly Retrieved Autobiographical Memories. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 38, 12961308.Google Scholar
van der Meer, L., Costafreda, S., Aleman, A., and David, A. S. (2010). Self-Reflection and the Brain: A Theoretical Review and Meta-Analysis of Neuroimaging Studies with Implications for Schizophrenia. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 34, 935946.Google Scholar
van Hoeck, N., Ma, N., Ampe, L., et al. (2013). Counterfactual Thinking: An fMRI Study on Changing the Past for a Better Future. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 8, 556564.Google Scholar
van Kesteren, M. T. R., Ruiter, D. J., Fernández, G., and Henson, R. N. (2012). How Schema and Novelty Augment Memory Formation. Trends in Neurosciences, 35, 211219.Google Scholar
Wagner, D. D., Haxby, J. V., and Heatherton, T. F. (2012). The Representation of Self and Person Knowledge in the Medial Prefrontal Cortex. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 3(4), 451470.Google Scholar
Williams, J. M. G., Barnhofer, T., Crane, C., et al. (2007). Autobiographical Memory Specificity and Emotional Disorder. Psychological Bulletin, 133, 122148.Google Scholar

References

Astington, J. W., and Baird, J. A. (eds.) (2005). Why Language Matters for Theory of Mind. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Astington, J. W., and Jenkins, J. M. (1999). A Longitudinal Study of the Relation between Language and Theory-of-Mind Development. Developmental Psychology, 35(5), 13111320.Google Scholar
Bauer, K. L., and Dettore, E. (1997). Superhero Play: What’s a Teacher to Do? Early Childhood Education Journal, 25(1), 1721.Google Scholar
Boone, O., Canetti, L., Bachar, E., De-Nour, A. K., and Shaley, A. (1999). Childhood Imaginary Companionship and Mental Health in Adolescence. Child Psychiatry and Human Development, 29(4), 277286.Google Scholar
Bouldin, P. (2006). An Investigation of the Fantasy Predisposition and Fantasy Style of Children with Imaginary Companions. The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 167(1), 1729.Google Scholar
Bouldin, P., and Pratt, C. (1999). Characteristics of Preschool and School-Age Children with Imaginary Companions. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 160(4), 397410.Google Scholar
Carlson, S. M., and Taylor, M. (2005). Imaginary Companions and Impersonated Characters: Sex Differences in Children’s Fantasy Play. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly Journal of Developmental Psychology, 51(1), 93118.Google Scholar
Cassidy, K. W., Fineberg, D. S., Brown, K., Perkins, A. (2005). Theory of Mind May Be Contagious, but You Don’t Catch It from Your Twin. Child Development, 76(1), 97106.Google Scholar
Cutting, A. L., and Dunn, J. (1999). Theory of Mind, Emotion Understanding, Language, and Family Background; Individual Differences and Interrelations. Child Development, 70(4), 853865.Google Scholar
Davis, P. E. (2011). Does Having an Imaginary Companion Relate to Children’s Understanding of Self and Others? (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). Durham, UK: Durham University.Google Scholar
Davis, P.E., Meins, E., and Fernyhough, C. (2011). Self-Knowledge in Childhood: Relations with Children’s Imaginary Companions and Understanding of Mind. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 29, 680686.Google Scholar
Davis, P.E., Meins, E., and Fernyhough, C. (2013). Individual Differences in Children’s Private Speech: The Role of Imaginary Companions. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 116, 561571.Google Scholar
Davis, P.E., Meins, E., and Fernyhough, C. (2014). Children with Imaginary Companions Focus on Mental Characteristics when Describing Their Real-Life Friends. Journal of Infant and Child Development, 23(3), 622633.Google Scholar
Davis, P. E., Simon, H., Robins, D., and Meins, E. (2018). Imaginary Companions in Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 48(8), 27902799.Google Scholar
Dunn, J., Brown, J., and Beardsall, L. (1991). Family Talk about Feeling States and Children’s Later Understanding of Others’ Emotions. Developmental Psychology, 27(3), 448455.Google Scholar
Fernyhough, C., Bland, K. A., Meins, E., and Coltheart, M. (2007). Imaginary Companions and Young Children’s Responses to Ambiguous Auditory Stimuli: Implications for Typical and Atypical Development. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 48(11), 10941101.Google Scholar
Flavell, J. H., Flavell, E. R., and Green, F. L. (1983). Development of the Appearance-Reality Distinction. Cognitive Psychology, 15, 95120.Google Scholar
Flavell, J. H., Flavell, E. R., and Green, F. L. (1987). Young Children’s Knowledge about the Apparent-Real and Pretend-Real Distinctions. Developmental Psychology, 23, 816822.Google Scholar
Flavell, J. H., Green, F. L., and Flavell, E. R. (1993). Children’s Understanding of the Stream of Consciousness. Child Development, 64, 387398.Google Scholar
Giménez-Dasí, M., Pons, F., and Bender, P. (2016). Imaginary Companions, Theory of Mind and Emotion Understanding in Young Children. European Early Childhood Education Research Journal, 24(2), 186197.Google Scholar
Gleason, T. R. (2002). Social Provisions of Real and Imaginary Relationships in Early Childhood. Developmental Psychology, 38(6), 979992.Google Scholar
Gleason, T. R. (2004). Imaginary Companions: An Evaluation of Parents as Reporters. Journal of Infant and Child Development, 13(3), 199215.Google Scholar
Gleason, T. R. (2005). Mothers’ and Fathers’ Attitudes Regarding Pretend Play in the Context of Imaginary Companions and of Child Gender. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly Journal of Developmental Psychology, 51(4), 412436.Google Scholar
Gleason, T. R. (2017). The Psychological Significance of Play with Imaginary Companions in Early Childhood. Learning Behaviour, 45, 432440.Google Scholar
Gleason, T. R., and Hohmann, L. M. (2006). Concepts of Real and Imaginary Friendships in Early Childhood. Social Development, 15(1), 128144.Google Scholar
Gleason, T. R., and Kalpidou, M. (2014). Imaginary Companions and Young Children’s Coping and Competence. Social Development, 23(4), 820839.Google Scholar
Gleason, T. R., Sebanc, A. M., and Hartup, W. W. (2000). Imaginary Companions of Preschool Children. Developmental Psychology, 36(4), 419428.Google Scholar
Golomb, C., and Galasso, L. (1995). Make Believe and Reality: Explorations of the Imaginary Realm. Developmental Psychology, 31(5), 800810.Google Scholar
Gupta, A., and Desai, N. G. (2006). Pathological Fantasy Friend Phenomenon. International Journal of Psychiatry in Clinical Practice, 10(2), 149151.Google Scholar
Harris, P. L. (2000). The Work of the Imagination. Malden, MA: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Harter, S., and Chao, C. (1992). Role of Competence in Children’s Creation of Imaginary Friends. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 38(3), 350363.Google Scholar
Hoff, E. V. (2004). A Friend Living Inside Me – The Forms and Functions of Imaginary Companions. Imagination, Cognition and Personality, 24(2), 151189.Google Scholar
Hughes, C., Adlam, A., Happé, F., et al. (2008). Good Test-Retest Reliability for Standard and Advanced False Belief Tasks Across a Wide Range of Abilities. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 41(4), 483490.Google Scholar
Hughes, C., Fujisawa, K. K., Ensor, R., Lecce, S., and Marfleet, R. (2006). Cooperation and Conversations about the Mind: A Study of Individual Differences in 2-Year-Olds and Their Siblings. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 24, 5372.Google Scholar
Jarrold, C. (2003). A Review of Research into Pretend Play in Autism. Autism, 7(4), 379390.Google Scholar
Jenkins, J. M., Turrell, S. L., Kogushi, Y., Lollis, S., and Ross, H. S. (2003). A Longitudinal Investigation of the Dynamics of Mental State Talk in Families. Child Development, 74(3), 905920.Google Scholar
Ketelaars, M. P., van Weerdenburg, M., Verhoeven, L., Cuperus, J. M., and Jansonius, K. (2010). Dynamics of the Theory of Mind Construct: A Developmental Perspective. European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 7(1), 85103.Google Scholar
Kirk, E., Pine, K., Wheatley, L., et al. (2015). A Longitudinal Investigation of the Relationship between Maternal Mind-Mindedness and Theory of Mind. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 33(4), 434445.Google Scholar
Klausen, E., and Passman, R. H. (2007). Pretend Companions (Imaginary Playmates): The Emergence of a Field. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 167(4), 349364.Google Scholar
Leach, J., Howe, N., and DeHart, G. (2016). “I Wish My People Can Be like the Ducks”: Children’s References to Internal States with Siblings and Friends from Early to Middle Childhood. Infant and Child Development, 26e.Google Scholar
Leblanc, É., Bernier, A., and Howe, N. (2017). The More the Merrier? Sibling Composition and Early Manifestations of Theory of Mind in Toddlers. Journal of Cognition and Development, 18(3), 375391.Google Scholar
