Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-5cf477f64f-fcbfl Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-03-29T11:54:31.257Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

1 - A Historical Overview of the Field

from Part I - History of the Field and Theoretical Frameworks

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  12 December 2024

Judith G. Smetana
Affiliation:
University of Rochester, New York
Nicole Campione-Barr
Affiliation:
University of Missouri, Columbia
Lauree C. Tilton-Weaver
Affiliation:
Örebro University

Summary

This chapter profiles a description of the paths that shaped research on parental monitoring and adolescents’ information management. As these areas developed, accounts of the interplay between parents’ attempts to regulate their adolescents’ behavior and adolescents’ responses grew in breadth and in detail. In this chapter, we introduce readers to the constructs and frameworks that have come to represent monitoring and information management research, including related topics that have been probed in diverse attempts to better understand parenting and adolescents’ behaviors. We track developments in the field from the initial challenges to research on parental monitoring, to the rapid shift emphasizing adolescents’ information management and challenging assumptions about monitoring specifically and parental control more generally. Finally, we not how these broad examinations of monitoring and parental control have led to theory development and offer suggestions for continuing these efforts.

Type
Chapter

In this chapter, we provide a relatively brief description of the paths that shaped research on parental monitoring and adolescents’ information management. As researchers explored and examined these constructs in ever greater detail, the landscape of these areas morphed into increasingly broader and more detailed accounts of the interplay between parents’ attempts to regulate their adolescents’ behavior and adolescents’ responses. Here, we introduce readers to the constructs and frameworks that have come to represent monitoring and information management research. This includes related topics, probed by researchers in their diverse attempts to better understand parent and adolescent behaviors. More specifically, we start with describing the beginnings of the collective body of research referred to in this chapter – challenging the literature on parental monitoring. We then move to the subsequent groundswell of research on adolescent information management, through to challenging assumptions about monitoring specifically and parental control in general. We end the chapter with some notes about how this broad reexamination of monitoring and parental control has led to theory development and some suggestions for how to continue this work. We do not go into great depth about the topics we introduce, as much is covered in other chapters of this handbook. Instead, we refer you to those chapters for more detailed accounts.

Where It All Began

In the late 1990s, several researchers (including the authors) started to present and publish research that was instigated by their dissatisfaction with the research on parental monitoring. Conceptualized as a parenting practice involving parents’ attempts to gain knowledge of their youths’ whereabouts and activities when away from adults, monitoring was framed as a behavior that protected against delinquency. Monitoring was viewed from several theoretical positions. Some saw it as representing the “demanding” dimension of Baumrind’s parenting styles (e.g., Baumrind, Reference Baumrind1978; Steinberg et al., Reference Steinberg, Elmen and Mounts1989, Reference Steinberg, Mounts, Lamborn and Dornbusch1991). Others described it as a form of behavioral control (Barber, Reference Barber1996), drawing attention to how it differed from psychological control. Prevention/intervention programs had been designed and promoted with focus on increasing parental monitoring (Dishion & McMahon, Reference Dishion and McMahon1998; Dishion et al., Reference Dishion, Nelson and Kavanagh2003).

The notion that parental monitoring would reduce youth engagement in delinquent behaviors spread. It was popular enough among researchers, practitioners, and the general public to gain credence at global levels. The World Health Organization included measures of monitoring in their assessments of child development, measures that were of questionable use, because parents’ monitoring behaviors were not actually assessed. Television advertisements advised parents to monitor with catchy voice-overs of parents commenting on scenes of adolescents and friends smoking, “My kid doesn’t smoke. How do I know? I ask!” or implied concerns that adolescents were not being monitored: “It’s 10 o’clock – Do you know where your children are?”

Against the backdrop of surging popularity, the theoretical and empirical record was fraught with untested assumptions, many of which were common to the perspectives described. These assumptions included the expectation that parental monitoring – a parent-directed process – unidirectionally affected adolescents’ delinquent behavior. This was dubious, given research on what adolescents contribute to the parenting process. This includes research showing that parents react to their children’s characteristics (child effects, Bell, Reference Bell1968), that parenting is a bidirectional processes, interdependent among family members (Lollis & Kuczynski, Reference Lollis and Kuczynski1997), and directed by adolescents (e.g., transactional models, Fiese & Sameroff, Reference Fiese and Sameroff1989). Moreover, up until the time the monitoring construct was being questioned, most of the research was based on cross-sectional data, which severely limited interpretations of directionality and causality.

