Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-lrblm Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-01-08T22:59:13.773Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

17 - Reaction Time Measures

from Part IV - Behavioral Measures

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  12 December 2024

John E. Edlund
Affiliation:
Rochester Institute of Technology, New York
Austin Lee Nichols
Affiliation:
Central European University, Vienna
Get access

Summary

The term reaction time (RT) describes the interval between the initial appearance of a stimulus and an organism’s response to that stimulus. Because RT data are notorious for substantial individual differences and positive skew, the overarching goal of this chapter is to equip researchers with sufficient knowledge to design RT studies that enhance experimental control, thereby reducing unwanted error variability. To accomplish this goal, the chapter discusses five major themes: (1) an overview of the evolution of RT research; (2) a discussion of the state of RT knowledge today, with a focus on moderating variables that researchers could control; (3) a review of some of the most common RT measures employed by contemporary social and behavioral scientists; (4) a description of specific technological tools that can be used to administer those measures; and (5) a discussion of basic considerations for statistical analysis of RT data.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2024

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Ando, S., Kida, N., & Oda, S. (2002). Practice effects on reaction time for peripheral and central visual fields. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 95(3), 747752. http://dx.doi.org/10.2466/pms.2002.95.3.747CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ando, S., Kida, N., & Oda, S. (2004). Retention of practice effects on simple reaction time for peripheral and central visual fields. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 98(3), 897900. http://dx.doi.org/10.2466/PMS.98.3.897-900CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Arent, S. M., & Landers, D. M. (2003). Arousal, anxiety, and performance: A reexamination of the inverted-U hypothesis. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 74(4), 436444. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02701367.2003.10609113CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Atkinson, R. C., & Shiffrin, R. M. (1968). Human memory: A proposed system and its control processes. In Spence, K. W. & Spence, J. T., The Psychology of Learning and Motivation (vol. 2, pp. 89195). Academic Press.Google Scholar
Babkoff, H., Genser, S., & Hegge, F. W. (1985). Lexical decision, parafoveal eccentricity and visual hemifield. Cortex, 21(4), 581593. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0010-9452(58)80006-4CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baghdadi, G., Towhidkhah, F., & Rajabi, M. (2021). Neurocognitive Mechanisms of Attention. Elsevier.Google Scholar
Boring, E. G. (1950). A History of Experimental Psychology, 2nd ed. Appleton-Century-Crofts.Google Scholar
Brendl, C. M., Markman, A. B., & Messner, C. (2001). How do indirect measures of evaluation work? Evaluating the inference of prejudice in the Implicit Association Test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81(5), 760773. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.81.5.760CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cedrus Corporation. (2022). Download SuperLab [webpage]. https://cedrus.com/superlab/download.htmGoogle Scholar
Damanpak, S., Mokhtari, P., & Mousavi, S. M. V. (2014). Relationship between arousal and choice reaction time. Biosciences Biotechnology Research Asia, 11(2), 803806. http://dx.doi.org/10.13005/bbra/1341CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Danek, R. H., & Mordkoff, J. T. (2011). Unequal motor durations under simple-, Go/No-go, and choice-RT tasks: Extension of Miller and Low (2001). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 37(4), 13231329. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0023092Google ScholarPubMed
de Leeuw, J. R., & Motz, B. A. (2016). Psychophysics in a Web browser? Comparing response times collected with JavaScript and Psychophysics Toolbox in a visual search task. Behavior Research Methods, 48(1), 112. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/s13428-015-0567-2CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Delwiche, J. F., Halpern, B. P., & Desimone, J. A. (1999). Anion size of sodium salts and simple taste reaction times. Physiology & Behavior, 66(1), 2732. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0031-9384(98)00273-XCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Der, G., & Deary, I. J. (2006). Age and sex differences in reaction time in adulthood: Results from the United Kingdom Health and Lifestyle Survey. Psychology and Aging, 21(1), 6273. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.21.1.62CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dovidio, J. F., Kawakami, K., & Gaertner, S. L. (2002). Implicit and explicit prejudice and interracial interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82(1), 6268. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.82.1.62CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dovidio, J. F., Kawakami, K., Johnson, C., Johnson, B., & Howard, A. (1997). On the nature of prejudice: Automatic and controlled processes. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 33(5), 510540. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/jesp.1997.1331CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Durlach, P. J., Edmunds, R., Howard, L., & Tipper, S. P. (2002). A rapid effect of caffeinated beverages on two choice reaction time tasks. Nutritional Neuroscience 5(6), 433442. https://doi.org/10.1080/1028415021000039211CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dutilh, G., Vandekerckhove, J., Tuerlinckx, F., & Wagenmakers, E.-J. (2009). A diffusion model decomposition of the practice effect. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 16(6), 10261036. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/16.6.1026CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dykiert, D., Der, G., Starr, J. M., & Deary, I. J. (2012). Age differences in intra-individual variability in simple and choice reaction time: Systematic review and meta-analysis. PLOS ONE, 7(10). https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0045759CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Elliott, E. M., Morey, C. C., Morey, R. D., Eaves, S. D., Shelton, J. T., & Lutfi-Proctor, D. A. (2014). The role of modality: Auditory and visual distractors in Stroop interference. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 26(1), 1526. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/20445911.2013.859133CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Empirisoft Corporation. (2020a). v2020 Downloads [DirectRT 2020.1.111]. www.empirisoft.com/download.aspxGoogle Scholar
Empirisoft Corporation. (2020b). v2020 Downloads [MediaLab 2020.1.111]. www.empirisoft.com/download.aspxGoogle Scholar
Epifania, O. M., Robusto, E., & Anselmi, P. (2023). Is the performance at the Implicit Association Test sensitive to feedback presentation? A Rasch-based analysis. Psychological Research, 87, 737750. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-022-01703-wCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eriksen, B. A., & Eriksen, C. W. (1974). Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task. Perception and Psychophysics, 16(1), 143149. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03203267CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eriksen, C. W., & St. James, J. D. (1986). Visual attention within and around the field of focal attention: A zoom lens model. Perception and Psychophysics, 40(4), 225240. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03211502CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Evans, N. J., Brown, S. D., Mewhort, D. J. K., & Heathcote, A. (2018). Refining the law of practice. Psychological Review, 125(4), 592605. https://doi.org/10.1037/rev0000105CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Exner, S. (1873). Experimentelle Untersuchung der einfachsten psychischen Processe: Erste Abhandlung. Archiv für die Gesammte Physiologie des Menschen und der Thiere, 7, 601660. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01613351CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fazio, R. H., Jackson, J. R., Dunton, B. C., & Williams, C. J. (1995). Variability in automatic activation as an unobtrusive measure of racial attitudes: A bona fide pipeline? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69(6), 10131027. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.69.6.1013CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fazio, R. H., Sanbonmatsu, D. M., Powell, M. C., & Kardes, F. R. (1986). On the automatic activation of attitudes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50(2), 229238. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.50.2.229CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fontani, G., Lodi, L., Felici, A., Migliorini, S., & Corradeschi, F. (2006). Attention in athletes of high and low experience engaged in different open skill sports. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 102(3), 791805. http://dx.doi.org/10.2466/PMS.102.3.791-805CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gorus, E., De Raedt, R., Lambert, M., Lemper, J.-C., & Mets, T. (2008). Reaction times and performance variability in normal aging, mild cognitive impairment, and Alzheimer’s disease. Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry and Neurology, 21(3), 204218.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gotlib, I. H., & McCann, C. D. (1984). Construct accessibility and depression: An examination of cognitive and affective factors. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47(2), 427439. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.47.2.427CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Green, D. M., & Swets, J. A. (1966). Signal Detection Theory and Psychophysics. John Wiley.Google Scholar