Lewis, C., and Mitchell, P. (eds.). (1994). Children’s Early Understanding of Mind: Origins and Development. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Lillard, A. S. (2017). Why Do the Children (Pretend) Play? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 21(11), 826834.Google Scholar
Lillard, A. S., and Kavanaugh, R. D. (2014). The Contribution of Symbolic Skills to the Development of an Explicit Theory of Mind. Child Development. 85(4), 15351551.Google Scholar
Manosevitz, M., Prentice, N. M., and Wilson, F. (1973). Individual and Family Correlates of Imaginary Companions in Preschool Children. Developmental Psychology, 8(1), 7279.Google Scholar
McAlister, A., and Peterson, C. (2007). A Longitudinal Study of Child Siblings and Theory of Mind Development. Cognitive Development, 22(2), 258270.Google Scholar
McInnis, M. A., Pierucci, J. M., and Gilpin, A. T. (2013). Investigating Valence and Autonomy in Children’s Relationships with Imaginary Companions. International Journal of Developmental Science, 7, 151159.Google Scholar
Meins, E. (1997). Security of Attachment and the Social Development of Cognition. Hove, UK: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Meins, E., Fernyhough, C., de Rosnay, , et al. (2012). Mind-Mindedness as a Multidimensional Construct: Appropriate and Nonattuned Mind-Related Comments Independently Predict Infant-Mother Attachment in a Socially Diverse Sample. Infancy, 17(4), 393415.Google Scholar
Meins, E., Fernyhough, C., Johnson, F., and Lidstone, J. (2006). Mind-Mindedness in Children: Individual Differences in Internal-State Talk in Middle Childhood. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 24, 181196.Google Scholar
Meins, E., Fernyhough, C., Wainwright, R., et al. (2002). Maternal Mind-Mindedness and Attachment Security as Predictors of Theory of Mind Understanding. Child Development, 73(6), 17151726.Google Scholar
Moriguchi, Y., Kanakogi, Y., Todo, N., et al. (2016). Goal Attribution toward Non-Human Objects during Infancy Predicts Imaginary Companion Status during Preschool Years. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 221.Google Scholar
Moriguchi, Y., and Shinohara, I. (2012). My Neighbor: Children’s Perception of Agency in Interactions with an Imaginary Agent. PLoS One, 7(9), e44463.Google Scholar
Motoshima, Y., Shinohara, I., Todo, N., and Moriguchi, Y. (2014). Parental Behavior and Children’s Creation of Imaginary Companions: A Longitudinal Study. European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 11(6), 716727.Google Scholar
Pearson, D., Rouse, H., Doswell, S., et al. (2001). Prevalence of Imaginary Companions in a Normal Child Population. Child Care, Health and Development, 27(1), 1322.Google Scholar
Perner, J., Ruffman, T., and Leekam, S. R. (1994). Theory of Mind Is Contagious: You Catch It from Your Sibs. Child Development, 65, 12281238.Google Scholar
Premack, D., and Woodruff, G. (1978). Does the Chimpanzee Have a Theory of Mind? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1(4), 515526.Google Scholar
Repacholi, B. M., and Gopnik, A. (1997). Early Reasoning about Desires: Evidence from 14- and 18-Month-Olds. Developmental Psychology, 33(1), 1221.Google Scholar
Roby, A. C., and Kidd, E. (2008). The Referential Communication Skills of Children with Imaginary Companions. Developmental Science, 11(4), 531540.Google Scholar
Ruffman, T., Perner, J., Naito, M., Parkin, L., and Clements, W. A. (1998). Older (But Not Younger) Siblings Facilitate False-Belief Understanding. Developmental Psychology, 34(1), 161174.Google Scholar
Ruffman, T., Slade, L., and Crowe, E. (2002). The Relation Between Children’s and Mothers’ Mental State Language and Theory-of-Mind Understanding. Child Development, 73(3), 734751.Google Scholar
Seiffge-Krenke, I. (1997). Imaginary Companions in Adolescence: Sign of a Deficit or Positive Development? Journal of Adolescence, 20, 137154.Google Scholar
Seiffge-Krenke, I. (2001). “Dear Kitty, You Asked Me …”: Imaginary Companions and Real Friends in Adolescence. Praxis der Kinderpsychologie und kinderpsychiatrie, 50(1), 115.Google Scholar
Sharon, T., and Woolley, D. (2004). Do Monsters Dream? Young Children’s Understanding of the Fantasy/Reality Distinction. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 22, 293310.Google Scholar
Shatz, M., Diesendruck, G., Martinez-Beck, I., and Akar, D. (2003). The Influence of Language and Socioeconomic Status on Children’s Understanding of False Belief. Developmental Psychology, 39(4), 717729.Google Scholar
Singer, D. G., and Singer, J. L. (1990). The House of Make-Believe: Children’s Play and the Developing Imagination. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Skolnick, D., and Bloom, P. (2006). What Does Batman Think about SpongeBob? Children’s Understanding of the Fantasy/Fantasy Distinction. Cognition 101, 1, B9B18.Google Scholar
Slaughter, V., Dennis, M. J., and Pritchard, M. (2002). Theory of Mind and Peer Acceptance in Preschool Children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 20, 545564.Google Scholar
Suway, J. G., Degan, K. A., Sussman, A. L., and Fox, N. A. (2012).The Relations Among Theory of Mind, Behavioural Inhibition, and Peer Interactions in Early Childhood. Social Development, 21(2), 331342.Google Scholar
Svendsen, M. (1934). Children’s Imaginary Companions. Archives of Neurology and Psychiatry, 2, 985999.Google Scholar
Tahiroglu, D., Mannering, A. M., and Taylor, M. (2011). Visual and Auditory Imagery Associated with Children’s Imaginary Companions. Imagination, Cognition, and Personality, 31(1–2), 99112.Google Scholar
Taylor, M. (1999). Imaginary Companions and the Children Who Create Them. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Taylor, M., and Carlson, S. (1997). The Relation between Individual Differences in Fantasy and Theory of Mind. Child Development, 68(3), 436455.Google Scholar
Taylor, M., and Carlson, S. (2002). Imaginary Companions and Elaborate Fantasy in Childhood: Discontinuity with Non-Human Animals. In Mitchell, R (ed.), Pretending and Imagination in Animals and Children. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, 67180.Google Scholar
Taylor, M., Carlson, S. M., Maring, B. L., Gerow, L., and Charley, C. M. (2004). The Characteristics and Correlates of Fantasy in School-Age Children: Imaginary Companions, Impersonation, and Social Understanding. Developmental Psychology, 40(6), 11731187.Google Scholar
Taylor, M., Carlson, S. M., and Shawber, A. B. (2007). Autonomy and Control in Children’s Interactions with Imaginary Companions. In Roth, I (ed.), Imaginative Minds. Oxford, UK: British Academy and Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Taylor, M., Hodges, S. D., and Kohanyi, A. (2003). The Characters Created by Adult Novelists and the Imaginary Companions Created by Children. Imagination, Cognition and Personality, 22, 361380.Google Scholar
Taylor, M., Sachet, A. B., Maring, B. L., and Mannering, A. M. (2013). The Assessment of Elaborated Role-Play in Young Children: Invisible Friends, Personified Objects, and Pretend Identities. Social Development, 22(1), 7593.Google Scholar
Trionfi, G., and Reese, E. (2009). A Good Story: Children with Imaginary Companions Create Richer Narratives. Child Development, 80(4), 13011313.Google Scholar
Vostrovsky, C. (1895). A Study of Imaginary Companions. Education, 15, 383398.Google Scholar
Vygotsky, L. S. (1931/1993). The Collected Works of L. S. Vygotsky. Volume 2: The Fundamentals of Defectology. Edited by R.W. Reiber and A. S. Carton. Translated by J. E. Knox and C. B. Stevens. New York, NY: Plenum.Google Scholar
Vygotsky, L. S. (1931/1998). The Collected Works of L. S. Vygotsky. Volume 5: Child Psychology. Edited by Reiber., R. W. Translated by M. J. Hall. New York, NY: Plenum.Google Scholar
Wellman, H. M., Cross, D., and Watson, J. (2001). Meta-Analysis of Theory-of-Mind Development: The Truth about False Belief. Child Development, 72(3), 655684.Google Scholar
Wellman, H. M., and Liu, D. (2004). Scaling of Theory-of-Mind Tasks. Child Development, 75(2), 523541.Google Scholar
Wigger, J. B. (2017). Invisible Friends Across Four Countries: Kenya, Malawi, Nepal, and the Dominican Republic. International Journal of Psychology, 53(1), 4652.Google Scholar
Wigger, J. B., Paxson, K., and Ryan, L. (2013). What Do Invisible Friends Know? Imaginary Companions, God, and Theory of Mind. The International Journal for the Psychology of Religion. 23, 214.Google Scholar
Wimmer, H., and Perner, J. (1983). Beliefs about Beliefs: Representation and Constraining Function of Wrong Beliefs in Young Children’s Understanding of Deception. Cognition, 13, 103128.Google Scholar
Wood, D., Bruner, J., and Ross, G. (1976). The Role of Tutoring in Problem Solving. Journal of Child Psychology and Child Psychiatry, 17, 89100.Google Scholar
Woolley, J. D. (2003). The Fantasy/Reality Distinction Revisited: The Case of Imaginary Companions. Social Development, 12(4), 622625.Google Scholar
