Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-t7czq Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-22T04:05:04.580Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

6 - Stress and Rhythm

from Section II - Prosodic Production

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  11 November 2021

Rachael-Anne Knight
Affiliation:
City, University of London
Jane Setter
Affiliation:
University of Reading
Get access

Summary

This chapter covers two related prosodic phenomena: stress, i.e. the relative perceived prominence of individual syllables, and speech rhythm, the distributed prominence of syllables across stretches of speech and their perceived regularity in time. Both stress and rhythm can be viewed from the angles of perception and production, and speakers of different languages differ in how stress and rhythm are produced, perceived and interpreted for linguistic meaning. The chapter explains which articulatory and phonatory factors have been found to play a role in the production of stressed syllables, and distinguishes between stress and accent. The historically important concepts of rhythm classes and isochrony are presented in the context of current developments and debates. Three recent issues for research are presented in some detail: the analysis of stress in different languages, rhythm metrics, and rhythm and perception. The chapter further explores the role of rhythm for turn-taking in everyday talk, showing that conversationalists aim to rhythmically integrate their turns at talk with those of other speakers.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2021

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

6.6 References

Abercrombie, D. (1967). Elements of General Phonetics. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press.Google Scholar
Abercrombie, D. (1976). Stress and some other terms. Work in Progress, 9, Edinburgh: Edinburgh University, 51–3.Google Scholar
Allen, G. D. (1972). The location of rhythmic stress beats in English: An experimental study II. Language and Speech, 15(2), 179–95.Google Scholar
Arvaniti, A. (2009). Rhythm, timing and the timing of rhythm. Phonetica, 66(1–2), 4663.Google Scholar
Arvaniti, A. (2012). The usefulness of metrics in the quantification of speech rhythm. Journal of Phonetics, 40(3), 351–73.Google Scholar
Athanasopoulou, A. & Vogel, I. (2014). Compounds in Modern Greek. Proceedings of Meetings on Acoustics, 22(1).Google Scholar
Auer, P. & Couper-Kuhlen, E. (1994). Rhythmus und tempo in konversationeller Alltagssprache. In Schlieben-Lange, B., ed. Rhythmus, Zeitschrift für Literaturwissenschaft und Linguistik, 24(96), 78106.Google Scholar
Auer, P., Couper-Kuhlen, E. & Müller, F. (1999). Language in Time: The Rhythm and Tempo of Spoken Interaction. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Bååth, R. (2012). A new look at subjective rhythmisation. In Perspectives on Rhythm and Timing (PoRT): Book of Abstracts. University of Glasgow. www.sumsar.net/papers/rasmus_baath_PoRT_abstract.pdf.Google Scholar
Beckman, M. E. (1986). Stress and Non-Stress Accent. Dordrecht: Foris.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beňuš, Š., Gravano, A. & Hirschberg, J. (2011). Pragmatic aspects of temporal accommodation in turn-taking. Journal of Pragmatics, 43(12), 3001–27.Google Scholar
Bertinetto, P. M. (1989). Reflections on the dichotomy ‘stress’ vs. ’syllable-timing’. Revue de Phonétique Appliquée, 1989, 99130Google Scholar
Bertinetto, P. M. & Bertini, C. (2010). Towards a unified predictive model of natural language rhythm. In Russo, M., ed., Prosodic Universals. Comparative Studies in Rhythmic Modeling and Rhythm Typology. Rome: Aracne, pp. 4378.Google Scholar
Bolinger, D. (1955). Intersections of stress and intonation. WORD, 11(2), 195203.Google Scholar
Bolinger, D. (1981). Two Kinds of Vowels, Two Kinds of Rhythm. Bloomington, IN: The Indiana University Linguistics Club.Google Scholar