Greenwald, A. G., & Farnham, S. D. (2000). Using the Implicit Association Test to measure self-esteem and self-concept. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79(6), 10221038. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.79.6.1022CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E., & Schwartz, J. L. K. (1998). Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: The Implicit Association Test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(6), 14641480. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.74.6.1464CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gyurak, A., Gross, J. J., & Etkin, A. (2011). Explicit and implicit emotion regulation: A dual-process framework. Cognition and Emotion, 25(3), 400412. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2010.544160CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harrison, C., Lim, B. Y., Shick, A., & Hudson, S. E. (2009, April). Where to locate wearable displays? Reaction time performance of visual alerts from tip to toe. In Chi ’09: Proceedings of the SIGCHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems (pp. 941944). ACM. https://doi.org/10.1145/1518701.1518845Google Scholar
Henrich, J., Heine, S. J., & Norenzayan, A. (2010). The weirdest people in the world? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33(23), 6183. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X0999152XCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hergenhahn, B. R. (2013). An Introduction to the History of Psychology, 7th ed. Wadsworth.Google Scholar
Hernandez, S., Nieto, A., & Barroso, J. (1992). Hemispheric specialization for word classes with visual presentations and lexical decision task. Brain and Cognition, 20(2), 399408. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0278-2626(92)90029-LCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hervey, A. S., Epstein, J. N., Curry, J. F., Tonev, S., Arnold, L. E., Conners, C. K., et al. (2006). Reaction time distribution analysis of neuropsychological performance in an ADHD sample. Child Neuropsychology, 12(2), 125140. https://doi.org/10.1080/09297040500499081CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hick, W. E. (1952). On the rate of gain of information. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 4, 1126. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17470215208416600CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Howse, D. (1989). Nevil Maskelyne: The Seaman’s Astronomer. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Jaśkowski, P., & Włodarczyk, D. (1997). Effect of sleep deficit, knowledge of results, and stimulus quality on reaction time and response force. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 84(2), 563572. http://dx.doi.org/10.2466/pms.1997.84.2.563CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johns, M., Crowley, K., Chapman, R., Tucker, A., & Hocking, C. (2009). The effect of blinks and saccadic eye movements on visual reaction times. Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics, 71(4), 783788. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/APP.71.4.783CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jones, E. E., & Sigall, H. (1971). The bogus pipeline: A new paradigm for measuring affect and attitude. Psychological Bulletin, 76(5), 349364. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0031617CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kling, J. W., & Riggs, L. A. (1971). Woodworth & Schlosberg’s experimental psychology, 3rd ed. Holt, Rinehart, & Winston.Google Scholar
Krizan, Z., & Suls, J. (2008). Are implicit and explicit measures of self-esteem related? A meta-analysis for the Name-Letter test. Personality and Individual Differences, 44(2), 521531. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2007.09.017CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lavoie, M. E., Dupuis, F., Johnston, K. M., Leclerc, S., & Lassonde, M. (2004). Visual P300 effects beyond symptoms in concussed college athletes. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 26(1), 5573. http://dx.doi.org/10.1076/jcen.26.1.55.23936CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levitt, S., & Gutin, B. (1971). Multiple choice reaction time and movement time during physical exertion. Research Quarterly of the American Association for Health, Physical Education, and Recreation, 42(4), 405410. https://doi.org/10.1080/10671188.1971.10615088CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luce, R. D. (1986). Response Times: Their Role in Inferring Elementary Mental Organization. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
MacLeod, C. M., & Dunbar, K. (1988). Training and Stroop-like interference: Evidence for a continuum of automaticity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 14(1), 126135. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.14.1.126Google ScholarPubMed
Medic-Pericevic, S., Mikov, I., Glavaski-Kraljevic, M., Spanovic, M., Bozic, A., Vasovic, V., & Mikov, M. (2020). The effects of aging and driving experience on reaction times of professional drivers. Work: Journal of Prevention, Assessment & Rehabilitation, 66(2), 405419. https://doi.org/10.3233/WOR-203181CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, J. O., & Low, K. (2001). Motor processes in simple, Go/No-go, and choice reaction time tasks: A psychophysiological analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27(2), 266289. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.27.2.266Google ScholarPubMed
