Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-t8hqh Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-22T05:51:22.908Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

3 - Getting Good Ideas and Making the Most of Them

from Part I - From Idea to Reality: The Basics of Research

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  25 May 2023

Austin Lee Nichols
Affiliation:
Central European University, Vienna
John Edlund
Affiliation:
Rochester Institute of Technology, New York
Get access

Summary

Good research ideas and hypotheses do not just magically exist, begging to be tested; they must be discovered and nurtured. Systematic methods can help. Drawing on relevant scholarly literatures (e.g., research on creativity) and on the published personal reflections of successful scientists, this chapter provides an overview of strategies that can help researchers to (1) gather research ideas in the first place, (2) figure out whether an idea is worth working on, and (3) transform a promising idea into a rigorous scientific hypothesis. In doing so, it provides pragmatic advice about how to get good ideas and make the most of them.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2023

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Amabile, T. M. (1998). How to kill creativity. Harvard Business Review, 76, 7687.Google ScholarPubMed
Baas, M., De Dreu, C. K. W., & Nijstad, B. A. (2008). A meta-analysis of 25 years of mood–creativity research: Hedonic tone, activation, or regulatory focus? Psychological Bulletin, 134, 779806. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0012815CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bahns, A. J., Crandall, C. S., Gillath, O., & Preacher, K. J. (2017). Similarity in relationships as niche construction: Choice, stability, and influence within dyads in a free choice environment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 112(2), 329355.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berscheid, E. (1992). A glance back at a quarter century of social psychology. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 525533. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.63.4.525CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beveridge, W. I. B. (1957). The Art of Scientific Investigation. Vintage Books.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Campbell, D. T. (1974). Evolutionary epistemology. In Schilpp, P. A. (ed.), The Philosophy of Karl Popper (vol. 1, pp. 413463). Open Court.Google Scholar
Casadevall, A. & Fang, F. C. (2016). Rigorous science: A how-to guide. mBio, 7, e01902e01916. https://doi.org/10.1128/MBIO.01902-16Google Scholar
Cialdini, R. B. (1980). Full-cycle social psychology. In Bickman, L. (ed.), Applied Social Psychology Annual, Volume 1 (pp. 2147). SAGE Publications.Google Scholar
Cialdini, R. B. (2006). Influence: The Psychology of Persuasion. Harper Business.Google Scholar
Clark, L. A. & Watson, D. (2019). Constructing validity: New developments in creating objective measuring instruments. Psychological Assessment, 31, 14121427. https://doi.org/10.1037/pas0000626CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1997). Happiness and creativity. The Futurist, 31, S8S12.Google Scholar
Davis, M. S. (1971). That’s interesting! Towards a phenomenology of sociology and a sociology of phenomenology. Philosophy of the Social Sciences, 1(2), 309344.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dialogue (2002). Which scientific problem to pursue? Eminent social/personality psychologists reveal their secrets of scientific success to the editors of Dialogue. Dialogue, 17(2), 1215. https://spsp.org/sites/default/files/dialogue172.pdfGoogle Scholar
Feist, G. J. (1998). A meta-analysis of the impact of personality on scientific and artistic creativity. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 2, 290309. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327957pspr0204_5CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Feyerabend, P. (1975). Against Method. New Left.Google Scholar
Fiedler, K. (2017). What constitutes strong psychological science? The (neglected) role of diagnosticity and a priori theorizing. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 12, 4661. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691616654458CrossRefGoogle Scholar
García Márquez, G. (1995). Of Love and Other Demons. Knopf. (Originally published as Del amor y otros demonios by Mondadori. Translated by Edith Grossman.)Google Scholar
Gervais, W. M. (2021). Practical methodological reform needs good theory. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 16, 827843. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691620977471CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grahek, I., Schaller, M., & Tackett, J. L. (2021). Anatomy of a psychological theory: Integrating construct validation and computational modeling methods to advance theorizing. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 16, 803815. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691620966794CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gray, K. (2017). How to map theory: Reliable methods are fruitless without rigorous theory. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 12, 731741. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691617691949Google Scholar
Gray, K. & Wegner, D. M. (2013). Six guidelines for interesting research. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 8, 549553. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691613497967Google Scholar
Grene, M. (1985). Perception, interpretation, and the sciences. In Depew, D. J & Weber, B. H (eds.), Evolution at a Crossroads: The New Biology and the New Philosophy of Science. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Hull, D. L. (1988). Science as a Process: An Evolutionary Account of the Social and Conceptual Development of Science. University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jaremka, L. M., Ackerman, J. M., Gawronski, B., et al. (2020). Common academic experiences no one talks about: Repeated rejection, impostor syndrome, and burnout. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 15, 519543. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691619898848Google Scholar