Reexamining Monitoring, Measuring Information Management

To walk readers through the developing areas of research, we begin by describing these assumptions and the ways that researchers were showing the problems that the assumptions created. We start with the definition of monitoring and how it was measured. In the original formulations, monitoring was defined as a parenting behavior directed at finding out what adolescents were doing when they were not being directly supervised by parents or another adult. Described as “attention to and tracking of the child’s whereabouts, activities, and adaptations” (Dishion & McMahon, Reference Dishion and McMahon1998, p. 61) or “the extent the parent is aware of the child’s whereabouts, his deviant behavior in and outside of home, and the degree the parent supervises the child’s activity” (Patterson & Dishion, Reference Patterson and Dishion1985, p. 69), the idea was that parents monitor to find out whether their adolescents’ unsupervised whereabouts, companions, and activities conformed to parents’ expectations and directives. Theoretically, parents who found out that their adolescents were with disreputable companions or engaged in misbehavior could then correct the behavior (possibly through punishment) or reduce contact with problematic peers (Dishion et al., Reference Dishion, Nelson and Kavanagh2003; Mounts, Reference Mounts2002). However, in most studies, monitoring was measured by asking adolescents “How much do your parents REALLY know about” their activities and whereabouts (Steinberg et al., Reference Steinberg, Fletcher and Darling1994, p. 1061). Alternatively, parents could be asked how much they knew about the same issues. Some compared what parents and adolescents reported about adolescents’ behavior (e.g., Crouter et al., Reference Crouter, MacDermid, McHale and Perry-Jenkins1990). The problem with these measures of parents’ knowledge is that they did not tap parental behaviors; instead, they assessed cognitions. The lack of concordance between definitions and measures revealed the unstated and untested assumption that parents who monitored would know where their adolescents were, whom they were with, and what they were doing. By extension, this measurement also implied that parents who do not monitor will not have that knowledge. In essence, in this research, monitoring was equated to parental knowledge. Both assumptions were concerning.

Researchers began questioning these assumptions, asserting that knowledge was not equal to monitoring (Darling et al., Reference Darling, Cumsille, Caldwell and Dowdy2006; Kerr & Stattin, Reference Kerr and Stattin2000; Marshall et al., Reference Marshall, Tilton-Weaver and Bosdet2005; Tilton-Weaver & Galambos, Reference Tilton‐Weaver and Galambos2003). From the standpoint of researchers considering child effects and bidirectionality, the idea that adolescents would passively accept and comply with parents’ demands failed to account for research showing noncompliance even in the toddler years (e.g., Kuczynski & Kochanska, Reference Kuczynski and Kochanska1990).

With other researchers, we argued that adolescents played an active role in determining what their parents knew or did not know. The studies began, with new measures created to assess actual monitoring behaviors (Kerr & Stattin, Reference Kerr and Stattin2000). This work, carried out with a sample of fourteen-year-old Swedish boys and girls, included parents’ requests for information (i.e., solicitation) and rules intended to gain information about adolescents’ whereabouts and activities (called parental control by Kerr & Stattin, Reference Kerr and Stattin2000; monitoring rules by Tilton-Weaver et al., Reference Tilton-Weaver, Burk, Kerr and Stattin2013). These pieces of research drew attention to another fact: Monitoring was multidimensional, rather than a single factor.

Importantly, monitoring also differs depending on what aspect of adolescents’ lives is being examined. Drawing from research on parent–adolescent conflict, Smetana and colleagues (Smetana et al., Reference Smetana, Metzger, Gettman and Campione‐Barr2006, Reference Smetana, Villalobos, Tasopoulos-Chan, Gettman and Campione-Barr2009; also see Chapter 3 in this volume) convincingly showed that the social domain had to be considered, as lower to middle class US parents and adolescent boys and girls often disagree about what parents should attempt to regulate. This research, framed by social-cognitive domain theory (Turiel et al., Reference Turiel, Smetana, Killen, Kurtines and Gewirtz1991), showed that attempts to monitor and regulate behaviors that adolescents viewed as personal could generate resistance and efforts to undermine parents’ monitoring. Thus, our notions of the scope of monitoring behaviors became more nuanced.