Millisecond Software. (2022). Download Inquisit Lab [Inquisit Lab 6.6.1]. www.millisecond.com/downloadGoogle Scholar
Müller, J. (1833–1840). Handbuch der Physiologie des Menschen für Vorlesungen. J. Hölscher.Google Scholar
Nosek, B. A. (2007). Implicit–explicit relations. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16(2), 6569. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00477.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nosek, B. A., & Banaji, M. R. (2001). The Go/No-go Association Task. Social Cognition, 19(6), 625666. http://dx.doi.org/10.1521/soco.19.6.625.20886CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nosek, B. A., Banaji, M. R., & Greenwald, A. G. (2002). Harvesting implicit group attitudes and beliefs from a demonstration web site. Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice, 6(1), 101115. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/1089-2699.6.1.101CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nosek, B. A., Smyth, F. L., Hansen, J. J., Devos, T., Lindner, N. M., Ranganath, K. A., et al. (2007). Pervasiveness and correlates of implicit attitudes and stereotypes. European Review of Social Psychology, 18, 3688. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10463280701489053CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Olson, M. A., & Fazio, R. H. (2004). Reducing the influence of extrapersonal associations on the Implicit Association Test: Personalizing the IAT. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86(5), 653667. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.86.5.653CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Osborne, J. W. (2002). Notes on the use of data transformations. Practical Assessment, Research, and Evaluation, 8(6). https://doi.org/10.7275/4vng-5608Google Scholar
Overbosch, P., de Wijk, R., de Jonge, T. J., & Köster, E. P. (1989). Temporal integration and reaction times in human smell. Physiology & Behavior, 45(3), 615626. https://doi.org/10.1016/0031-9384(89)90082-6CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pavelka, R., Třebicky, V., Fialova, J. T., Zdobinsky, A., Coufalova, K., Havlicek, J., & Tufano, J. J. (2020). Acute fatigue affects reaction times and reaction consistency in mixed martial arts fighters. PLOS ONE, 15(1), e0227675. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0227675CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peschke, C., Hilgetag, C. C., & Olk, B. (2013). Influence of stimulus type on effects of flanker, flanker position, and trial sequence in a saccadic eye movement task. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 66(11), 22532267. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2013.777464CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pins, D., & Bonnet, C. (1996). On the relation between stimulus intensity and processing time: Piéron’s law and choice reaction time. Perception & Psychophysics, 58(3), 390400. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03206815CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Psychology Software Tools. (2016). Psychology Software Tools homepage [E-Prime 3.0]. www.pstnet.comGoogle Scholar
Ratcliff, R. (1993). Methods for dealing with reaction time outliers. Psychological Bulletin, 114(3), 510532. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.114.3.510CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Robinson, E. S. (1934). Work of the integrated organism. In Murchison, C. (ed.), A Handbook of General Experimental Psychology (pp. 571650). Clark University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Robinson, M. D., & Tamir, M. (2005). Neuroticism as mental noise: A relation between neuroticism and reaction time standard deviations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89(1), 107114. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.89.1.107CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rodriguez-Raecke, R., Schrader, C., Tacik, P., Dressler, D., Lanfermann, H., & Wittfoth, M. (2022). Conflict adaptation and related neuronal processing in Parkinson’s disease. Brain Imaging and Behavior, 16, 455463.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sanders, A. F. (1998). Elements of Human Performance: Reaction Processes and Attention in Human Skill. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Satorres, E., Oliva, I., Escudero, J., & Meléndez, J. C. (2020). Conflict monitoring on an emotional Stroop task: Comparison of healthy older adults and patients with major neurocognitive disorders due to probable AD. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 42(5), 485494. https://doi.org/10.1080/13803395.2020.1761946CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schultz, D. P., & Schultz, S. E. (2016). A History of Modern Psychology, 11th ed. Cengage Learning.Google Scholar
