Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-gxg78 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-22T20:14:16.611Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

2 - Developmental Theories of Prosociality

from Part I - Development of Prosociality

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  25 May 2023

Tina Malti
Affiliation:
University of Toronto
Maayan Davidov
Affiliation:
The Hebrew University of Jerusalem
Get access

Summary

In this chapter, we review developmental theories of prosocial behavior. We begin by briefly discussing the ways in which the grand theories of development have been used to explain prosocial development, as well as some of the major biologically oriented arguments. We then present a comprehensive model of prosocial behavior that includes the multiple individual and contextual factors that contribute to prosocial actions in children. Next, we discuss the multidimensional nature of young children’s prosocial behavior – a topic of considerable research in the last decade – and suggest ways that the comprehensive model is relevant for prosocial development in very young children.

Type
Chapter
Information
The Cambridge Handbook of Prosociality
Development, Mechanisms, Promotion
, pp. 17 - 36
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2023

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Abramson, L., Uzefovsky, F., Toccaceli, V., & Knafo-Noam, A. (2020). The genetic and environmental origins of emotional and cognitive empathy: Review and meta-analyses of twin studies. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 114, 113133. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2020.03.023Google Scholar
Aitken, J., Ruffman, T., & Taumoepeau, M. (2019). Toddlers’ self‐recognition and progression from goal‐ to emotion‐based helping: A longitudinal study. Child Development. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13304CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Aronfreed, J. (1970). The socialization of altruistic and sympathetic behavior: Some theoretical and experimental analyses. In Macaulay, J. & Berkowitz, L. (Eds.), Altruism and helping behavior (pp. 103126). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action: A social cognitive theory. Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
Bandura, A. (2002). Selective moral disengagement in the exercise of moral agency. Journal of Moral Education, 31, 101119.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bandura, A., Caprara, G. V., Barbaranelli, C., Gerbino, M., & Pastorelli, C. (2003). Role of affective self-regulatory efficacy in diverse sphere of psychosocial functioning. Child Development, 74(3), 769782.Google Scholar
Bandura, A., Caprara, G. V., Barbaranelli, C., Pastorelli, C., & Regalia, C. (2001). Sociocognitive self-regulatory mechanisms governing transgressive behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80(1), 125135. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.80.1.125Google Scholar
Batson, C. D. (1991). The altruism question: Toward a social-psychological answer. Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Batson, C. D., Lishner, D. A., & Stocks, E. L. (2015). The empathy-altruism hypothesis. In Schroeder, D. A & Graziano, W. G. (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of prosocial behavior. Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780195399813.013.023Google Scholar
Bischof-Köhler, D. (2012). Empathy and self-recognition in phylogenetic and ontogenetic perspective. Emotion Review, 4(1), 4048. https://doi.org/10.1177/1754073911421377Google Scholar
Böhm, R., & Buttelmann, D. (2017). The impact of resource valence on children’s other- regarding preferences. Developmental Psychology, 53(9), 1656. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000365Google Scholar
Bowles, S. (2006). Group competition, reproductive leveling, and the evolution of human altruism. Science, 314(5805), 15691572. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1134829Google Scholar
Brownell, C. A., Svetlova, M., Anderson, R., Nichols, S. R., & Drummond, J. (2013). Socialization of early prosocial behavior: Parents’ talk about emotions is associated with sharing and helping in toddlers. Infancy, 18(1), 91119. https://do.org/10.1111/j.1532-7078.2012.00125.xGoogle Scholar
Brownell, C. A., Svetlova, M., & Nichols, S. (2009). To share or not to share: When do toddlers respond to another’s needs? Infancy, 14(1), 117130. https://doi.org/10.1080/15250000802569868Google Scholar
Carpendale, J. I. M., Kettner, V. A., & Audet, K. N. (2015). On the nature of toddlers’ helping: Helping or interest in others’ activity? Social Development, 24(2), 357366. https://doi.org/10.1111/sode.12094Google Scholar