At the same time, other measures were created to tap adolescents’ willingness to tell their parents about issues pertinent to monitoring (Marshall et al., Reference Marshall, Tilton-Weaver and Bosdet2005) and their actual disclosure (Kerr & Stattin, Reference Kerr and Stattin2000). Collectively, we and other researchers asked early, middle, and late adolescents from the United States and Europe to tell us what their parents knew and why they knew it; what they were willing to tell their parents and what kinds of information they were unwilling to provide; how they provided or withheld information from their parents (e.g., Darling et al., Reference Darling, Cumsille, Caldwell and Dowdy2006; Finkenauer et al., Reference Finkenauer, Engels and Meeus2002; Marshall et al., Reference Marshall, Tilton-Weaver and Bosdet2005; Smetana et al., Reference Smetana, Metzger, Gettman and Campione‐Barr2006). We called this information management (Marshall et al., Reference Marshall, Tilton-Weaver and Bosdet2005) in recognition of what studies were revealing: Adolescents actively and strategically manage the information their parents can gain from them.

This body of research on information management expanded to include other forms of revealing, including providing partial information (Smetana et al., Reference Smetana, Villalobos, Tasopoulos-Chan, Gettman and Campione-Barr2009), disclosing only if asked (Laird et al., Reference Laird, Marrero, Melching and Kuhn2013), and forced disclosure (Kearney & Bussey, Reference Kearney and Bussey2015), in recognition that disclosure of monitoring-relevant information did not have to be “all or nothing” and that disclosure was not always voluntary. The latter is an important distinction, as Kerr and Stattin mistakenly claimed that their measure assessed voluntary disclosure, when adolescents’ willingness to disclose was assessed in only one of their items (Stattin & Kerr, Reference Stattin and Kerr2000).

The first record of measures involving volition was by Waizenhofer and colleagues (Waizenhofer et al., Reference Waizenhofer, Buchanan and Jackson-Newsom2004); asked adolescents in the United States (aged 10–17 years) about their provisions of information without being asked (calling it passive monitoring). In addition, researchers began to either refer to Kerr’s measure of disclosure as “self-disclosure” or using self-disclosure scales to tap what adolescents told parents about their lives. This conflated disclosure relevant to monitoring with a different interpersonal process. This happened often enough that it prompted us, with Darling, to provide a theoretical review where we sought to delineate the differences and commonalities between disclosure related to monitoring issues (i.e., routine disclosure) and self-disclosure (Tilton-Weaver et al., Reference Tilton‐Weaver, Marshall and Darling2014).

In addition to new measures of disclosure, researchers were dealing with the nuances of concealing, distinguishing between lying (first used in Marshall et al., Reference Marshall, Tilton-Weaver and Bosdet2005), keeping secrets (identified by Frijns et al., Reference Frijns, Keijsers, Branje and Meeus2010 as part of Kerr & Stattin’s measure of disclosure), and throughavoiding talking about an issue (Cumsille et al., Reference Cumsille, Darling and Martínez2010). Lying was largely construed as provisions of false information, in an attempt to deceive (Marshall et al., Reference Marshall, Tilton-Weaver and Bosdet2005), but is now recognized as including lying by omission, which overlaps conceptually with keeping secrets (to read more about these issues, see Chapters 1114, this volume).

Qualitative analyses also revealed that middle adolescents living in the United States used information management to serve their own goals (Marshall et al., Reference Marshall, Tilton-Weaver and Bosdet2005; Tilton-Weaver & Marshall, Reference Tilton-Weaver, Marshall, Kerr, Stattin and Engels2008). Moreover, these goals were sometimes, but not always, about avoiding punishment for engaging in behaviors that they knew their parents would not like. Adolescents indicated they would disclose when they felt that their parents had jurisdiction over the behavior or wanted to make sure their parents were available for instrumental or social support. Others indicated they would provide information only if their parents asked or otherwise looked for information. They would provide less information (or partial information) when they deemed it unnecessary (e.g., going out with friends to known places, with an expected time to return), but also to evade unwanted parental control. Thus, what they revealed or concealed, as well as how much and when they revealed or concealed, was tied to their goals.