Bolinger, D. (1986). Intonation and Its Parts: Melody in Spoken English, Palo Alto. CA: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Bunta, F. & Ingram, D. (2007). The acquisition of speech rhythm by bilingual Spanish- and English-speaking 4- and 5-year-old children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 50(4), 9991014.Google Scholar
Campbell, N. & Beckman, M. (1997). Stress, prominence, and spectral tilt. In Intonation: Theory, Models and Applications. Proceedings of an ESCA Workshop. www.isca-speech.org/archive_open/int_97/inta_067.html.Google Scholar
Cauldwell, R. (2002). The functional irrhythmicality of spontaneous speech: A discourse view of speech rhythms. Apples – Journal of Applied Language Studies, 2(1), 124.Google Scholar
Chafe, W. (1976). Givenness, contrastiveness, definiteness, subjects, topics and point of view. In Li, C., ed., Subject and Topic. New York: Academic Press, pp. 2555.Google Scholar
Chartrand, T. L. & Bargh, J. A. (1999). The chameleon effect: The perception–behavior link and social interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 893910.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. & Halle, M. (1968). The Sound Pattern of English. New York: Harper & Row.Google Scholar
Cinque, G. (1993). A null theory of phrase and compound stress. Linguistic Inquiry, 24(2), 239–97.Google Scholar
Clark, J., Yallop, C. & Fletcher, J. (2007). An Introduction to Phonetics and Phonology, 3rd ed. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Classe, A. (1939/1978). The Rhythm of English Prose. London: Norwood Editions.Google Scholar
Couper-Kuhlen, E. (1991). A rhythm-based metric for turn-taking. Proceedings of the 12th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences, 1, 275–8.Google Scholar
Couper-Kuhlen, E. (1993). English Speech Rhythm: Form and Function in Everyday Verbal Interaction. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Couper-Kuhlen, E. (2014). Prosody as dialogic interaction. In Barth-Weingarten, D. & Szczepek Reed, B., eds., Prosodie und Phonetik in der Interaktion – Prosody and Phonetics in Interaction. Mannheim: Verlag für Gesprächsforschung, pp. 229–59.Google Scholar
Couper-Kuhlen, E. & Auer, P. (1991). On the contextualizing function of speech rhythm in conversation: Question–answer sequences. In Verschueren, J., ed., Levels of Linguistic Adaptation: Selected Papers of the International Pragmatics Conference, Antwerp, 1987. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 118.Google Scholar
Cowley, S. (1994). Conversational functions of rhythmical patterning. Language and Communication, 14(4), 353–76.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cruttenden, A. (1993). The de-accenting and re-accenting of repeated lexical items. In ESCA Workshop on Prosody. www.isca-speech.org/archive_open/archive_papers/prosody_93/pro3_016.pdf.Google Scholar
Cruttenden, A. (1997). Intonation. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Cummins, F. (2002). Speech rhythm and rhythmic taxonomy. In Proceedings of Speech Prosody 2002. www.isca-speech.org/archive/sp2002/sp02_121.html.Google Scholar
Cummins, F. (2009). Rhythm as an affordance for the entrainment of movement. Phonetica, 66(1–2), 1528.Google Scholar
Cummins, F. & Port, R. (1998). Rhythmic constraints on stress timing in English. Journal of Phonetics, 26(2), 145–71.Google Scholar
Cutler, A. & Clifton, C. E. (1984). The use of prosodic information in word recognition. In Bouma, H. & Bouwhuis, D. G., eds., Attention and Performance X. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum, pp. 183–96.Google Scholar
D’Ausilio, A., Bartoli, E. & Maffongelli, L. (2015). Grasping synergies: A motor-control approach to the mirror neuron mechanism. Physics of Life Reviews, 12(1), 91103.Google Scholar