Woolley, J. D., and Ma, L. (2009). Children’s Use of Conversational Cues to Infer Reality Status. In A. Gopnick (chair), The Role of Testimony and Domain Knowledge in Children’s Navigation of the Reality/Fantasy Distinction. Symposium conducted at the Society for Research in Child Development Biennial meeting, May, Denver, Colorado, USA.Google Scholar
Woolley, J. D., and van Reet, J. (2006). Effects of Context on Judgements Concerning Reality Status of Novel Entities. Child Development, 77(6), 17781793.Google Scholar
Youngblade, L. M., and Dunn, J. (1995). Individual Differences in Young Children’s Pretend Play with Mother and Sibling: Links to Relationships and Understandings of Other People’s Feelings and Beliefs. Child Development, 66, 14721492.Google Scholar

References

Abraham, A. (2013). The Promises and Perils of the Neuroscience of Creativity. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 246. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2013.00246.Google Scholar
Amit, E., and Greene, J. D. (2012). You See, the Ends Don’t Justify the Means: Visual Imagery and Moral Judgment. Psychological Science, 23(8), 861868. doi:10.1177/0956797611434965.Google Scholar
Andrade, J., Khalil, M., Dickson, J., May, J., and Kavanagh, D. J. (2016). Functional Imagery Training to Reduce Snacking: Testing a Novel Motivational Intervention Based on Elaborated Intrusion Theory. Appetite, 100, 256262. doi:10.1016/j.appet.2016.02.015.Google Scholar
Bell, B. E., and Loftus, E. F. (1985). Vivid Persuasion in the Courtroom. Psychological Journal of Personality Assessment, 49(6), 659664. doi:10.1207/s15327752jpa4906_16.Google Scholar
Branscombe, N. R., Owen, S., Garstka, T. A., and Coleman, J. (1996). Rape and Accident Counterfactuals: Who Might Have Done Otherwise and Would It Have Changed the Outcome? Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 26(12), 10421067.Google Scholar
Byrne, R. M. (2016). Counterfactual Thought. Annual Review of Psychology, 67, 135157. doi:10.1146/annurev-psych-122414-033249.Google Scholar
Bzdok, D., Groß, D., and Eickhoff, S. B. (2015). The Neurobiology of Moral Cognition: Relation to Theory of Mind, Empathy, and Mind-Wandering. In Clausen, J. O. and Levy, N (eds.), Handbook of Neuroethics. Dordrecht, Netherlands: Springer, 127148.Google Scholar
Caruso, E. M., and Gino, F. (2011). Blind Ethics: Closing One’s Eyes Polarizes Moral Judgments and Discourages Dishonest Behavior. Cognition, 118(2), 280285. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2010.11.008.Google Scholar
Chaiken, S., and Trope, Y. (1999). Dual Process Theories in Social Psychology. New York, NY: Guildford Press.Google Scholar
Christensen, J. F., Flexas, A., Calabrese, M., Gut, N. K., and Gomila, A. (2014). Moral Judgment Reloaded: A Moral Dilemma Validation Study. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 607. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00607.Google Scholar
Christensen, J. F., and Gomila, A. (2012). Moral Dilemmas in Cognitive Neuroscience of Moral Decision-Making: A Principled Review. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 36(4), 12491264. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.02.008.Google Scholar
Cipolletti, H., McFarlane, S., and Weissglass, C. (2016). The Moral Foreign-Language Effect. Review of Philosophy and Psychology, 29(1), 2340.Google Scholar
Civai, C., Corradi-Dell’Acqua, C., Gamer, M., and Rumiati, R. I. (2010). Are Irrational Reactions to Unfairness Truly Emotionally-Driven? Dissociated Behavioural and Emotional Responses in the Ultimatum Game Task. Cognition, 114(1), 8995. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2009.09.001.Google Scholar
Corey, J. D., Hayakawa, S., Foucart, A., et al. (2017). Our Moral Choices Are Foreign to Us. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 43(7), 11091128. doi:10.1037/xlm0000356.Google Scholar
Costa, A., Foucart, A., Hayakawa, S., et al. (2014). Your Morals Depend on Language. PLoS One, 9(4), e94842. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0094842.Google Scholar
Crisp, R. J., and Turner, R. N. (2009). Can Imagined Interactions Produce Positive Perceptions? Reducing Prejudice Through Simulated Social Contact. American Psychologist, 64(4), 231240. doi:10.1037/a0014718.Google Scholar
Crockett, M. J. (2013). Models of Morality. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17(8), 363366. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2013.06.005.Google Scholar
Cushman, F. (2008). Crime and Punishment: Distinguishing the Roles of Causal and Intentional Analyses in Moral Judgment. Cognition, 108(2), 353380. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2008.03.006.Google Scholar
Cushman, F. (2013). Action, Outcome, and Value: A Dual-System Framework for Morality. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 17(3), 273292. doi:10.1177/1088868313495594.Google Scholar
FeldmanHall, O., Dalgleish, T., Thompson, R., et al. (2012). Differential Neural Circuitry and Self-Interest in Real vs Hypothetical Moral Decisions. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 7(7), 743751. doi:10.1093/scan/nss069.Google Scholar
Foot, P. (1967). The Problem of Abortion and the Doctrine of the Double Effect. Oxford Review, 5, 17.Google Scholar
Forsythe, R., Horowitz, J. L., Savin, N. E., and Sefton, M. (1994). Fairness in Simple Bargaining Experiments. Games and Economic Behavior, 6, 347369.Google Scholar
Francis, K. B., Gummerum, M., Ganis, G., Howard, I. S., and Terbeck, S. (2018). Virtual Morality in the Helping Professions: Simulated Action and Resilience. British Journal of Psychology, 109(3), 442465. doi:10.1111/bjop.12276.Google Scholar
Francis, K. B., Howard, C., Howard, I. S., et al. (2016). Virtual Morality: Transitioning from Moral Judgment to Moral Action? PLoS One, 11(10), e0164374. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0164374.Google Scholar
Francis, K. B., Terbeck, S., Briazu, R. A., et al. (2017). Simulating Moral Actions: An Investigation of Personal Force in Virtual Moral Dilemmas. Scientific Reports, 7(1), 13954. doi:10.1038/s41598-017-13909-9.Google Scholar
Fulford, J., Milton, F., Salas, D., et al. (2018). The Neural Correlates of Visual Imagery Vividness – An fMRI Study and Literature Review. Cortex, 105, 2640. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2017.09.014.Google Scholar
Gaesser, B., Dodds, H., and Schacter, D. L. (2017). Effects of Aging on the Relation Between Episodic Simulation and Prosocial Intentions. Memory, 25(9), 12721278. doi:10.1080/09658211.2017.1288746.Google Scholar
Gaesser, B., Keeler, K., and Young, L. (2018). Moral Imagination: Facilitating Prosocial Decision-Making Through Scene Imagery and Theory of Mind. Cognition, 171, 180193. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2017.11.004.Google Scholar
Gaesser, B., and Schacter, D. L. (2014). Episodic Simulation and Episodic Memory Can Increase Intentions to Help Others. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 111(12), 44154420. doi:10.1073/pnas.1402461111.Google Scholar
Garrigan, B., Adlam, A. L., and Langdon, P. E. (2016). The Neural Correlates of Moral Decision-Making: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis of Moral Evaluations and Response Decision Judgements. Brain and Cognition, 108, 8897. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2016.07.007.Google Scholar
Geipel, J., Hadjichristidis, C., and Surian, L. (2015). The Foreign Language Effect on Moral Judgment: The Role of Emotions and Norms. PLoS One, 10(7), e0131529. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0131529.Google Scholar
Geipel, J., Hadjichristidis, C., and Surian, L. (2016). Foreign Language Affects the Contribution of Intentions and Outcomes to Moral Judgment. Cognition, 154, 3439. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2016.05.010.Google Scholar
Gold, V. (1987). Psychological Manipulation in the Courtroom. Nebraska Law Review, 66(3), 562583.Google Scholar
Goldinger, S. D., Kleider, H. M., Azuma, T., and Beike, D. R. (2003). “Blaming the Victim” Under Memory Load. Psychological Science, 14(1), 8185. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.01423.Google Scholar
Greene, J. D. (2013). Moral Tribes: Emotion, Reason, and the Gap Between Us and Them: New York, NY: The Penguin Press.Google Scholar
Greene, J. D. (2014). The Cognitive Neuroscience of Moral Judgment and Decision-Making. In Gazzaniga, M. S. (ed.), The Cognitive Neurosciences V. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Greene, J. D. (2015). The Rise of Moral Cognition. Cognition, 135, 3942. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2014.11.018.Google Scholar
Greene, J. D., and Haidt, J. (2002). How (and Where) Does Moral Judgment Work? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6(12), 517523.Google Scholar