Dahl, A., & Paulus, M. (2019). From interest to obligation: The gradual development of human altruism. Child Development Perspectives, 13(1), 1014. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12298Google Scholar
Davidov, M., Paz, Y., Roth‐Hanania, R., Uzefovsky, F., Orlitsky, T., Mankuta, D., & Zahn‐Waxler, C. (2020). Caring babies: Concern for others in distress during infancy. Developmental Science, 17. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.13016Google Scholar
Davidov, M., Zahn‐Waxler, C., Roth‐Hanania, R., & Knafo, A. (2013). Concern for others in the first year of life: Theory, evidence, and avenues for research. Child Development Perspectives, 7(2), 126131. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12028Google Scholar
Demetriou, H., & Hay, D. F. (2004). Toddlers’ reactions to the distress of familiar peers: The importance of context. Infancy, 6(2), 299318. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327078in0602_9Google Scholar
Dondi, M., Simion, F., & Caltran, G. (1999). Can newborns discriminate between their own cry and the cry of another newborn infant? Developmental Psychology, 35(2), 418426. https://doi.org./10.1037/0012-1649.35.2.418Google Scholar
Drummond, J. D. K., Hammond, S. I., Satlof‐Bedrick, E., Waugh, W. E., & Brownell, C. A. (2017). Helping the one you hurt: Toddlers’ rudimentary guilt, shame, and prosocial behavior after harming another. Child Development, 88(4), 13821397. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12653Google Scholar
Drummond, J., Paul, E. F., Waugh, W. E., Hammond, S. I., & Brownell, C. A. (2014). Here, there and everywhere: Emotion and mental state talk in different social contexts predicts empathic helping in toddlers. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 361. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00361Google Scholar
Dunfield, K. A., & Kuhlmeier, V. A. (2010). Intention-mediated selective helping in infancy. Psychological Science, 21(4), 523527. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797610364119Google Scholar
Dunfield, K. A., & Kuhlmeier, V. A. (2013). Classifying prosocial behavior: Children’s responses to instrumental need, emotional distress, and material desire. Child Development, 84(5), 17661776. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12075Google Scholar
Dunfield, K., Kuhlmeier, V. A., O’Connell, L., & Kelley, E. (2011). Examining the diversity of prosocial behavior: Helping, sharing, and comforting in infancy. Infancy, 16(3), 227247. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1532-7078.2010.00041.xGoogle Scholar
Edwards, A., Eisenberg, N., Spinrad, T. L., Reiser, M., Eggum‐Wilkens, N. D., & Liew, J. (2015). Predicting sympathy and prosocial behavior from young children’s dispositional sadness. Social Development, 24(1), 7694. https://doi.org/10.1111/sode.12084Google Scholar
Eisenberg, N. (1986). Altruistic emotion, cognition, and behavior. Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Eisenberg, N., & Fabes, R. A. (1998). Prosocial development. In Damon, W. (Series Ed.) & Eisenberg, N. (Volume Ed.), Handbook of child psychology, Vol. 3: Social, emotional, and personality development (5th ed., pp. 701778). John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., & Spinrad, T. L. (2006). Prosocial behavior. In Eisenberg, N. (Vol. Ed) and Damon, W. & Lerner, R. M. (Series Eds.), Handbook of child psychology, Vol. 3: Social, emotional, and personality development (6th ed., pp. 646718). Wiley. https://doi.org/10.1002/9780470147658Google Scholar
Eisenberg, N., Guthrie, I., Cumberland, A., Murphy, B. C., Shepard, S. A., Zhou, Q., & Carlo, G. (2002). Prosocial development in early adulthood: A longitudinal study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82, 9931006. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.82.6.993CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eisenberg, N., Guthrie, I. K., Murphy, B. C., Shepard, S. A., Cumberland, A., & Carlo, G. (1999). Consistency and development of prosocial dispositions: A longitudinal study. Child Development, 70, 13601372. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00100Google Scholar
Eisenberg, N., Hofer, C., Sulik, M. J., & Liew, J. (2014). The development of prosocial moral reasoning and a prosocial orientation in young adulthood: Concurrent and longitudinal correlates. Developmental Psychology, 50, 5870. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0032990Google Scholar
Eisenberg, N., Spinrad, T. L., & Knafo‐Noam, A. (2015). Prosocial development. In Lamb, M. E., & Lerner, R. M. (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology and developmental science (7th ed., Vol. 3, pp. 610656). John Wiley & Sons. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781118963418.childpsy315Google Scholar