Dauer, R. M. (1983). Stress-timing and syllable-timing reanalyzed. Journal of Phonetics, 11, 5162.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dauer, R. M. (1987). Phonetic and phonological components of language rhythm. In Proceedings of the 11th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences, vol. 5, pp. 447–50.Google Scholar
de Jong, K. J. (1995). The supraglottal articulation of prominence in English: Linguistic stress as localized hyperarticulation. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 97(1), 491504.Google Scholar
Dell, F. (1984). L’accentuation dans les phrases en français. In Dell, F., Hirst, D. J. & Vergnaud, J. R., eds., Forme Sonore Du Langage. Paris: Herman, pp. 65122.Google Scholar
Dellwo, V. (2006). Rhythm and speech rate: A variation coefficient for∆C. In Karnowski, P. & Szigeti, I., eds., Language and Language-Processing. Frankfurt am Main: Peter Lang, pp. 231–41.Google Scholar
Deterding, D. (2001). The measurement of rhythm: A comparison of Singapore and British English. Journal of Phonetics, 29(2), 217–30.Google Scholar
Dryer, M. S. & Haspelmath, M., eds. (2013). WALS Online. Leipzig: Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology.Google Scholar
Echols, C. H., Crowhurst, M. J. & Childers, J. B. (1997). The perception of rhythmic units in speech by infants and adults. Journal of Memory and Language, 36(2), 202–25.Google Scholar
Efron, D. (1941). Gesture and Environment: A Tentative Study of Some of the Spatio-temporal and Linguistic Aspects of the Gestural Behavior of Eastern Jews and Southern Italians in New York City, Living Under Similar as Well as Different Environmental Conditions. Columbia, NY: King’s Crown Press.Google Scholar
Ekman, P. (1999). Emotional and conversational nonverbal signals. In Messing, L. S. & Campbell, R., eds., Gesture, Speech, and Sign. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 3950.Google Scholar
Erickson, F. (1992). They know all the lines: Rhythmic organization and contextualization in a conversational listing routine. In Auer, P. & di Luzio, A., eds., The Contextualization of Language. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 365–97.Google Scholar
Eriksson, A. & Heldner, M. (2015). The acoustics of word stress in English as a function of stress level and speaking style. In 16th Annual Conference of the International Speech Communication Association (INTERSPEECH 2015). Dresden, pp. 41–5.Google Scholar
Féry, C. (2016). Intonation and Prosodic Structure. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Féry, C. & Krifka, M. (2008). Information structure: Notional distinctions, ways of expression. In van Sterkenburg, P., ed., Unity and Diversity of Languages. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 123–35.Google Scholar
Fletcher, J. (2010). The prosody of speech: Timing and rhythm. In Hardcastle, W. J., Laver, J. & Gibbon, F. E., eds., The Handbook of Phonetic Sciences. London: Blackwell, pp. 521602.Google Scholar
Fowler, C. A. (1979). ‘Perceptual centers’ in speech production and perception. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics, 25(5), 375–88.Google Scholar
Fox, A. (2000). Prosodic Features and Prosodic Structure: The Phonology of Suprasegmentals. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fraisse, P. (1963). The Psychology of Time. Translated by Jennifer Leith, New York: Harper Row.Google Scholar
Fraisse, P. (1978). Time and rhythm perception. In Carterette, E. C. & Friedman, M. P., eds., Perceptual Coding. New York: Academic Press, pp. 203–54.Google Scholar
Fry, D. B. (1955). Duration and intensity as physical correlates of linguistic stress. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 27(4), 765–8.Google Scholar
Fry, D. B. (1958). Experiments in the perception of stress. Language and Speech, 1(2), 126–52.Google Scholar
Gordon, M. (2011). Stress: Phonotactic and phonetic evidence. In van Oostendorp, M., Ewen, C. J., Hume, E. V. & Rice, K., eds., The Blackwell Companion to Phonology, 2. London: Wiley-Blackwell, pp. 924–48.Google Scholar