Greene, J. D., Nystrom, L. E., Engell, A. D., Darley, J. M., and Cohen, J. D. (2004). The Neural Bases of Cognitive Conflict and Control in Moral Judgment. Neuron, 44(2), 389400. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2004.09.027.Google Scholar
Greene, J. D., Sommerville, R. B., Nystrom, L. E., Darley, J. M., and Cohen, J. D. (2001). An fMRI Investigation of Emotional Engagement in Moral Judgment. Science, 293(5537), 21052108. doi:10.1126/science.1062872.Google Scholar
Hackel, L. M., and Amodio, D. M. (2018). Computational Neuroscience Approaches to Social Cognition. Current Opinion in Psychology, 24, 9297. doi:10.1016/j.copsyc.2018.09.001.Google Scholar
Haidt, J. (2001). The Emotional Dog and its Rational Tail: A Social Intuitionist Approach to Moral Judgment. Psychological Review, 108(4), 814834.Google Scholar
Hayakawa, S., and Keysar, B. (2018). Using a Foreign Language Reduces Mental Imagery. Cognition, 173, 815. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2017.12.010.Google Scholar
Helion, C., and Pizarro, D. A. (2015). Beyond Dual-Processes: The Interplay of Reason and Emotion in Moral Judgment. In Clausen, J. O. and Levy, N (eds.), Handbook of Neuroethics. Dordrecht, Netherlands: Springer, 109126.Google Scholar
Holmes, E. A., Mathews, A., Mackintosh, B., and Dalgleish, T. (2008). The Causal Effect of Mental Imagery on Emotion Assessed Using Picture-Word Cues. Emotion, 8(3), 395409. doi:10.1037/1528-3542.8.3.395.Google Scholar
Kanheman, D. (2011). Thinking, Fast and Slow. New York, NY: Farrar, Strauss and Giroux.Google Scholar
Kohlberg, L. (1971). From Is to Ought: How to Commit the Naturalistic Fallacy and Get Away with It in the Study of Moral Development. In Mischel, T (ed.), Cognitive Development and Epistemology. New York, NY: Academic Press, 151235.Google Scholar
Kohlberg, L. (1981). Essays on Moral Development, Vol. I: The Philosophy of Moral Development. San Francisco, CA: Harper & Row.Google Scholar
Leloup, L., Miletich, D. D., Andriet, G., Vandermeeren, Y., and Samson, D. (2016). Cathodal Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation on the Right Temporo-Parietal Junction Modulates the Use of Mitigating Circumstances during Moral Judgments. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 10, 355. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2016.00355.Google Scholar
Lim, D., and DeSteno, D. (2016). Suffering and Compassion: The Links among Adverse Life Experiences, Empathy, Compassion, and Prosocial Behavior. Emotion, 16(2), 175182. doi:10.1037/emo0000144.Google Scholar
Macrae, C. N., Milne, A. B., and Griffiths, R. J. (1993). Counterfactual Thinking and the Perception of Criminal Behaviour. British Journal of Psychology, 84(2), 221226.Google Scholar
Maguire, E. A., and Mullally, S. L. (2013). The Hippocampus: A Manifesto for Change. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 142(4), 11801189. doi:10.1037/a0033650.Google Scholar
Mikhail, J. (2007). Universal Moral Grammar: Theory, Evidence and the Future. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(4), 143152. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2006.12.007.Google Scholar
Morawetz, C., Bode, S., Baudewig, J., and Heekeren, H. R. (2017). Effective Amygdala-Prefrontal Connectivity Predicts Individual Differences in Successful Emotion Regulation. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 12(4), 569585. doi:10.1093/scan/nsw169.Google Scholar
Ochsner, K. N., and Gross, J. J. (2005). The Cognitive Control of Emotion. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(5), 242249. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2005.03.010.Google Scholar
Parham, S. C., Kavanagh, D. J., Shimada, M., May, J., and Andrade, J. (2018). Qualitative Analysis of Feedback on Functional Imagery Training: A Novel Motivational Intervention for Type 2 Diabetes. Journal of Health Psychology, 33(3), 416429. doi:10.1080/08870446.2017.1360493.Google Scholar
Race, E., Keane, M. M., and Verfaellie, M. (2011). Medial Temporal Lobe Damage Causes Deficits in Episodic Memory and Episodic Future Thinking not Attributable to Deficits in Narrative Construction. Journal of Neuroscience, 31(28), 1026210269. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1145-11.2011.Google Scholar
Schilbach, L., Bzdok, D., Timmermans, B., et al. (2012). Introspective Minds: Using ALE Meta-Analyses to Study Commonalities in the Neural Correlates of Emotional Processing, Social and Unconstrained Cognition. PLoS One, 7(2), e30920. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0030920.Google Scholar
Schmidt, M. F., Rakoczy, H., and Tomasello, M. (2012). Young Children Enforce Social Norms Selectively Depending on the Violator’s Group Affiliation. Cognition, 124(3), 325333. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2012.06.004.Google Scholar
Sevinc, G., Gurvit, H., and Spreng, R. N. (2017). Salience Network Engagement with the Detection of Morally Laden Information. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 12(7), 11181127. doi:10.1093/scan/nsx035.Google Scholar
Sheeham, P. W. (1967). A Shortened Form of Betts’ Questionnaire upon Mental Imagery. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 23(3), 386389.Google Scholar
Shulman, G. L., Fiez, J. A., Corbetta, M., et al. (1997). Common Blood Flow Changes across Visual Tasks: II. Decreases in Cerebral Cortex. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 9(5), 648663. doi:10.1162/jocn.1997.9.5.648.Google Scholar
Solbrig, L., Whalley, B., Kavanagh, D. J., et al. (2018). Functional Imagery Training versus Motivational Interviewing for Weight Loss: A Randomised Controlled Trial of Brief Individual Interventions for Overweight and Obesity. International Journal of Obesity. doi:10.1038/s41366-018-0122-1.Google Scholar
Spreng, R. N., Mar, R. A., and Kim, A. S. (2009). The Common Neural Basis of Autobiographical Memory, Prospection, Navigation, Theory of Mind, and the Default Mode: A Quantitative Meta-Analysis. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21(3), 489510. doi:10.1162/jocn.2008.21029.Google Scholar
Sripada, C., Angstadt, M., Kessler, D., et al. (2014). Volitional Regulation of Emotions Produces Distributed Alterations in Connectivity between Visual, Attention Control, and Default Networks. Neuroimage, 89, 110121. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.11.006.Google Scholar
Teigen, K. H., and Jensen, T. K. (2011). Unlucky Victims or Lucky Survivors?: Spontaneous Counterfactual Thinking by Families Exposed to the Tsunami Disaster. European Psychologist, 16(1), 4857.Google Scholar
Thomson, J. J. (1985). The Trolley Problem. Yale Law Journal, 94, 13951415.Google Scholar
Urry, H. L., van Reekum, C. M., Johnstone, T., and Davidson, R. J. (2009). Individual Differences in Some (But Not All) Medial Prefrontal Regions Reflect Cognitive Demand While Regulating Unpleasant Emotion. Neuroimage, 47(3), 852863. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.05.069.Google Scholar
Young, L., Nichols, S., and Saxe, R. (2010). Investigating the Neural and Cognitive Basis of Moral Luck: It’s Not What You Do but What You Know. Review of Philosophy and Psychology, 1(3), 333349. doi:10.1007/s13164-010-0027-y.Google Scholar
Young, L., and Phillips, J. (2011). The Paradox of Moral Focus. Cognition, 119(2), 166178. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2011.01.004.Google Scholar
Young, L., and Saxe, R. (2011). When Ignorance Is no Excuse: Different Roles for Intent across Moral Domains. Cognition, 120(2), 202214. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2011.04.005.Google Scholar
Zeman, A., Dewar, M., and Della Sala, S. (2015). Lives without Imagery – Congenital Aphantasia. Cortex, 73, 378380. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2015.05.019.Google Scholar
Zeman, A., Dewar, M., and Della Sala, S. (2016). Reflections on Aphantasia. Cortex, 74, 336337. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2015.08.015.Google Scholar
Zhan, Y., Xiao, X., Li, J., et al. (2018). Interpersonal Relationship Modulates the Behavioral and Neural Responses during Moral Decision-Making. Neuroscience Letters, 672, 1521. doi:10.1016/j.neulet.2018.02.039.Google Scholar

References

Barbey, A. K. (2018). Network Neuroscience Theory of Human Intelligence. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 22, 820.Google Scholar
Barbey, A. K., Colom, R., Paul, E. J., and Grafman, J. (2014). Architecture of Fluid Intelligence and Working Memory Revealed by Lesion Mapping. Brain Structure & Function, 219(2), 485494.Google Scholar
Bassett, D. S., and Sporns, O. (2017). Network Neuroscience. Nature Neuroscience, 20(3), 353364.Google Scholar
Benoit, R. G., Szpunar, K. K., and Schacter, D. L. (2014). Ventromedial Prefrontal Cortex Supports Affective Future Simulation by Integrating Distributed Knowledge. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 111(46), 1655016555.Google Scholar
Betzel, R. F., Gu, S., Medaglia, J. D., Pasqualetti, F., and Bassett, D. S. (2016). Optimally Controlling the Human Connectome: The Role of Network Topology. Scientific Reports, 6, 30770.Google Scholar