Senholzi, K. B., Depue, B. E., Correll, J., Banich, M. T., & Ito, T. A. (2015). Brain activation underlying threat detection to targets of different races. Social Neuroscience, 10(6), 651662. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17470919.2015.1091380CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stafford, T., Ingram, L., & Gurney, K. N. (2011). Piéron’s law holds during Stroop conflict: Insights into the architecture of decision making. Cognitive Science, 35(8), 15531566. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1551-6709.2011.01195.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sternberg, S. (1969). Memory-scanning: Mental processes revealed by reaction-time experiments. American Scientist, 57(4), 421457.Google ScholarPubMed
Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18(6), 643662. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0054651CrossRefGoogle Scholar
VaezMousavi, S. M., Barry, R. J., & Clarke, A. R. (2009). Individual differences in task-related activation and performance. Physiology and Behavior, 98(3), 326330. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2009.06.007CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vakil, E., Lowe, M., & Goldfus, C. (2015). Performance of children with developmental dyslexia on two skill learning tasks – Serial Reaction Time and Tower of Hanoi Puzzle: A test of the specific procedural learning difficulties theory. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 48(5), 471481. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0022219413508981CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Valiente, C., Cantero, D., Vázquez, C., Sanchez, Á., Provencio, M., & Espinosa, R. (2011). Implicit and explicit self-esteem discrepancies in paranoia and depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 120(3), 691699. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0022856CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van Bavel, J. J., & Cunningham, W. A. (2010). A social neuroscience approach to self and social categorisation: A new look at an old issue. European Review of Social Psychology, 21(1), 237284. https://doi.org/10.1080/10463283.2010.543314CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Zandt, T. (2002). Analysis of response time distributions. In Pashler, H. & Wixted, J. (eds.), Stevens’ Handbook of Experimental Psychology: Methodology in Experimental Psychology, 3rd ed. (vol. 4, pp. 461516). John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Volkan, E., & Hadjimarkou, M. M. (2019). Undivided trauma in a divided Cyprus: Modified emotional Stroop study. Psychological Trauma: Theory, Research, Practice, and Policy, 14(6), 989997. https://doi.org./10.1037/tra0000527CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Welford, A. T. (1981). Signal, noise, performance, and age. Human Factors, 23(1), 97109.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whelan, R. (2008). Effective analysis of reaction time data. Psychological Record, 58(3), 475482.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wilcox, R. R. (1998). How many discoveries have been lost by ignoring modern statistical methods? American Psychologist, 53(3), 300314. https://doi.org./10.1037/0003-066X.53.3.300CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wylie, S. A., van den Wildenberg, W. P. M., Ridderinkhof, K. R., Bashore, T. R., Powell, V. D., Manning, C. A., & Wooten, G. F. (2009). The effect of speed-accuracy strategy on response interference control in Parkinson’s disease. Neuropsychologia, 47(8–9), 18441853. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.02.025CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yerkes, R. M., & Dodson, J. D. (1908). The relation of strength of stimulus to rapidity of habit formation. Journal of Comparative Neurology and Psychology, 18, 459482. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/cne.920180503CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zayas, V., Wang, A. M., & McCalla, J. D. (2022). Me as good and me as bad: Priming the self triggers positive and negative implicit evaluations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 122(1), 106134. http://dx.doi.org/2048/10.1037/pspp0000332CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

  • Reaction Time Measures
  • Edited by John E. Edlund, Rochester Institute of Technology, New York, Austin Lee Nichols, Central European University, Vienna
  • Book: The Cambridge Handbook of Research Methods and Statistics for the Social and Behavioral Sciences
  • Online publication: 12 December 2024
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/9781009000796.018
Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

  • Reaction Time Measures
  • Edited by John E. Edlund, Rochester Institute of Technology, New York, Austin Lee Nichols, Central European University, Vienna
  • Book: The Cambridge Handbook of Research Methods and Statistics for the Social and Behavioral Sciences
  • Online publication: 12 December 2024
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/9781009000796.018
Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

  • Reaction Time Measures
  • Edited by John E. Edlund, Rochester Institute of Technology, New York, Austin Lee Nichols, Central European University, Vienna
  • Book: The Cambridge Handbook of Research Methods and Statistics for the Social and Behavioral Sciences
  • Online publication: 12 December 2024
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/9781009000796.018
Available formats
×