Gordon, M. (2014). Disentangling stress and pitch-accent: A typology of prominence at different prosodic levels. In van der Hulst, H., ed., Word Stress: Theoretical and Typological Issues. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 83118.Google Scholar
Gordon, M. & Roettger, T. (2017). Acoustic correlates of word stress: A cross-linguistic survey. Linguistics Vanguard, 3(1), 404.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gordon, M. & van der Hulst, H. (in press). Word stress system. In Gussenhoven, C. & Chen, A., eds., The Oxford Handbook of Language Prosody. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Goedemans, R. W., Heinz, J. & van der Hulst, H. (2015). StressTyp2, version 1. http://st2.ullet.net.Google Scholar
Grabe, E. & Low, E. L. (2002). Durational variability in speech and the rhythm class hypothesis. In Gussenhoven, C. & Warner, N., eds., Papers in Laboratory Phonology 7. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 515–46.Google Scholar
Graham, C. (1978). Jazz Chants. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Grahn, J. A. (2012). Neural mechanisms of rhythm perception: current findings and future perspectives. Topics in Cognitive Science, 4(4), 585606.Google Scholar
Gussenhoven, C. (1984). On the Grammar and Semantics of Sentence Accents. Dordrecht: Foris.Google Scholar
Gussenhoven, C. (2004). The Phonology of Tone and Intonation. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Halle, M. & Vergnaud, J.-R. (1987). An Essay on Stress. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Halliday, M. A. K. (1967). Intonation and Grammar in British English. The Hague: Mouton.Google Scholar
Harlig, J. & Bardovi-Harlig, K. (1988). Accentuation typology, word order and theme-rheme structure. In Hammond, M., Moravcsik, E. A. & Wirth, J., eds., Studies in Syntactic Typology, Vol. 17. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 125–46.Google Scholar
Hayes, B. (1984). The phonology of rhythm in English. Linguistic Inquiry, 15(1), 3374.Google Scholar
Hayes, B. (1989). Compensatory lengthening in moraic phonology. Linguistic Inquiry, 20, 253306.Google Scholar
Hayes, B. (1995). Metrical Stress Theory: Principles and Case Studies, Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Heinz, J., Goedemans, R. & van der Hulst, H. (2016). Dimensions of Phonological Stress. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Hickok, G., Farahbod, H. & Saberi, K. (2015). The rhythm of perception: Entrainment to acoustic rhythms induces subsequent perceptual oscillation. Psychological Science, 26(7), 1006–13.Google Scholar
Hirst, D. & Di Cristo, A. (1998). Intonation Systems: A Survey of Twenty Languages. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Hyman, L. M. (2006). Word-prosodic typology. Phonology, 23(2), 225–57.Google Scholar
Hyman, L. M. (2014). Do all languages have word accent? In van der Hulst, H., ed., Word Stress: Theoretical and Typological Issues. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 5682.Google Scholar
Inkelas, S. (2011). The interaction between morphology and phonology. In Goldsmith, J., Riggle, J. & Yu, A. C. L., eds., The Handbook of Phonological Theory. Oxford: Wiley-Blackwell, pp. 68102.Google Scholar
James, A. L. (1940). Speech Signals in Telephony. London: Sir Pitman & Sons Limited.Google Scholar
Jassem, W. & Gibbon, D. (1980). Re-defining English accent and stress. Journal of the International Phonetic Association, 10(1–2), 216.Google Scholar
Jefferson, G. (1973). A case of precision timing in ordinary conversation: Overlapped tagpositioned address terms in closing sequences. Semiotica, 9, 4796.Google Scholar
Jefferson, G. (1984). Notes on some orderlinesses of overlap onset. In D’Urso, V. & Leonardi, P., eds., Discourse Analysis and Natural Rhetoric. Padua: Cleup Editore, pp. 1138.Google Scholar