Braun, U., Schäfer, A., Walter, H., et al. (2015). Dynamic Reconfiguration of Frontal Brain Networks during Executive Cognition in Humans. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 112(37), 1167811683.Google Scholar
Bressler, S. L., and Menon, V. (2010). Large-Scale Brain Networks in Cognition: Emerging Methods and Principles. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14(6), 277290.Google Scholar
Bullmore, E., and Sporns, O. (2012). The Economy of Brain Network Organization. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 13, 336349.Google Scholar
Carmichael, S. T., and Price, J. L. (1995). Limbic Connections of the Orbital and Medial Prefrontal Cortex in Macaque Monkeys. The Journal of Comparative Neurology, 363(4), 615641.Google Scholar
Chiong, W., Wilson, S. M., D’Esposito, M., et al. (2013). The Salience Network Causally Influences Default Mode Network Activity during Moral Reasoning. Brain, 136(6), 19291941.Google Scholar
Chuang, C.-C., and Sun, C.-W. (2014). Gender-Related Effects of Prefrontal Cortex Connectivity: A Resting-State Functional Optical Tomography Study. Biomedical Optics Express, 5(8), 25032516.Google Scholar
Ciaramelli, E., Muccioli, M., Làdavas, E., and di Pellegrino, G. (2007). Selective Deficit in Personal Moral Judgment Following Damage to Ventromedial Prefrontal Cortex. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 2(2), 8492.Google Scholar
Cima, M., Tonnaer, F., and Hauser, M. D. (2010). Psychopaths Know Right from Wrong but Don’t Care. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 5(1), 5967.Google Scholar
Cohen, , Jonathan, D. (2005). The Vulcanization of the Human Brain: A Neural Perspective on Interactions between Cognition and Emotion. Journal of Economic Perspectives, 19(4), 324.Google Scholar
Cole, M. W., Bassett, D. S., Power, J. D., Braver, T. S., and Petersen, S. E. (2014). Intrinsic and Task-Evoked Network Architectures of the Human Brain. Neuron, 83(1), 238251.Google Scholar
Cole, M. W., Ito, T., Bassett, D. S., and Schultz, D. H. (2016). Activity Flow over Resting-State Networks Shapes Cognitive Task Activations. Nature Neuroscience, 19(12), 17181726.Google Scholar
Cole, M. W., Yarkoni, T., Repovs, G., Anticevic, A., and Braver, T. S. (2012). Global Connectivity of Prefrontal Cortex Predicts Cognitive Control and Intelligence. Journal of Neuroscience, 32(26), 89888999.Google Scholar
Crockett, M. J. (2013). Models of Morality. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17(8), 363366.Google Scholar
Cushman, F. (2013). Action, Outcome, and Value: A Dual-System Framework for Morality. Personality and Social Psychology Review: An Official Journal of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, Inc, 17(3), 273292.Google Scholar
Daw, N. D., Gershman, S. J., Seymour, B., Dayan, P., and Dolan, R. J. (2011). Model-Based Influences on Humans’ Choices and Striatal Prediction Errors. Neuron, 69(6), 12041215.Google Scholar
Dayan, P. (2012). How to Set the Switches on this Thing. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 22(6), 10681074.Google Scholar
Dayan, P., and Berridge, K. C. (2014). Model-Based and Model-Free Pavlovian Reward Learning: Revaluation, Revision and Revelation. Cognitive, Affective and Behavioral Neuroscience, 14(2), 473492.Google Scholar
Domenech, P., Redouté, J., Koechlin, E., and Dreher, J.-C. (2018). The Neuro-Computational Architecture of Value-Based Selection in the Human Brain. Cerebral Cortex, 28(2), 585601.Google Scholar
Dosenbach, N. U. F., Fair, D. A., Cohen, A. L., Schlaggar, B. L., and Petersen, S. E. (2008). A Dual-Networks Architecture of Top-Down Control. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12(3), 99105.Google Scholar
Economides, M., Kurth-Nelson, Z., Lübbert, A., Guitart-Masip, M., and Dolan, R. J. (2015). Model-Based Reasoning in Humans Becomes Automatic with Training. PLOS Computational Biology, 11(9), e1004463.Google Scholar
Evans, J. S., (2008). Dual-Processing Accounts of Reasoning, Judgment, and Social Cognition. The Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 255278.Google Scholar
Everitt, B. J., Morris, K. A., O’Brien, A., and Robbins, T. W. (1991). The Basolateral Amygdala-Ventral Striatal System and Conditioned Place Preference: Further Evidence of Limbic-Striatal Interactions Underlying Reward-Related Processes. Neuroscience, 42(1), 118.Google Scholar
Friesdorf, R., Conway, P., and Gawronski, B. (2015). Gender Differences in Responses to Moral Dilemmas: A Process Dissociation Analysis. Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin, 41(5), 696713.Google Scholar
Gallos, L. K., Makse, H. A., and Sigman, M. (2012). A Small World of Weak Ties Provides Optimal Global Integration of Self-Similar Modules in Functional Brain Networks. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 109(8), 28252830.Google Scholar
Garrigan, B., Adlam, A. L. R., and Langdon, P. E. (2016). The Neural Correlates of Moral Decision-Making: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis of Moral Evaluations and Response Decision Judgements. Brain and Cognition, 108, 8897.Google Scholar
Gläscher, J., Adolphs, R., Damasio, H., et al. (2012). Lesion Mapping of Cognitive Control and Value-Based Decision Making in the Prefrontal Cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109(36), 1468114686.Google Scholar
Gläscher, J., Daw, N., Dayan, P., and O’Doherty, J. P. (2010). States versus Rewards: Dissociable Neural Prediction Error Signals Underlying Model-Based and Model-Free Reinforcement Learning. Neuron, 66(4), 585595.Google Scholar
Gray, R. T., and Robinson, P. A. (2013). Stability Constraints on Large-Scale Structural Brain Networks. Frontiers in Computational Neuroscience, 7.Google Scholar
Greene, J. D. (2017). The Rat-a-Gorical Imperative: Moral Intuition and the Limits of Affective Learning. Cognition, 167, 6677.Google Scholar
Greene, J. D., Sommerville, R. B., Nystrom, L. E., Darley, J. M., and Cohen, J. D. (2001). An fMRI Investigation of Emotional Engagement in Moral Judgment. Science, 293(5537), 21052108.Google Scholar
Gu, S., Pasqualetti, F., Cieslak, M., et al. (2015). Controllability of Structural Brain Networks. Nature Communications, 6(1), 110.Google Scholar
Hare, T. A., Camerer, C. F., and Rangel, A. (2009). Self-Control in Decision-Making Involves Modulation of the vmPFC Valuation System. Science (New York, N.Y.), 324(5927), 646648.Google Scholar
Harenski, C. L., Antonenko, O., Shane, M. S., and Kiehl, K. A. (2010). A Functional Imaging Investigation of Moral Deliberation and Moral Intuition. NeuroImage, 49(3), 27072716.Google Scholar
Jeurissen, D., Sack, A. T., Roebroeck, A., Russ, B. E., and Pascual-Leone, A. (2014). TMS Affects Moral Judgment, Showing the Role of DLPFC and TPJ in Cognitive and Emotional Processing. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 8.Google Scholar
Kédia, G., Berthoz, S., Wessa, M., Hilton, D., and Martinot, J.-L. (2008). An Agent Harms a Victim: A Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study on Specific Moral Emotions. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20(10), 17881798.Google Scholar
Kerr, D. L., McLaren, D. G., Mathy, R. M., and Nitschke, J. B. (2012). Controllability Modulates the Anticipatory Response in the Human Ventromedial Prefrontal Cortex. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 557.Google Scholar
Koch, K., Pauly, K., Kellermann, T., et al. (2007). Gender Differences in the Cognitive Control of Emotion: An fMRI Study. Neuropsychologia, 45(12), 27442754.Google Scholar
Koenigs, M. (2012). The Role of Prefrontal Cortex in Psychopathy. Reviews in the Neurosciences, 23(3), 253.Google Scholar
Koenigs, M., Kruepke, M., Zeier, J., and Newman, J. P. (2012). Utilitarian Moral Judgment in Psychopathy. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 7(6), 708714.Google Scholar
Koenigs, M., Young, L., Adolphs, R., et al. (2007). Damage to the Prefrontal Cortex Increases Utilitarian Moral Judgements. Nature, 446(7138), 908911.Google Scholar
Kogler, L., Müller, V. I., Seidel, E.-M., et al. (2016). Sex Differences in the Functional Connectivity of the Amygdalae in Association with Cortisol. NeuroImage, 134, 410423.Google Scholar
Levy, D. J., and Glimcher, P. W. (2012). The Root of All Value: A Neural Common Currency for Choice. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 22(6), 10271038.Google Scholar
Medaglia, J. D., Satterthwaite, T. D., Kelkar, A., et al. (2018). Brain State Expression and Transitions Are Related to Complex Executive Cognition in Normative Neurodevelopment. NeuroImage, 166, 293306.Google Scholar
Mendez, M. F., and Shapira, J. S. (2009). Altered Emotional Morality in Frontotemporal Dementia. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 14(3), 165179.Google Scholar
Meunier, D., Lambiotte, R., and Bullmore, E. T. (2010). Modular and Hierarchically Modular Organization of Brain Networks. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 4, 200.Google Scholar