Others were showing similarly rich pictures, that early and middle adolescents’ information management was also predicated on their belief that their parents’ monitoring was legitimate and that they were obligated to provide information (Smetana et al., Reference Smetana, Metzger, Gettman and Campione‐Barr2006). Clearly, the picture was more complicated than originally described.

Tests of the assumption that measuring knowledge would be equal to measuring parental behavior were also underway. The tests were relatively simple: What predicted parental knowledge? Parental monitoring or adolescents’ information management? At first, research findings from samples from the United States, Chile, and the Philippines pointed to knowledge being predicated on adolescents’ providing information – parents knew when adolescents revealed, but not if adolescents concealed (Darling et al., Reference Darling, Cumsille, Caldwell and Dowdy2006, Reference Darling, Cumsille, Peña‐Alampay and Coatsworth2009). In essence, parents who tried to monitor did not always gain information. Later evidence with a large sample of Chilean adolescents would also show that some disclose virtually everything to their parents, even those who have not been actively monitored (Cumsille et al., Reference Cumsille, Darling and Martínez2010). The conclusion was that parental knowledge was a poor proxy for monitoring behaviors.

Researchers turned then to trying to understand how parents gained knowledge, other than from adolescents’ disclosure. Could they look for information elsewhere and expect to gain it? Looking at sources of knowledge other than (or in comparison to) adolescents’ revealing or concealing, some researchers found that parents might be able to gain information from each other (as adolescents may disclose to one, but not both; Waizenhofer et al., Reference Waizenhofer, Buchanan and Jackson-Newsom2004). Researchers explored the conditions under which monitoring led to disclosure. When examined with better measures, accounts became more nuanced, showing conditions under which adolescents were more or less inclined to provide information (e.g., when parents are supportive, Baudat et al., Reference Baudat, Van Petegem, Antonietti and Zimmerman2020). Readers can find an up-to-date account of the sources of parental knowledge in Buchanan and Selçuk (Chapter 6, this volume). As researchers were documenting the variation in adolescents’ information management, researchers were also exploring to whom adolescents disclosed, documenting that disclosures to siblings were part of family-level processes involved in monitoring (Campione-Barr et al., Reference Campione-Barr, Lindell, Giron, Killoren and Greer2015). They found what might be a developmental shift in how much adolescents in the United States disclosed to siblings relative to mothers, where early adolescents disclosed more to mothers than siblings, middle adolescents disclosing more to mothers only about personal and prudential issues, and older adolescents disclosing equally to mothers and siblings. They also found that disclosure to siblings about personal and multifaceted issues were sometimes linked to symptoms of depression, depending on gender, birth order, and who was disclosing to whom. For example, boys disclosing to sisters reported fewer symptoms, but girls whose brothers were disclosing reported more symptoms. Nondisclosure in family and other relationships have also been compared in ethnically diverse US families (Guo et al., Reference Guo, Killoren and Campione-Barr2022), where age-related differences again emerged, with older adolescents keeping more information from family members than younger. This topic is covered in the chapter by Campione-Barr and colleagues (Chapter 14, this volume).

Researchers were also interested in how parents reacted to disclosure, particularly when adolescents disclosed about misconduct. Unsurprisingly, angry reactions do not seem to help (see for example, studies of middle adolescents in Sweden: Kerr & Stattin, Reference Kerr, Stattin, Crouter and Booth2003; Tilton-Weaver et al., Reference Tilton-Weaver, Kerr, Pakalniskeine, Tokic, Salihovic and Stattin2010; see also Main & Disla, Chapter 9, this volume).