Jones, D. (1967). The Phoneme: Its Nature and Use. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Kager, R. (2007). Feet and metrical stress. In de Lacy, P., ed., The Cambridge Handbook of Phonology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 195227.Google Scholar
Kingdon, R. (1958). The Groundword of English Stress. London: Longman.Google Scholar
Kiparsky, P. (1982). Lexical morphology and phonology. Linguistics in the Morning Calm: Selected Papers from SICOL-1981, 391.Google Scholar
Ladd, D. R. (1978). The Structure of Intonational Meaning, Bloomington, IN: Indiana University Press.Google Scholar
Ladd, D. R. (2008). Intonational Phonology. Cambridge: Cambridge University PressGoogle Scholar
Ladefoged, P. (1967). Three Areas of Experimental Phonetics. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Ladefoged, P., Draper, M. H. & Whitteridge, D. (1958). Syllables and stress. Le Maître Phonétique, 36(73), 114.Google Scholar
Langus, A., Mehler, J. & Nespor, M. (2017). Rhythm in language acquisition. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 81(Pt B), 158–66.Google Scholar
Laver, J. (1994). Principles of Phonetics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Lehiste, I. (1970). Suprasegmentals. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Lehiste, I. (1973). Rhythmic units and syntactic units in production and perception. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 54(5), 1228–34.Google Scholar
Lehiste, I. (1977). Isochrony reconsidered. Journal of Phonetics, 5(3), 253–63.Google Scholar
Leonard, T. & Cummins, F. (2011). The temporal relation between beat gestures and speech. Language and Cognitive Processes, 26(10), 1457–71.Google Scholar
Liberman, M. & Prince, A. (1977). On stress and linguistic rhythm. Linguistic Inquiry, 8(2), 249336.Google Scholar
Lieberman, P. (1960). Some acoustic correlates of word stress in American English. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 32(4), 451–4.Google Scholar
Loehr, D. (2007). Aspects of rhythm in gesture and speech. Gesture, 7(2), 179214.Google Scholar
Loukina, A., Kochanski, G., Rosner, B., Keane, E. & Shih, C. (2011). Rhythm measures and dimensions of durational variation in speech. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 129(5), 3258–70.Google Scholar
McClave, E. (1994). Gestural beats: The rhythm hypothesis. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 23(1), 4566.Google Scholar
McNeill, D. (1992). Hand and Mind: What Gestures Reveal about Thought. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Mohanan, K. P. (2012). The Theory of Lexical Phonology. Berlin: Springer Science & Business Media.Google Scholar
Morton, J. & Jassem, W. (1965). Acoustic correlates of stress. Language and Speech, 8(3), 159–81.Google Scholar
Morton, J., Marcus, S. & Frankish, C. (1976). Perceptual centers (P-centers). Psychological Review, 83(5), 405–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mukai, M. (2006). A Comparative Study of Compound Words in English, Japanese and Mainland Scandinavian. PhD thesis Newcastle University. Retrieved from https://theses.ncl.ac.uk/dspace/handle/10443/243.Google Scholar
Nazzi, T., Bertoncini, J. & Mehler, J. (1998). Language discrimination by newborns: toward an understanding of the role of rhythm. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance, 24(3), 756–66.Google Scholar
Nespor, M. (1990). On the rhythm parameter in phonology. In Roca, I., ed., The Logical Problem of Language Acquisition. Dordrecht: Foris, pp. 157–75.Google Scholar
Nespor, M. & Vogel, I. (1989). On clashes and lapses. Phonology, 6(1), 69116.Google Scholar
Nespor, M. & Vogel, I. (2007). Prosodic Phonology: With a New Foreword. Berlin: Walter de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Newman, S. S. (1946). On the stress system of English. WORD, 2(3), 171–87.Google Scholar