Eisenberg, N., VanSchydel, S. K., & Spinrad, T. L. (2016). Prosocial motivation: Inferences from an opaque body of work. Child Development, 87(6), 16681678. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12638Google Scholar
Eisenberg‑Berg, N. (1979). The development of children’s prosocial moral judgment. Developmental Psychology, 15(2), 128137. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.15.2.128Google Scholar
Eisenberg-Berg, N., & Hand, M. (1979). The relationship of preschoolers’ reasoning about prosocial moral conflicts to prosocial behavior. Child Development, 50(2), 356363. https://doi.org/10.2307/1129410Google Scholar
Ekstein, R. (1978). Psychoanalysis, sympathy, and altruism. In Wispe, L. G. (Ed.), Altruism, sympathy, and helping: Psychological and sociological principles. Academic Press.Google Scholar
Fehr, E., Bernhard, H., & Rockenbach, B. (2008). Egalitarianism in young children. Nature, 454, 10791084. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature07155Google Scholar
Fenichel, O. (1945). The psychoanalytic theory of neurosis. Norton.Google Scholar
Feshbach, N. D. (1978). Studies of empathic behavior in children. In Maher, B. A. (Ed.), Progress in experimental personality research (Vol. 8, pp. 147). Academic Press.Google Scholar
Flook, L., Zahn-Waxler, C., & Davidson, R. J. (2019). Developmental differences in prosocial behavior between preschool and late elementary school. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 8. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00876Google Scholar
Fortuna, K., & Knafo, A. (2014). Parental and genetic contributions to prosocial behavior during childhood. In Padilla-Walker, L. & Carlo, G. (Eds.). The multidimensionality of prosocial behavior: Biological, socialization, and contextual perspectives (pp. 7089). Oxford University Press. http://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199964772.003.0004Google Scholar
Freud, A. (1946). A form of altruism. In Freud, A. (Ed.), The ego and the mechanisms of defense. International Universities Press.Google Scholar
Freud, S. (1930). Civilization and its discontents (J. Riviere, Trans.). Hogarth Press.Google Scholar
Freud, S. (1968). New introductory lectures on psychoanalysis. Hogarth (originally published in 1933).Google Scholar
Geangu, E., Benga, O., Stahl, D., & Striano, T. (2011). Individual differences in infants’ emotional resonance to a peer in distress: Self–other awareness and emotion regulation. Social Development, 20(3), 450470. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9507.2010.00596.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gelfand, D. M., Hartmann, D. P., Cromer, C. C., Smith, C. L., & Page, B. C. (1975). The effects of instructional prompts and praise on children’s donation rates. Child Development, 46(4), 980983. https://doi.org/10.2307/1128408Google Scholar
Glover, E. (1968). The birth of the ego. George Allen and Unwin.Google Scholar
Graziano, W. G., Habashi, M. M., Sheese, B. E., & Tobin, R. M. (2007). Agreeableness, empathy, and helping: A person X situation perspective. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93(4), 583599. https:/doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.93.4.583Google Scholar
Gross, R. L., Drummond, J., Satlof-Bedrick, E., Waugh, W. E., Svetlova, M., & Brownell, C. A. (2015). Individual differences in toddlers’ social understanding and prosocial behavior: Disposition or socialization? Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 11. https://doi.org./10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00600Google Scholar
Hamlin, J. K. (2015). The case for social evaluation in preverbal infants: Gazing toward one’s goal drives infants’ preferences for helpers over hinderers in the hill paradigm. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 9. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01563Google Scholar
Hamlin, J. K., & Wynn, K. (2011). Young infants prefer prosocial to antisocial others. Cognitive Development, 26(1), 3039. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2010.09.001Google Scholar
Hamlin, J. K., Wynn, K., & Bloom, P. (2007). Social evaluation in preverbal infants. Nature, 450(7169), 557559. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature06288Google Scholar
Hamlin, J. K., Wynn, K., Bloom, P., & Mahajan, N. (2011). How infants and toddlers react to antisocial others. PNAS Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 108(50), 1993119936. Htpps://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1110306108Google Scholar