Monitoring and Delinquent Behavior

The focus on reinterpreting monitoring also meant that attention was given to the assumption that monitoring would prevent or reduce delinquent behavior and association with delinquent peers. Unfortunately, many of the studies attempting to show that monitoring was related to reductions in delinquent behaviors used the knowledge measure (e.g., De Kemp et al., Reference De Kemp, Scholte, Overbeek and Engels2006) and/or used data collected in a single wave (e.g., Bowman et al., Reference Bowman, Prelow and Weaver2007; Caldwell et al., Reference Caldwell, Beutler, Ross and Silver2006), even after these limitations were criticized. Others attempted to show that monitoring increased knowledge, in turn reducing delinquency (e.g., Fletcher et al., Reference Fletcher, Steinberg and Williams‐Wheeler2004), but assessed monitoring and knowledge within the same wave of data collection, calling directionality into question.

The need for longitudinal data became very apparent at this point, when researchers were questioning the directionality of the links between monitoring, information management, and delinquent behaviors. In fact, one of the enduring problems is that the statistical models being tested often do not fully account for the possibility of bidirectionality. Part of resolving this issue is developing more sophisticated ways of modeling directionality, which readers can find addressed in Chapter 5 by Keijsers in this handbook.

Indeed, when researchers began to use more appropriate measures and longitudinal data, a different picture emerged. This is in part because researchers were looking at the potential for bidirectional associations among monitoring, information management, and adolescent adjustment. Assessing developmental patterns of monitoring, information management, and delinquent behavior from ages thirteen to sixteen years, Keijsers and colleagues (Reference Keijsers, Frijns, Branje and Meeus2009) found that parents in the Netherlands relaxed their control over time as delinquent behavior increased and disclosure diminished. These temporal patterns suggest that coordination between parents and adolescents, rather than monitoring itself, offer protection from engagement in delinquency. However, others modeling a single direction find that there may be some conditions under which monitoring works. For example, Laird and his colleagues (Laird et al., Reference Laird, Marrero and Sentse2010) found that from the perspectives of early adolescents from the United States, mothers’ solicitation was related to decreases in delinquent behavior when adolescents either reported having a lot of unsupervised time or when their beliefs about parents’ legitimacy were weak.

Protection from engaging with delinquent peers was also proposed as a means by which parental monitoring reduces delinquent behaviors (Fletcher et al., Reference Fletcher, Darling, Steinberg and McCord1995) or allows parents to intervene in peer relationships when they became aware of them. However, many used the knowledge measure (e.g., Mounts, Reference Mounts2002), mixed monitoring and disclosure items (e.g., Barnes et al., Reference Barnes, Hoffman, Welte, Farrell and Dintcheff2006), or used data collected in a single wave (e.g., Kiesner et al., Reference Kiesner, Poulin and Dishion2010).

Examining the connections among parental monitoring, delinquency, and peers introduced other forms of bias. Many have used adolescents’ reports on their friends’ delinquency (e.g., Barnes et al., Reference Barnes, Hoffman, Welte, Farrell and Dintcheff2006). This bias is problematic, as adolescents tend to estimate their friends’ behaviors as more similar to their own than they are in reality (Kandel, Reference Kandel1996; known as assumed similarity bias, Cronbach, Reference Cronbach1955). Kandel (Reference Kandel1996) also pointed out that peer influence is often overestimated when the selection of friends is not separated from the influence of friends. These issues called for examining the question with reports taken from friends and with statistical models that can separate selection from influence (e.g., social network analysis). In research using such methods, there has been only limited support for the idea that monitoring reduces contact with delinquent peers, at least among community samples of Swedish adolescents (Tilton-Weaver et al., Reference Tilton-Weaver, Burk, Kerr and Stattin2013). For example, no support has been found for the belief that soliciting information reduces the selection or influence of delinquent peers. Monitoring rules were not linked to peer influence and only linked to selection of less delinquent peers among older adolescent cohorts. Much like research on delinquent behavior, in younger adolescent cohorts, monitoring rules were related to selecting more delinquent peers. Like other studies, these results suggest that monitoring may not be particularly effective and could have unintended consequences. Another construct examined was parents’ communicating disapproval – testing the mechanism by which monitoring and knowledge should reduce peer selection or influence (Tilton-Weaver et al., Reference Tilton-Weaver, Burk, Kerr and Stattin2013). This too was mixed in terms of potential effectiveness. Among middle adolescents, communicating disapproval increased the likelihood of befriending a delinquent peer. By contrast, among late adolescents communicating disapproval reduced the influence of delinquent peers, but only for those reporting higher levels of delinquent behavior. Among those who reported lower rates, communicating disapproval was related to increased peer influence.