Moll, J., de Oliveira-Souza, R., Zahn, R., and Grafman, J. (2008). The Cognitive Neuroscience of Moral Emotions. In Sinnott-Armstrong, W (ed.), Moral Psychology, Vol 3. The Neuroscience of Morality: Emotion, Brain Disorders, and Development. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 117.Google Scholar
Morgane, P. J., Galler, J. R., and Mokler, D. J. (2005). A Review of Systems and Networks of the Limbic Forebrain/Limbic Midbrain. Progress in Neurobiology, 75(2), 143160.Google Scholar
Motzkin, J. C., Newman, J. P., Kiehl, K. A., and Koenigs, M. (2011). Reduced Prefrontal Connectivity in Psychopathy. Journal of Neuroscience, 31(48), 1734817357.Google Scholar
Østby, Y., Walhovd, K., Tamnes, C., et al. (2012). Mental Time Travel and Default-Mode Network Functional Connectivity in the Developing Brain. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 109, 1680016804.Google Scholar
Pascual, L., Rodrigues, P., and Gallardo-Pujol, D. (2013). How Does Morality Work in the Brain? A Functional and Structural Perspective of Moral Behavior. Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience, 7, 65.Google Scholar
Pessoa, L. (2008). On the Relationship between Emotion and Cognition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 9(2), 148158.Google Scholar
Polanía, R., Moisa, M., Opitz, A., Grueschow, M., and Ruff, C. C. (2015). The Precision of Value-Based Choices Depends Causally on Fronto-Parietal Phase Coupling. Nature Communications, 6, 8090.Google Scholar
Power, J. D., Cohen, A. L., Nelson, S. M., et al. (2011). Functional Network Organization of the Human Brain. Neuron, 72(4), 665678.Google Scholar
Raine, A., and Yang, Y. (2006). Neural Foundations to Moral Reasoning and Antisocial Behavior. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 1(3), 203213.Google Scholar
Rajmohan, V., and Mohandas, E. (2007). The Limbic System. Indian Journal of Psychiatry, 49(2), 132139.Google Scholar
Rangel, A., and Hare, T. (2010). Neural Computations Associated with Goal-Directed Choice. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 20(2), 262270.Google Scholar
Roy, M., Shohamy, D., and Wager, T. (2012). Ventromedial Prefrontal-Subcortical Systems and the Generation of Affective Meaning. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16(3), 147–156.Google Scholar
Schneider, K., Pauly, K. D., Gossen, A., et al. (2013). Neural Correlates of Moral Reasoning in Autism Spectrum Disorder. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 8(6), 702710.Google Scholar
Sescousse, G., Caldú, X., Segura, B., and Dreher, J.-C. (2013). Processing of Primary and Secondary Rewards: A Quantitative Meta-Analysis and Review of Human Functional Neuroimaging Studies. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 37(4).Google Scholar
Shenhav, A., and Greene, J. D. (2014). Integrative Moral Judgment: Dissociating the Roles of the Amygdala and Ventromedial Prefrontal Cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 34(13), 47414749.Google Scholar
Shine, J. M., Bissett, P. G., Bell, P.T., et al. (2016). The Dynamics of Functional Brain Networks: Integrated Network States during Cognitive Task Performance. Neuron, 92(2), 544554.Google Scholar
Sporns, O., Tononi, G., and Edelman, G. M. (2000). Theoretical Neuroanatomy: Relating Anatomical and Functional Connectivity in Graphs and Cortical Connection Matrices. Cerebral Cortex, 10(2), 127141.Google Scholar
Spreng, R. N., Stevens, W. D., Chamberlain, J. P., Gilmore, A. W., and Schacter, D. L. (2010). Default Network Activity, Coupled with the Frontoparietal Control Network, Supports Goal-Directed Cognition. NeuroImage, 53(1), 303317.Google Scholar
Thomson, J. J. (1985). The Trolley Problem. The Yale Law Journal, 94(6), 13951415.Google Scholar
Tomasi, D., and Volkow, N. D. (2012). Gender Differences in Brain Functional Connectivity Density. Human Brain Mapping, 33(4), 849860.Google Scholar
van den Heuvel, M. P., Stam, C. J., Kahn, R. S., and Hulshoff Pol, H. E. (2009). Efficiency of Functional Brain Networks and Intellectual Performance. Journal of Neuroscience, 29(23), 76197624.Google Scholar
Vincent, J. L., Kahn, I., Snyder, A. Z., Raichle, M. E., and Buckner, R. L. (2008). Evidence for a Frontoparietal Control System Revealed by Intrinsic Functional Connectivity. Journal of Neurophysiology, 100(6), 33283342.Google Scholar
Welborn, B. L., Papademetris, X., Reis, D. L., et al. (2009). Variation in Orbitofrontal Cortex Volume: Relation to Sex, Emotion Regulation and Affect. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 4(4), 328339.Google Scholar
Wunderlich, K., Dayan, P., and Dolan, R. J. (2012). Mapping Value Based Planning and Extensively Trained Choice in the Human Brain. Nature Neuroscience, 15(5), 786791.Google Scholar
Yeo, B. T. T., Krienen, F. M., Eickhoff, S. B., et al. (2015). Functional Specialization and Flexibility in Human Association Cortex. Cerebral Cortex (New York, NY), 25(10), 36543672.Google Scholar
Yeo, B. T. T., Krienen, F. M., Sepulcre, J., et al. (2011). The Organization of the Human Cerebral Cortex Estimated by Intrinsic Functional Connectivity. Journal of Neurophysiology, 106(3), 11251165.Google Scholar
Young, L., and Dungan, J. (2012). Where in the Brain is Morality? Everywhere and Maybe Nowhere. Social Neuroscience, 7(1), 110.Google Scholar
Young, L., and Saxe, R. (2008). The Neural Basis of Belief Encoding and Integration in Moral Judgment. NeuroImage, 40(4), 19121920.Google Scholar

References

Ainslie, G. (1975). Specious Reward: A Behavioral Theory of Impulsiveness and Impulse Control. Psychological Bulletin, 82, 463496.Google Scholar
Balcomb, F. K., and Gerken, L. A. (2008). Three-Year-Old Children Can Access Their Own Memory to Guide Responses on a Visual Matching Task. Developmental Science, 11, 750760.Google Scholar
Barnier, A. J., Sutton, J., Harris, C. B., and Wilson, R. A. (2008). A Conceptual and Empirical Framework for the Social Distribution of Cognition: The Case of Memory. Cognitive Systems Research, 9, 3351.Google Scholar
Barsalou, L. W. (2009). Simulation, Situated Conceptualization, and Prediction. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 364, 12811289.Google Scholar
Bateson, M., Brilot, B., and Nettle, D. (2011). Anxiety: An Evolutionary Approach. The Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 56, 707715.Google Scholar
Baumeister, R. F., and Tierney, J. (2011). Willpower: Rediscovering the Greatest Human Strength. New York, NY: Penguin Press.Google Scholar
Baxter, W. T. (1989). Early Accounting: The Tally and the Checkerboard. The Accounting Historians Journal, 16, 4383.Google Scholar
Beck, S. R., Robinson, E. J., Carroll, D. J., and Apperly, I. A. (2006). Children’s Thinking about Counterfactuals and Future Hypotheticals as Possibilities. Child Development, 77, 413426.Google Scholar
Berns, G. S., Chappelow, J., Cekic, M., et al. (2006). Neurobiological Substrates of Dread. Science, 312, 754758.Google Scholar
Bischof-Köhler, D. (1985). Zur Phylogenese menschlicher Motivation. In Eckensberger, L. H, Baltes, M. M (eds.), Emotion und Reflexivität. Munich, Germany: Urban & Schwarzenberg, 347.Google Scholar
Bjork, R. A. (1994). Memory and Metamemory Considerations in the Training of Human Beings. In Metcalfe, J and Shimamura, A (eds.), Metacognition: Knowing about Knowing. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 185205.Google Scholar
Boyer, P. (2008). Evolutionary Economics of Mental Time Travel? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12, 219224.Google Scholar
Brandimonte, M. A., Einstein, G. O., and McDaniel, M. A. (2014). Prospective Memory: Theory and Applications. New York, NY: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Bulley, A. (2018). The History and Future of Human Prospection. Evolutionary Studies in Imaginative Culture, 1, 7593.Google Scholar
Bulley, A., Henry, J., and Suddendorf, T. (2016). Prospection and the Present Moment: The Role of Episodic Foresight in Intertemporal Choices between Immediate and Delayed Rewards. Review of General Psychology, 20, 2947.Google Scholar
Bulley, A., Henry, J., and Suddendorf, T. (2017). Thinking about Threats: Memory and Prospection in Human Threat Management. Consciousness and Cognition, 49, 5369.Google Scholar
Byrne, R. M. J. (2016). Counterfactual Thought. Annual Review of Psychology, 67, 135157.Google Scholar
Clark, A. (2006). Language, Embodiment, and the Cognitive Niche. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10, 370374.Google Scholar
Clark, A. (2008). Supersizing the Mind: Embodiment, Action, and Cognitive Extension. Oxford University Press. doi:10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195333213.001.0001.Google Scholar
Clark, A. (2015). Surfing Uncertainty: Prediction, Action, and the Embodied Mind. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Clark, A., and Chalmers, D. (1998). The Extended Mind. Analysis, 58, 719.Google Scholar