Klein, S. B. (2014). What can recent replication failures tell us about the theoretical commitments of psychology? Theory & Psychology, 24, 326338. https://doi.org/10.1177/0959354314529616Google Scholar
Lam, T. W. H. & Chiu, C.-Y. (2002). The motivational function of regulatory focus in creativity. Journal of Creative Behavior, 36, 138150. https://doi.org/10.1002/j.2162-6057.2002.tb01061.xGoogle Scholar
Leung, A. K.-Y., Maddux, W. W., Galinsky, A. D., & Chiu, C.-Y. (2008). Multicultural experience enhances creativity: The when and how. American Psychologist, 63, 169181. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.63.3.169CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Longino, H. E. (1990). Science as Social Knowledge: Values and Objectivity in Scientific Inquiry. Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Maddux, W. W. & Galinsky, A. D. (2009). Cultural borders and mental barriers: The relationship between living abroad and creativity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96, 10471061. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0014861CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McGuire, W. J. (1997). Creative hypothesis generating in psychology: Some useful heuristics. Annual Review of Psychology, 48, 130. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.48.1.1CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morewedge, C. K., Shu, L. L., Gilbert, D. T., & Wilson, T. D. (2009). Bad riddance or good rubbish: Ownership and not loss aversion cause the endowment effect. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45, 947951. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2009.05.014CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Murray, D. R. & Schaller, M. (2016). The behavioral immune system: Implications for social cognition, social interaction, and social influence. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 53, 75129. https://doi.org/10.1016/bs.aesp.2015.09.002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Muthukrishna, M. & Henrich, J. (2019). A problem in theory. Nature Human Behavior, 3, 221229. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-018-0522-1CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Niiniluoto, I. (2019). Scientific progress. In Zalta, E. N. (ed.), The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy (Winter 2019 Edition). Available at: https://plato.stanford.edu/archives/win2019/entries/scientific-progress/.Google Scholar
Nisbett, R. E. (1990). The anti-creativity letters: Advice from a senior tempter to a junior tempter. American Psychologist, 45, 10781082.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Noah, T., Schul, Y., & Mayo, R. (2018). When both the original study and its failed replication are correct: Feeling observed eliminates the facial-feedback effect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 114(5), 657664. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspa0000121CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oberauer, K., & Lewandowsky, S. (2019). Addressing the theory crisis in psychology. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 26, 15961618. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-019-01645-2CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Perry-Smith, J. & Mannucci, P. V. (2015). Social networks, creativity, and entrepreneurship. In Shalley, C. E., Hitt, M. A., & Zhou, J. (eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Creativity, Innovation, and Entrepreneurship (pp. 205224). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Popper, K. (1959/2005). The Logic of Scientific Discovery. Routledge. (Originally published in 1934 as Logik der Forschung by Verlag von Julius Springer.)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Popper, K. (1963). Conjectures and Refutations. Routledge and Keagan Paul.Google Scholar
Root-Bernstein, R. & Root-Bernstein, M. (2004). Artistic scientists and scientific artists: The link between polymathy and creativity. In Sternberg, R. J., Grigorenko, E. L., & Singer, J. L. (eds.), Creativity: From Potential to Realization (pp. 127151). American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Schaller, M. (2016). The empirical benefits of conceptual rigor: Systematic articulation of conceptual hypotheses can reduce the risk of non-replicable results (and facilitate novel discoveries too). Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 66, 107115. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2015.09.006CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Skinner, B. F. (2019). The Behavior of Organisms: An Experimental Analysis. Appleton-Century. (First published by Appleton-Century Company in 1938).Google Scholar
Smaldino, P. E. (2017). Models are stupid, and we need more of them. In Vallacher, R., Read, S., & Nowak, A. (eds.), Computational Social Psychology (pp. 311331). Routledge. https://doi.org/10.4324/9781315173726-14Google Scholar
Smaldino, P. E. (2020). How to translate a verbal theory into a formal model. Social Psychology, 51, 207218. https://doi.org/10.1027/1864-9335/a000425CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sosa, M. E. (2011). Where do creative interactions come from? The role of tie content and social networks. Organization Science, 22(1), 121. https://doi.org/10.1287/orsc.1090.0519Google Scholar
Taylor, S. E. & Fiske, S. T. (2019) Interview with Shelley E. Taylor. Annual Review of Psychology, 70, 18. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-041818-040645Google Scholar
Thagard, P. R. (1978). Why astrology is pseudoscience, PSA, 1, 223234.Google Scholar
Van Lange, P. A. M. (2013). What we should expect from theories in social psychology: Truth, abstraction, progress, and applicability as standards (TAPAS). Personality and Social Psychology Review, 17, 4055. https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868312453088CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whillans, A. V., Weidman, A. C., & Dunn, E. W. (2016). Valuing time over money is associated with greater happiness. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 7, 213222. https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550615623842Google Scholar
Yong, E. (2012). Replication studies: Bad copy. Nature, 485, 298300.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×