In other research, early to middle Irish adolescents’ secrecy contributed to shaping peer relationships related to delinquency (McCann et al., Reference McCann, Jordan, Higgins and Moore2019), rather than parental control. Together, these pieces of research suggest that monitoring has limits to its effectiveness and is not as broadly useful as implied by the early research using knowledge measures.

Broadening Horizons

Collectively, many of the researchers engaged in studying parental monitoring concluded that the associations previously found by researchers examining parental knowledge and delinquency (e.g., Jacobson & Crockett, Reference Jacobson and Crockett2000; Mounts, Reference Mounts2002; Steinberg et al., Reference Steinberg, Fletcher and Darling1994) were tapping something more than parents’ active monitoring. We collectively also suspected that knowledge was more of a proxy for adolescents’ information management than for monitoring. Given that research was now suggesting that parental monitoring could also backfire, researchers moved on to examining the limits of parental monitoring and the nexus of parents’ goals (e.g., keeping their adolescents safe) and adolescents’ perspectives.

Some of the questions that emerged related to whether parents could monitor too much. Some theorists, best represented by Barber and colleagues (Barber et al., Reference Barber, Olsen and Shagle1994), suggested that it was the absence of behavioral control (for which parental knowledge was an oft-used measure; Barber et al., Reference Barber, Olsen and Shagle1994) that was connected to delinquency. Moreover, they argued that behavioral control, including monitoring, was theoretically distinct from psychological control in its goals and its outcomes.

These ideas came under more scrutiny. Here we review a few examples of research testing the idea that the absence of behavioral control and presence of psychological control created conditions that increased adolescents’ maladjustment. Using an experimental design with hypothetical vignettes (inspired by Pomerantz & Eaton, Reference Pomerantz and Eaton2000) manipulating moderate and high levels of control across personal and prudential domains, Kakihara and Tilton-Weaver (Reference Kakihara and Tilton‐Weaver2009) examined US middle adolescents’ interpretations of behavioral and psychological control in terms of what it would mean for adolescents’ competence, their connectedness to parents, and parents’ intrusiveness. Although not originally conceived this way, it was clear that these interpretations patterned onto Ryan and Deci’s (Reference Ryan and Deci2000) three nutriments of self-determination theory (SDT): competence, relatedness, and psychological autonomy. The results showed that adolescents interpreted high levels of behavioral and psychological control as indicating that they would matter less to their parents and that the depicted parents were intrusive. The most negative interpretations were generated when the depicted parents were highly controlling over personal domain issues, as was suggested by Smetana and Daddis (Reference Smetana and Daddis2002). In other words, at high levels, and particularly over personal issues, behavioral control and psychological control were indistinguishable in terms of adolescents’ interpretations. These ideas were then applied to data from a community sample of Swedish early and middle adolescents’ reporting on their parents’ monitoring, their perceptions of needs satisfaction, and their adjustment (Kakihara et al., Reference Kakihara, Tilton-Weaver, Kerr and Stattin2010). Results showed that rules, while not significantly related to adolescents’ perceptions of autonomy (indexed as feeling overcontrolled) and relatedness (assessed as feeling connected to parents), directly predicted declines in norm-breaking but also declines in self-esteem. Restrictive forms of monitoring were related to increases in feeling overcontrolled by parents. We interpreted this as frustration of autonomy needs, which was subsequently related to increases in norm-breaking, increases in depressive symptoms, and drops in self-esteem. The same paths were found for love withdrawal.