Hammond, S. I., Al-Jbouri, E., Edwards, V., & Feltham, L. E. (2017). Infant helping in the first year of life: Parents’ recollection of infants’ earliest prosocial behaviors. Infant Behavior & Development, 47, 5457. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2017.02.004Google Scholar
Hartmann, D. P., Gelfand, D. M., Smith, C. L., Paul, S. C., Cromer, C. C., Page, B. C., & Lebenta, D. V. (1976). Factors affecting the acquisition and elimination of children’s donation behavior. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 21(2), 328338. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-0965(76)90046-1Google Scholar
Hay, D. F. (1979). Cooperative interactions and sharing between very young children and their parents. Developmental Psychology, 15(6), 647653. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.15.6.647Google Scholar
Hay, D. F. (2009). The roots and branches of human altruism. British Journal of Psychology, 100(3), 473479. https://doi.org/10.1348/000712609X442096Google Scholar
Hay, D. F., Caplan, M. Z., Stimson, C., & Castle, J. (1991). Does sharing become increasingly “rational” in the second year of life? Developmental Psychology, 27, 987993.Google Scholar
Hay, D. F., Castle, J., Davies, L., Demetriou, H., & Stimson, C. A. (1999). Prosocial action in very early childhood. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 40(6), 905916. https://do.org/10.1111/1469-7610.00508Google Scholar
Hay, D. F., Nash, A., & Pedersen, J. (1981). Responses of six-month-olds to the distress of their peers. Child Development, 52(3), 10711075. https://doi.org/10.2307/1129114Google Scholar
Hay, D. F., & Rheingold, H. L. (1983). The early appearance of some valued behaviors. In Bridgeman, D. L. (Ed.), The nature of prosocial development: Interdisciplinary theories and strategies (pp. 7394). Academic Press.Google Scholar
Hepach, R. (2017). Prosocial arousal in children. Child Development Perspectives, 11(1), 5055. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdep.1220Google Scholar
Hepach, R., Haberl, K., Lambert, S., & Tomasello, M. (2017). Toddlers help anonymously. Infancy, 22(1), 130145. https://doi.org/10.1111/infa.12143Google Scholar
Hepach, R., Vaish, A., & Tomasello, M. (2013). Young children sympathize less in response to unjustified emotional distress. Developmental Psychology, 49(6), 11321138. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0029501Google Scholar
Hoffman, M. L. (1970). Moral development. In Mussen, P. H. (Ed.), Carmichael’s manual of child development (Vol. 2, pp. 261359). Wiley.Google Scholar
Hoffman, M. L. (1975). Developmental synthesis of affect and cognition and its implications for altruistic motivation. Developmental Psychology, 11(5), 607622. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.11.5.607Google Scholar
Hoffman, M. L. (2000). Empathy and moral development: Implications for caring and justice. Cambridge University Press. http://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511805851Google Scholar
House, B., Henrich, J., Sarnecka, B., & Silk, J. B. (2013). The development of contingent reciprocity in children. Evolution and Human Behavior, 34(2), 8693. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2012.10.001Google Scholar
Johnson, D. B. (1982). Altruistic behavior and the development of the self in infants. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 28(3), 379388.Google Scholar
Kiang, L., Moreno, A. J., & Robinson, J. L. (2004). Maternal preconceptions about parenting predict child temperament, maternal sensitivity, and children’s empathy. Developmental Psychology, 40(6), 10811092. htpps://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.40.6.1081Google Scholar
Knafo, A., & Uzefovsky, F. (2013). Variation in empathy: The interplay of genetic and environmental factors. In Legerstee, M., Haley, D. W., & Bornstein, M. H. (Eds.), The infant mind: Origins of the social brain (pp. 97122). Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Knafo, A., Zahn-Waxler, C., Van Hulle, C., Robinson, J. L., & Rhee, S. H. (2008). The developmental origins of a disposition toward empathy: Genetic and environmental contributions. Emotion, 8(6), 737752. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0014179Google Scholar
Knafo-Noam, A., Uzefovsky, F., Israel, S., Davidov, M., & Zahn-Waxler, C. (2015). The prosocial personality and its facets: Genetic and environmental architecture of mother-reported behavior of 7-year-old twins. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 9. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00112Google Scholar
Knafo-Noam, A., Vertsberger, D., & Israel, S. (2018). Genetic and environmental contributions to children’s prosocial behavior: Brief review and new evidence from a reanalysis of experimental twin data. Current Opinion in Psychology, 2018(20), 6065. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.08.013Google Scholar