Nolan, F. & Asu, E. L. (2009). The Pairwise Variability Index and coexisting rhythms in language. Phonetica, 66(1–2), 6477.Google Scholar
Nolan, F. & Jeon, H. S. (2014). Speech rhythm: A metaphor? Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 369(1658), 20130396.Google Scholar
O’Connor, J. D. & Arnold, G. F. (1973). Intonation of Colloquial English: A Practical Handbook. London: Longman.Google Scholar
Ogden, R. & Hawkins, S. (2015). Entrainment as a basis for co-ordinated actions in speech. In Proceedings of International Congress of the Phonetic Sciences. University of Glasgow. http://eprints.whiterose.ac.uk/88780/.Google Scholar
Ordin, M. & Polyanskaya, L. (2014). Development of timing patterns in first and second languages. System, 42, 244–57.Google Scholar
Ortiz Lira, H. (1998). Word Stress and Sentence Accent. Santiago de Chile: Universidad Metropolitana de Ciencias de La Educación.Google Scholar
Palmer, H. E. (1924). English Intonation with Systematic Exercises. London: W. Heffer & Sons.Google Scholar
Payne, E., Post, B., Astruc, L., Prieto, P. & Vanrell, M. del M. (2012). Measuring child rhythm. Language and Speech, 55(Pt 2), 203–29.Google Scholar
Pike, K. L. (1945). The Intonation of American English. Ann Arbor, MI: University of Michigan Publications.Google Scholar
Pickering, M. J. & Garrod, S. (2004). Toward a mechanistic psychology of dialogue. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 27, 169225.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Port, R. F., Dalby, J. & O’Dell, M. (1987). Evidence for mora timing in Japanese.Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 81(5), 1574–85.Google Scholar
Poser, W. J. (1984). The Phonetics and Phonology of Tone and Intonation in Japanese. PhD thesis, Massachusetts Institute of Technology. https://dspace.mit.edu/bitstream/handle/1721.1/15169/14709025-MIT.pdf?sequence=2.Google Scholar
Prince, A. (1989). Metrical forms. In Kiparsky, P. & Youmans, G., eds., Rhythm and Meter. San Diego, CA: Academic Press, pp. 4580.Google Scholar
Ramus, F., Nespor, M. & Mehler, J. (1999). Correlates of linguistic rhythm in the speech signal. Cognition, 73(3), 265–92.Google Scholar
Ramus, F., Dupoux, E. & Mehler, J. (2003). The psychological reality of rhythm classes: Perceptual studies. Cogprints. http://cogprints.org/3079/.Google Scholar
Roach, P. (1982). On the distinction between ‘stress-timed’ and ‘syllable-timed’ languages. In Crystal, D., ed., Linguistic Controversies, London: Edward Arnold, pp. 73–9.Google Scholar
Sacks, H., Schegloff, E. A. & Jefferson, G. (1974). A simplest systematics for the organization of turn-taking for conversation. Language, 50, 696735.Google Scholar
Schegloff, E. A. (1984). On some gestures’ relation to talk. In Atkinson, J. H. J., ed., Structures of Social Action: Studies in Conversation Analysis. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 266–95.Google Scholar
Schegloff, E. A. (1998). Reflections on studying prosody in talk-in-interaction. Language and Speech, 41(3/4), 235–63.Google Scholar
Schmerling, S. F. (1976). Aspects of English Sentence Stress. Austin, TX: University of Texas Press.Google Scholar
Scott, S. K. (1998). The point of P-centres. Psychological Research, 6, 14.Google Scholar
Selkirk, E. (1984). Phonology and Syntax: The Relation between Sound and Structure, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Selkirk, E. (2011). The syntax–phonology interface. In Goldsmith, J., Riggle, J. & Yu, A. C. L., eds., The Handbook of Phonological Theory, 2nd ed. Oxford: Wiley-Blackwell, pp. 435–83.Google Scholar
Sluijter, A. (1995). Phonetic Correlates of Stress and Accent. PhD thesis, Holland Institute of Generative Linguistics. https://ci.nii.ac.jp/naid/10022502005/.Google Scholar
Sluijter, A. M. & van Heuven, V. J. (1996). Spectral balance as an acoustic correlate of linguistic stress. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 100(4 Pt 1), 2471–85.Google Scholar