Clarke, K. C. (2013). What is the World’s Oldest Map? The Cartographic Journal, 50, 136143.Google Scholar
Coffman, D. D. (1990). Effects of Mental Practice, Physical Practice, and Knowledge of Results on Piano Performance. Journal of Research in Music Education, 38(3), 187196.Google Scholar
Corballis, M. C. (2013). Wandering Tales: Evolutionary Origins of Mental Time Travel and Language. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 485.Google Scholar
Craik, K. J. W. (1943). The Nature of Explanation. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
D’Argembeau, A. (2016). The Role of Personal Goals in Future-Oriented Mental Time Travel. In Michaelian, K, Klein, S. B., and Szpunar, K. K. (eds.), Seeing the Future: Theoretical Perspectives on Future-Oriented Mental Time Travel. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. doi:10.1093/acprof:oso/9780190241537.003.0010.Google Scholar
Damasio, A. R. (1994). Descartes’ Error: Emotion, Reason, and the Human Brain. New York, NY: Putnam Publishing.Google Scholar
Davis, J. T. M., Cullen, E., and Suddendorf, T. (2016 ). Understanding Deliberate Practice in Preschool Aged Children. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 69(2), 361380.Google Scholar
Dawkins, R. (1998). Unweaving the Rainbow: Science, Delusion and the Appetite for Wonder. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin Harcourt.Google Scholar
Devereux, C. L., Whittingham, M. J., Fernández-Juricic, E., Vickery, J. A., and Krebs, J. R. (2006). Predator Detection and Avoidance by Starlings under Differing Scenarios of Predation Risk. Behavioral Ecology, 17, 303309.Google Scholar
Dohrn-van Rossum, G. (1996). History of the Hour: Clocks and Modern Temporal Orders. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Donald, M. (1991). Origins of the Modern Mind: Three Stages in the Evolution of Culture and Cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Dror, I., and Harnad, S. (2008). Offloading Cognition onto Cognitive Technology. Amsterdam, Netherlands: JohnBenjamins Publishing.Google Scholar
Duckworth, A. L., Gendler, T. S., and Gross, J. J. (2016). Situational Strategies for Self-Control. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 11, 3555.Google Scholar
Dufresne, A., and Kobasigawa, A. (1989). Children’s Spontaneous Allocation of Study Time: Differential and Sufficient Aspects. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 47, 274296.Google Scholar
Dunlosky, J., and Tauber, S. K. (2016). The Oxford Handbook of Metamemory. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Eiseley, L. C. (1970). The Invisible Pyramid. Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
Elster, J. (2000). Ulysses Unbound: Studies in Rationality, Precommitment, and Constraints. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Fawcett, T. W., McNamara, J. M., and Houston, A. I. (2012). When Is it Adaptive to Be Patient? A General Framework for Evaluating Delayed Rewards. Behavioural Processes, 89, 128136.Google Scholar
Finnbogadóttir, H., and Berntsen, D. (2013). Involuntary Future Projections Are as Frequent as Involuntary Memories, but More Positive. Consciousness and Cognition, 22, 272280.Google Scholar
Fischer, S. R. (2001). A History of Writing. London, UK: Reaktion Books.Google Scholar
Flavell, J. H., and Wellman, H. M. (1977). Metamemory. In Kail, R. V, Jr., and Hagen, J (eds.), Perspectives on the Development of Memory and Cognition. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 333.Google Scholar
Frankfurt, H. G. (1988). Freedom of the Will and the Concept of a Person. In Goodman, M. F. (ed.), What Is a Person? Clifton, NJ: Humana Press: 127144.Google Scholar
Gaesser, B., and Schacter, D. L. (2014). Episodic Simulation and Episodic Memory Can Increase Intentions to Help Others. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 111, 44154420.Google Scholar
Gilbert, D. T., and Wilson, T. D. (2007). Prospection: Experiencing the Future. Science, 317, 13511354.Google Scholar
Gullo, M. J., Dawe, S., Kambouropoulos, N., Staiger, P. K., and Jackson, C. J. (2010). Alcohol Expectancies and Drinking Refusal Self‐Efficacy Mediate the Association of Impulsivity with Alcohol Misuse. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 34, 13861399.Google Scholar
Hallos, J. (2005). “15 Minutes Of Fame”: Exploring the Temporal Dimension of Middle Pleistocene Lithic Technology. Journal of Human Evolution, 49(2), 155179.Google Scholar
Hassabis, D., and Maguire, E. A. (2009). The Construction System of the Brain. Philosophical Transactions: Biological Sciences, 364, 12631271.Google Scholar
Heersmink, R. (2013). A Taxonomy of Cognitive Artifacts: Function, Information, and Categories. Review of Philosophy and Psychology, 4, 465481.Google Scholar
Heersmink, R. (2016). The Cognitive Integration of Scientific Instruments: Information, Situated Cognition, and Scientific Practice. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 15, 517537.Google Scholar
Hesslow, G. (2012). The Current Status of the Simulation Theory of Cognition. Brain Research, 1428, 7179.Google Scholar
Heyes, C. (2016). Who Knows? Metacognitive Social Learning Strategies. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, xx, 110.Google Scholar
Hirst, W., and Echterhoff, G. (2012). Remembering in Conversations: The Social Sharing and Reshaping of Memories. Annual Review of Psychology, 63, 5579.Google Scholar
Hoerl, C., and McCormack, T. (2018). Thinking in and About Time: A Dual Systems Perspective on Temporal Cognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 177. doi:10.1017/S0140525X18002157.Google Scholar
Hutchins, E. (1999). Cognitive Artifacts. The MIT Encyclopedia of the Cognitive Sciences, 126, 127.Google Scholar
Irish, M., and Piguet, O. (2013). The Pivotal Role of Semantic Memory in Remembering the Past and Imagining the Future. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 7, 27.Google Scholar
Jones, A. (2007). Memory and Material Culture. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Kelly, L. (2015). Knowledge and Power in Prehistoric Societies: Orality, Memory, and the Transmission of Culture. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Kelly, L. (2017). The Memory Code: The Secrets of Stonehenge, Easter Island and Other Ancient Monuments. New York, NY: Pegasus Books. books.google.com.au/books?id=dfa8DAEACAAJ.Google Scholar
Kidd, C., Palmeri, H., and Aslin, R. N. (2013). Rational Snacking: Young Children’s Decision-Making on the Marshmallow Task Is Moderated by Beliefs about Environmental Reliability. Cognition, 126, 109114.Google Scholar
Kirby, K. N., Petry, N. M., and Bickel, W. K. (1999). Heroin Addicts Have Higher Discount Rates for Delayed Rewards than Non-Drug-Using Controls. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 128, 7887.Google Scholar
Klein, S. B. (2013). The Temporal Orientation of Memory: It’s Time for a Change of Direction. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 2, 222234.Google Scholar
Koffarnus, M. N., Jarmolowicz, D. P., Mueller, E. T., and Bickel, W. K. (2013). Changing Delay Discounting in the Light of the Competing Neurobehavioral Decision Systems Theory: A Review. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 99, 3257.Google Scholar
Köhler, W. (1925). The Mentality of Apes. New York, NY: Read Books Ltd.Google Scholar
Kohn, M., and Mithen, S. (1999). Handaxes: Products of Sexual Selection? Antiquity, 73, 518526.Google Scholar
Legare, C. H., and Nielsen, M. (2015). Imitation and Innovation: The Dual Engines of Cultural Learning. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 19, 688699.Google Scholar
Lepre, C. J., Roche, H., Kent, D. V., et al. (2011). An Earlier Origin for the Acheulian. Nature, 477(7362), 8285.Google Scholar
Loewenstein, G. F., and Lerner, J. (2003). The Role of Affect in Decision-Making. In Davidson, R, Goldsmith, H, and Scherer, K (ed.), Handbook of Affective Science. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press, 619642.Google Scholar
Loewenstein, G. F., Read, D., and Baumeister, R. F. (2003). Time and Decision: Economic and Psychological Perspectives of Intertemporal Choice. New York, NY: Russell Sage Foundation. books.google.com.au/books?id=iHJcQJcg3BICx.Google Scholar
Machin, A. J. (2008). Why Handaxes Just Aren’t That Sexy: A Response to Kohn & Mithen (1999). Antiquity, 82(317), 761766.Google Scholar
Meece, S. (2006). A Bird’s Eye View – of a Leopard’s Spots: The Çatalhöyük “Map” and the Development of Cartographic Representation in Prehistory. Anatolian Studies, 56, 116.Google Scholar
Mellaart, J. (1967). Çatal Hüyük: a Neolithic Town in Anatolia. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Michaelian, K., and Sutton, J. (2013). Distributed Cognition and Memory Research: History and Current Directions. Review of Philosophy and Psychology, 4, 124.Google Scholar
Miloyan, B., Bulley, A., and Suddendorf, T. (2016). Episodic Foresight and Anxiety: Proximate and Ultimate Perspectives. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 55(1), 422.Google Scholar