Kohlberg, L. (1969). Stage and sequence: The cognitive-developmental approach to socialization. In Goslin, A. (Ed.), Handbook of socialization theory and research (pp. 325480). Rand McNally.Google Scholar
Kohlberg, L. (1984). Essays on moral development, Vol. 2: The psychology of moral development. Harper & Row.Google Scholar
Knafo, A., Steinberg, T., & Goldner, I. (2011). Children’s low affective perspective-taking ability is associated with low self-initiated pro-sociality. Emotion 11(1), 194198. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0021240Google Scholar
Liddle, M. E., Bradley, B. S., & Mcgrath, A. (2015). Baby empathy: Infant distress and peer prosocial responses. Infant Mental Health Journal, 36(4), 446458. https://doi.org/10.1002/imhj.21519Google Scholar
Liszkowski, U., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2008). Twelve-month-olds communicate helpfully and appropriately for knowledgeable and ignorant partners. Cognition, 108(3), 732739. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2008.06.013Google Scholar
Lu, H. J., & Chang, L. (2016). Resource allocation to kin, friends, and strangers by 3- to 6-year-old children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 150, 194206. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2016.05.018Google Scholar
Malti, T., Gummerum, M., Ongley, S., Chaparro, M., Nola, M., & Bae, N. Y. (2016). “Who is worthy of my generosity?” Recipient characteristics and the development of children’s sharing. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 40(1), 3140. https://doi.org/10.1177/0165025414567007Google Scholar
Marsh, A. A. (2016). Neural, cognitive, and evolutionary foundations of human altruism. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 7, 5971. https:/doi.org/10.1002/wcs.1377Google Scholar
Moore, C. (2009). Fairness in children’s resource allocation depends on the recipient. Psychological Science, 20(8), 944948. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02378.xGoogle Scholar
Newton, E. K., Goodman, M., & Thompson, R. A. (2014). Why do some toddlers help a stranger? Origins of individual differences in prosocial behavior. Infancy, 19, 214226. https://doi.org/10.1111/infa.12043CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nichols, S. R., Svetlova, M., & Brownell, C. A. (2009). The role of social understanding and empathic disposition in young children’s responsiveness to distress in parents and peers. Cognition, Brain, Behavior: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 13(4), 449478.Google Scholar
Nichols, S. R., Svetlova, M., & Brownell, C. A. (2014). Toddlers’ responses to Infants’ negative emotions. Infancy, 20(1), 7097. https://doi.org/10.1111/infa.12066Google Scholar
Nowak, M. A. (2006). Five rules for the evolution of cooperation. Science, 314(5805), 15601563. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1133755Google Scholar
Olson, K. R., & Spelke, E. S. (2008). Foundations of cooperation in young children. Cognition, 108(1), 222231. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2007.12.003Google Scholar
Ongley, S. F., & Malti, T. (2014). The role of moral emotions in the development of children’s sharing behavior. Developmental Psychology, 50(4), 11481159. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0035191Google Scholar
Paulus, M. (2014). The emergence of prosocial behavior: Why do infants and toddlers help, comfort, and share? Child Development Perspectives, 8(2), 7781. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12066CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paulus, M. (2018). The multidimensional nature of early prosocial behavior: A motivational perspective. Current Opinion in Psychology, 20, 111116. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.09.003Google Scholar
Paulus, M., & Moore, C. (2014). The development of recipient-dependent sharing behavior and sharing expectations in preschool children. Developmental Psychology, 50(3), 914921. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0034169Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1965). The moral judgment of the child. Free Press (originally published in 1932).Google Scholar
Pinker, S. (2012, June 19). The false allure of group selection. Edge. Retrieved from http://edge.org/conversation/the-false-allure-of-groupGoogle Scholar
Rochat, P., & Striano, T. (2002). Who’s in the mirror? Self-other discrimination in specular images by four- and nine-month-old infants. Child Development, 73(1), 3546. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00390Google Scholar
Roth-Hanania, R., Davidov, M., & Zahn-Waxler, C. (2011). Empathy development from 8 to 16 months: Early signs of concern for others. Infant Behavior & Development, 34(3), 447458. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2011.04.007Google Scholar