Taken together, these studies suggested that parents could use too much behavioral control and when their monitoring became restrictive, behavioral control looked much like what Barber had described as psychological control (since referred to as intrusive parenting, Barber, Reference Barber2002; and more recently, disrespect, Barber et al., Reference Barber, Xia, Olsen, McNeely and Bose2012). Through research questioning the limits of behavioral control, monitoring and information management became tied to ideas about intrusive parenting (more on this topic can be found in Chapters 2, 7, and 8 by Hawk and Peng, Padilla-Walker et al., and Rote et al., in this volume).

These types of studies, showing the consistency in signs that monitoring could be counterproductive, opened up areas not previously considered in monitoring research. For example, psychological reactance (Brehm, Reference Brehm1966) became a working idea about mechanisms of adolescents’ resistance to parental monitoring (e.g., Van Petegem et al., Reference Van Petegem, Soenens, Vansteenkiste and Beyers2015). As elaborated by Gingo (Chapter 13, this volume), other forms of resistance inspired researchers’ thinking.

As researchers thought about monitoring being intrusive, the limits of monitoring and behavioral control were further probed. Privacy issues, previously subsumed under the personal domain of social-cognitive domain theory (Smetana, Chapter 3; Turiel et al., Reference Turiel, Smetana, Killen, Kurtines and Gewirtz1991), garnered increasing interest. Using ideas from communications research (most notably Petronio, Reference Petronio, Cupach and Spitzberg1994), researchers started to think about some forms and levels of monitoring as invasions of adolescents’ privacy. Led by Hawk’s work with early adolescents in the Netherlands (e.g., Hawk et al., Reference Hawk, Hale, Raaijmakers and Meeus2008, Reference Hawk, Keijsers, Hale and Meeus2009), this body of work showed that parents sometimes invade adolescents’ privacy, at times intentionally and at other times, unintentionally (see Hawk & Peng, Chapter 2 of this volume, for an in-depth review).

The important picture coming from this body of research was that parents’ and adolescents’ goals and needs can conflict with each other, such that one party’s needs might be met while the other’s is not. This was not a new idea; it can be seen in research using social-cognitive domain theory, in research probing differences and similarities between behavioral and psychological control, and in research examining discrepancies between parent and adolescent reports (e.g., De Los Reyes et al., Reference De Los Reyes, Goodman, Kliewer and Reid-Quinones2010). Generally speaking, societies expect parents to keep their children safe from harm; to do otherwise would be neglectful or abusive. Societies also expect parents to limit the harm their children can inflict on others. When children and adolescents inflict harm on others or their property, many people ask about what parents are doing to limit such behaviors. Thus, there are expectations that parents will regulate behavior. Juxtaposed with these safety and security needs are adolescents’ needs for autonomy and self-direction. Researchers generally recognize that the desire for independent and self-directed behavior increases across adolescence (Zimmer-Gembeck & Collins, Reference Zimmer-Gembeck, Collins, Adams and Berzonsky2008). At the heart of privacy invasions are these conflicting needs – parents who are worried and concerned about their adolescents can deliberately invade privacy in attempts to gain the very information that adolescents want to keep to themselves (Hawk et al., Reference Hawk, Becht and Branje2016). However, this does not characterize all intentional invasions of privacy. Sometimes parents simply feel they have the right to the information when adolescents disagree (Chan et al., Reference Chan, Brown and Von Bank2015; Rote & Smetana, Reference Rote and Smetana2016; Tilton-Weaver & Trost, Reference Tilton-Weaver and Trost2012).

The studies of privacy invasions can be summarized as showing that under some conditions, monitoring can be viewed as “hypo-parenting” (Pedersen, Reference Pedersen2013), connecting it to other forms of parenting that may be meant to be protective, such as “helicopter parenting” (Padilla-Walker et al., Reference Padilla-Walker and Nelson2012) and “curling” (Hougaard, Reference Hougaard2004). This drove research into even more diverse directions. In particular, researchers began to question what monitoring looked like in the Digital Age (see Beyens et al., Chapter 10 of this volume, for more). Questions about what adolescents are doing online and how parents would know, as well as questions about whether tracking applications are another form of invasive behavior.