Stetson, R. H. (1928). Motor Phonetics: A Study of Speech Movements in Action, 2nd ed. Amsterdam: North Holland.Google Scholar
Szczepek Reed, B. (2006). Prosodic Orientation in English Conversation, Basingstoke: Palgrave.Google Scholar
Szczepek Reed, B. (2009). Prosodic orientation: A practice for sequence organization in broadcast telephone openings. Journal of Pragmatics, 41(6), 1223–47.Google Scholar
Szczepek Reed, B. (2010). Speech rhythm across turn transitions in cross-cultural talk-in-interaction. Journal of Pragmatics, 42(4), 1037–59.Google Scholar
Szczepek Reed, B. (2011). Analysing Conversation: An Introduction to Prosody, Basingstoke: Palgrave.Google Scholar
Szczepek Reed, B. (2012a). Beyond the particular: Prosody and the coordination of actions. Language and Speech, 55(1), 1233.Google Scholar
Szczepek Reed, B. (2012b). A conversation analytic perspective on teaching English pronunciation: The case of speech rhythm. International Journal of Applied Linguistics, 22(1), 6787.Google Scholar
Teschner, R. V. & Whitley, M. S. (2004). Pronouncing English: A Stress-Based Approach. Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press.Google Scholar
Thaut, M. H., Trimarchi, P. D. & Parsons, L. M. (2014). Human brain basis of musical rhythm perception: common and distinct neural substrates for meter, tempo, and pattern. Brain Sciences, 4(2), 428–52.Google Scholar
Tilsen, S. & Arvaniti, A. (2013). Speech rhythm analysis with decomposition of the amplitude envelope: characterizing rhythmic patterns within and across languages. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 134(1), 628–39.Google Scholar
Trager, G. L. & Smith, H. L. (1951). An Outline of English Structure. Oklahoma, OK: Battenberg Press.Google Scholar
Trouvain, J. & Gut, U., eds. (2007). Non-Native Prosody: Phonetic Description and Teaching Practice. New York: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Van der Hulst, H. (2014a). Representing rhythm. In van der Hulst, H., ed., Word Stress: Theoretical and Typological Issues. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 325–65.Google Scholar
Van der Hulst, H. (2014b). The study of word accent and stress: past, present, and future. In van der Hulst, H., ed., Word Stress: Theoretical and Typological Issues. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 355.Google Scholar
Vaughan-Rees, M. (2010). Rhymes and Rhythm: A Poem-based Course for English Pronunciation. Reading: Garnet Education.Google Scholar
Vos, P. G., Mates, J. & van Kruysbergen, N. W. (1995). The perceptual centre of a stimulus as the cue for synchronization to a metronome: Evidence from asynchronies. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. A, Human Experimental Psychology, 48(4), 1024–40.Google Scholar
White, L. & Mattys, S. L. (2007). Calibrating rhythm: First language and second language studies. Journal of Phonetics, 35(4), 501–22.Google Scholar
Wiget, L., White, L., Schuppler, B., Grenon, I., Rauch, O. & Mattys, S. L. (2010). How stable are acoustic metrics of contrastive speech rhythm? Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 127(3), 1559–69.Google Scholar
Wilson, M. & Wilson, T. P. (2005). An oscillator model of the timing of turn-taking. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12(6), 957–68.Google Scholar
Woodrow, H. (1951). Time perception. In Stevens, S. S., ed., Handbook of Experimental Psychology. New York: Wiley, pp. 1224–36.Google Scholar
Zimmerman, T. P. & West, D. H. (1986). The structure of silence between turns in two-party conversation. Discourse Processes, 9, 375–90.Google Scholar
Zubizarreta, M. L. (1998). Prosody, Focus, and Word Order. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Zubizarreta, M. L. & Vergnaud, J. R. (2006). Phrasal stress and syntax. In Everaert, M. & Riemsdijk, H. van, eds., The Blackwell Companion to Syntax. Malden: Blackwell Publishing, pp. 522–68.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×