Ruffman, T., Lorimer, B., & Scarf, D. (2017). Do infants really experience emotional contagion? Child Development Perspectives, https://doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12244Google Scholar
Schmidt, M. F. H., Svetlova, M., Johe, J., & Tomasello, M. (2016). Children’s developing understanding of legitimate reasons for allocating resources unequally. Cognitive Development, 37, 4252. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2015.11.001Google Scholar
Schuhmacher, N., Collard, J., & Kärtner, J. (2017). The differential role of parenting, peers, and temperament for explaining interindividual differences in 18-months-olds’ comforting and helping. Infant Behavior & Development, 46, 124134. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2017.01.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shaw, A., & Olson, K. R. (2012). Children discard a resource to avoid inequity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 141(2), 382395. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0025907Google Scholar
Sommerville, J. A., Schmidt, M. F. H., Yun, J., & Burns, M. (2013). The development of fairness expectations and prosocial behavior in the second year of life. Infancy, 18(1), 4066. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1532-7078.2012.00129.xGoogle Scholar
Sparks, E., Schinkel, M. G., & Moore, C. (2017). Affiliation affects generosity in young children: The roles of minimal group membership and shared interests. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 159, 242262. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2017.02.007Google Scholar
Spinrad, T. L., & Stifter, C. A. (2006). Toddlers’ empathy-related responding to distress: Predictions from negative emotionality and maternal behavior in infancy. Infancy, 10(2), 97121. htpps://doi.org/10.1207/s15327078in1002_1Google Scholar
Svetlova, M., Nichols, S. R., & Brownell, C. A. (2010). Toddlers’ prosocial behavior: From instrumental to empathic to altruistic helping. Child Development, 81(6), 18141827. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01512.xGoogle Scholar
Vaish, A., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2009). Sympathy through affective perspective taking and its relation to prosocial behavior in toddlers. Developmental Psychology, 45(2), 534543. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0014322Google Scholar
Vaish, A., & Tomasello, M. (2014). The early ontogeny of human cooperation and morality. In Killen, M. & Smetana, J. G. (Eds.), Handbook of moral development (2nd ed., pp. 279298). Psychology Press. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780203581957.ch13Google Scholar
Warneken, F., & Tomasello, M. (2006). Altruistic helping in human infants and young chimpanzees. Science, 311(5765), 13011303. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1121448Google Scholar
Warneken, F., & Tomasello, M. (2007). Helping and cooperation at 14 months of age. Infancy, 11, 271294. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1532-7078.2007.tb00227.xGoogle Scholar
Warneken, F., & Tomasello, M. (2008). Extrinsic rewards undermine altruistic tendencies in 20-month-olds. Developmental Psychology, 44(6), 17851788. htpps://doi.org/10.1037/a0013860Google Scholar
Warneken, F., & Tomasello, M. (2009). The roots of human altruism. British Journal of Psychology, 100(3), 455471. https://doi.org/10.1348/000712608X379061Google Scholar
Waugh, W. E., & Brownell, C. A. (2017). “Help yourself!” What can toddlers’ helping failures tell us about the development of prosocial behavior? Infancy, 22(5), 665680. https://doi.org/10.1111/infa.12189Google Scholar
Wilson, D. S. (1975). A theory of group selection. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 72(1), 143146.Google Scholar
Wilson, E. O. (2004). On human nature (rev. ed.). Harvard University Press (originally published in 1978).Google Scholar
Woodward, A. L. (2003). Infants’ developing understanding of the link between looker and object. Developmental Science, 6(3), 297311. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-7687.00286Google Scholar
Zahn-Waxler, C., Radke-Yarrow, M., Wagner, E., & Chapman, M. (1992). Development of concern for others. Developmental Psychology, 28, 126136.Google Scholar
Zahn-Waxler, C., Schiro, K., Robinson, J. L., Emde, R. N., & Schmitz, S. (2001). Empathy and prosocial patterns in young MZ and DZ twins: Development and genetic and environmental influences. In Emde, R. N., & Hewitt, J. K. (Eds.), Infancy to early childhood: Genetic and environmental influences on developmental change (pp. 141162). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×