Miloyan, B., Bulley, A., and Suddendorf, T. (2019). Anxiety: Here and Beyond. Emotion Review, 11(1), 3949.Google Scholar
Mischel, W. (2014). The Marshmallow Test: Understanding Self-Control and How to Master it. New York, NY: Random House.Google Scholar
Mischel, W., Shoda, Y., and Rodriguez, M. I. (1989). Delay of Gratification in Children. Science, 244, 933938.Google Scholar
Mithen, S. J. (1996). The Prehistory of the Mind: a Search for the Origins of Art, Religion and Science. London, UK: Weidenfeld & Nicholson.Google Scholar
Müller, C. P., and Schumann, G. (2011). Drugs as Instruments: A New Framework for Non-Addictive Psychoactive Drug Use. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 34, 293310.Google Scholar
Neldner, K., Collier-Baker, E., and Nielsen, M. (2015). Chimpanzees (Pan Troglodytes) and Human Children (Homo Sapiens) Know When They Are Ignorant about the Location of Food. Animal Cognition, 18, 683699.Google Scholar
Nelson, T., and Narens, L. (1990). Metamemory: A Theoretical Framework and New Findings. Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 26, 125173.Google Scholar
Nestojko, J. F., Finley, J. R., and Roediger, H. L. (2013). Extending Cognition to External Agents. Psychological Inquiry, 24, 321325.Google Scholar
Oakley, K. P. (1981). Emergence of Higher Thought 3.0-0.2 Ma B.P. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 292, 205211.Google Scholar
Palermos, S. O. (2016). The Dynamics of Group Cognition. Minds and Machines, 26, 409440.Google Scholar
Parfit, D. (1971). Personal Identity. The Philosophical Review, 80, 327.Google Scholar
Perner, J. (1991). Learning, Development, and Conceptual Change. Understanding the Representational Mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Peters, J., and Büchel, C. (2010). Episodic Future Thinking Reduces Reward Delay Discounting through an Enhancement of Prefrontal-Mediotemporal Interactions. Neuron, 66, 138148.Google Scholar
Pezzulo, G. (2008). Coordinating with the Future: The Anticipatory Nature of Representation. Minds and Machines, 18, 179225.Google Scholar
Pezzulo, G., and Rigoli, F. (2011). The Value of Foresight: How Prospection Affects Decision-Making. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 5, 79.Google Scholar
Popper, K. (1934). The Logic of Scientific Discovery. London, UK: Routledge.Google Scholar
Pylyshyn, Z. W. (1978). When Is Attribution of Beliefs Justified? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1(4), 592593.Google Scholar
Rachlin, H. (1995). Self-Control: Beyond Commitment. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 18(1), 109121.Google Scholar
Rachlin, H. (2016). Self-Control Based on Soft Commitment. Behavior Analyst, 39, 259268.Google Scholar
Rafetseder, E., Cristi-Vargas, R., and Perner, J. (2010). Counterfactual Reasoning: Developing a Sense of “Nearest Possible World”. Child Development, 81, 376389.Google Scholar
Redshaw, J. (2014). Does Metarepresentation Make Human Mental Time Travel Unique? Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 5(5), 519531.Google Scholar
Redshaw, J., and Bulley, A. (2018). Future-Thinking in Animals Capacities and Limits. In Oettingen, G, Sevincer, A. T., and Gollwitzer, P (eds.), The Psychology of Thinking about the Future. New York, NY: Guilford Press, 3151.Google Scholar
Redshaw, J., and Suddendorf, T. (2016). Children’s and Apes’ Preparatory Responses to Two Mutually Exclusive Possibilities. Current Biology, 26(13), 17581762.Google Scholar
Redshaw, J., Suddendorf, T., Neldner, K., et al. (2019). Young Children From Three Diverse Cultures Spontaneously and Consistently Prepare for Alternative Future Possibilities. Child Development, 90(1), 5161.Google Scholar
Redshaw, J., Vandersee, J., Bulley, A., and Gilbert, S. J. (2018). Development of Children’s Use of External Reminders for Hard-to-Remember Intentions. Child Development, 89( 6), 2099–2118.Google Scholar
Risko, E. F., and Gilbert, S. J. (2016). Cognitive Offloading. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 20, 676688.Google Scholar
Rorty, A. O. (1980). Where Does the Akratic Break Take Place? Australasian Journal of Philosophy, 58, 333346.Google Scholar
Santos, L. R., and Rosati, A. G. (2015). The Evolutionary Roots of Human Decision-Making. Annual Review of Psychology, 66, 321347.Google Scholar
Schacter, D. L., Addis, D. R., and Buckner, R. L. (2008). Episodic Simulation of Future Events: Concepts, Data, and Applications. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1124, 3960.Google Scholar
Schacter, D. L., Addis, D. R., Hassabis, D., et al. (2012). The Future of Memory: Remembering, Imagining, and the Brain. Neuron, 76, 677694.Google Scholar
Schacter, D. L., Benoit, R. G., De Brigard, F., and Szpunar, K. K. (2015). Episodic Future Thinking and Episodic Counterfactual Thinking: Intersections between Memory and Decisions. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 117, 1421.Google Scholar
Schacter, D. L., Benoit, R. G., and Szpunar, K. K. (2017). Episodic Future Thinking: Mechanisms and Functions. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 17, 4150.Google Scholar
Schelling, T. C. (1960). The Strategy of Conflict. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Schelling, T. C. (1984). Self-Command in Practice, in Policy, and in a Theory of Rational Choice. The American Economic Review, 74(2), 111.Google Scholar
Seli, P., Kane, M. J., Smallwood, J., et al. (2018). Mind-Wandering as a Natural Kind: A Family-Resemblances View. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 22, 479490.Google Scholar
Shipton, C., and Nielsen, M. (2015). Before Cumulative Culture: The Evolutionary Origins of Overimitation and Shared Intentionality. Human Nature, 26, 331345.Google Scholar
Smith, C. D. (1987). Cartography in the Prehistoric Period in the Old World: Europe, the Middle East, and North Africa. The History of Cartography, 1, 54102.Google Scholar
Sterelny, K. (2010). Minds: Extended or Scaffolded? Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 9, 465481.Google Scholar
Stevens, J. R., and Stephens, D. (2008). Patience. Current Biology: CB, 18, R11–2.Google Scholar
Suddendorf, T. (1994). Discovery of the Fourth Dimension: Mental Time Travel and Human Evolution. Waikato, New Zealand: University of Waikato, PhD thesis. cogprints.org/729/1/THESIS.txt.Google Scholar
Suddendorf, T. (1999). The Rise of the Metamind. In Corballis, M and Lee, S (eds.), The Descent of Mind: Psychological Perspectives on Hominid Evolution. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press, 218260.Google Scholar
Suddendorf, T. (2010). Episodic Memory versus Episodic Foresight: Similarities and Differences. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 1, 99107.Google Scholar
Suddendorf, T. (2013). The Gap: The Science of What Separates us from Other Animals. New York, NY: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Suddendorf, T., Brinums, M., and Imuta, K. (2016). Shaping One’s Future Self – The Development of Deliberate Practice. In Michaelian, K, Klein, S. B., and Szpunar, K. K. (eds.), Seeing the Future: Theoretical Perspectives on Future-Oriented Mental Time Travel. New York, NY: Oxford University Press, 343366.Google Scholar
Suddendorf, T., and Corballis, M. C. (1997). Mental Time Travel and the Evolution of the Human Mind. Genetic Social and General Psychology Monographs, 123, 133167.Google Scholar
Suddendorf, T., and Corballis, M. C. (2007). The Evolution of Foresight: What Is Mental Time Travel, and Is it Unique to Humans? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 30, 299351.Google Scholar
Suddendorf, T., Crimston, J., and Redshaw, J. (2017). Preparatory Responses to Socially Determined, Mutually Exclusive Possibilities in Chimpanzees and Children. Biology Letters, 13.Google Scholar
Sutton, J. (2006). Exaograms and Interdisciplinarity: History, the Extended Mind, and the Civilizing Process. In Menary, R (ed.), The Extended Mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 189225.Google Scholar
Szpunar, K. K. (2010). Episodic Future Thought: An Emerging Concept. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 5, 142162.Google Scholar
Thaler, R. H., and Shefrin, H. (1981). An Economic Theory of Self-Control. Journal of Political Economy, 89(2), 392406.Google Scholar
Utrilla, P., Mazo, C., Sopena, M. C., Martínez-Bea, M., and Domingo, R. (2009). A Palaeolithic Map from 13,660 calBP: Engraved Stone Blocks from the Late Magdalenian in Abauntz Cave (Navarra, Spain). Journal of Human Evolution, 57, 99111.Google Scholar
Whiten, A., and Erdal, D. (2012). The Human Socio-Cognitive Niche and its Evolutionary Origins. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 367, 21192129.Google Scholar
Woodward, D., and Harley, J. B. (1987). The History of Cartography, Volume 1: Cartography in Prehistoric, Ancient, and Medieval Europe and the Mediterranean. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×