With related topics opening venues for exploration, others had already shown the importance of context, especially ethnic and cultural variation. What is clear is that adolescents vary across ethnicities and cultures in how they manage information and what information they provide to their parents (Bakken & Brown, Reference Bakken and Brown2010; Nucci et al., Reference Nucci, Smetana, Araki, Nakaue and Comer2014; Tasopoulos-Chan et al., Reference Tasopoulos-Chan, Smetana and Yau2009). Chapters 2, 15, and 16, in this volume by Hawk and Peng, Dost-Gözkan, and Killoren et al. provide more insight into variations across cultural contexts.

As questions about contexts arose, so did the desire to understand what monitoring and information management looked like when there were special conditions such as monitoring adolescents and young adults with chronic illnesses or disabilities (e.g., Ellis et al., Reference Ellis, Templin, Naar-King and Frey2008). In such populations, monitoring and information management have been applied to disease management and medical adherence (e.g., Hilliard et al., Reference Hilliard, Holmes, Chen, Maher, Robinson and Streisand2013) to understand how information necessary for health management is shared between parents and adolescents. Today, such research includes LGBTQIA+ (lesbian, gay, bisexual, transgender, queer or questioning, intersex, asexual, and more) populations (e.g., Montano et al., Reference Montano, McCauley, Miller, Chisolm and Marshal2017). Chapters 17, 18, and 19 in this handbook by McCurdy and Russell, Berg and Marion, and Darling cover these topics for readers who are interested in knowing more.

Other avenues for rethinking parental monitoring and information management were introduced by bringing in other theoretical frameworks. Soenens and colleagues (e.g., Soenens & Vansteenkiste, Reference Soenens and Vansteenkiste2010; see also Soenens and Vansteenkiste, Chapter 4, this volume) focused first on achievement, showing that SDT (Ryan & Deci, Reference Ryan and Deci2000) provided a way of understanding the importance of supporting psychological autonomy. As psychological control was a focal issue in their framework, this construct garnered more attention than did monitoring. Nonetheless, research on monitoring using SDT has emerged, but with cross-sectional samples (e.g., Rodríguez-Meirinhos et al., Reference Rodríguez-Meirinhos, Vansteenkiste, Soenens, Oliva, Brenning and Antolín-Suárez2020 using community samples of early to middle adolescents in Spain).

The shift in focus from delinquent behavior to psychological autonomy added new parenting constructs, notably autonomy supportive parenting. The use of the term autonomy support was a deliberate attempt to address parenting that was not controlling (see Grolnick et al., Reference Grolnick, Gurland, DeCourcey and Jacob2002), and differentiated autonomy support from both behavioral and psychological control. An important advance, in this literature, was to differentiate supporting (psychological) autonomy from supporting independence (Ryan et al., Reference Ryan, Deci, Grolnick, La Guardia, Ciccheti and Cohen2015).

At the same time that SDT was being applied to the questions surrounding parental control and adolescent autonomy, we saw that another area that needed attention was defining autonomy. Despite past efforts to delineate emotional, cognitive, and behavioral autonomy from each other, there were problems with the tendency to define behavioral autonomy as independence (e.g., Peterson, Reference Peterson1986; Simmons & Blythe, Reference Simmons and Blyth1987). First, equating behavioral autonomy with independence meant that delinquent behaviors were viewed as autonomy. This working definition failed to acknowledge the boundaries that societies place around individual actions, as indicated by taboos, mores, laws, and social conventions. A second issue was the lack of attention to the daily transactional processes between parents and children as regulation of actions is gradually transferred from parent to child. We described this as governance transfer (Tilton-Weaver & Marshall, Reference Tilton-Weaver, Marshall, Soenens, Vansteenkiste and Van Petegem2017). As readers can see, what started as dissatisfaction with a single construct grew to encompass much more.

Conclusions and Future Directions

Authors of chapters in this handbook and others have made considerable advances in understanding what is encompassed in parental monitoring and adolescents’ information management. Having been part of this journey from the beginning, we hope that researchers continue to uncover the nuances we and others have missed. Despite the advances, we have two enduring concerns that we argue need to be addressed. The first has to do with measurement. Although researchers interested in these topics have come a long way in improving conceptualization, operationalization, and measurement, there is still much that can be improved. For example, we speculate that much more needs to be known about solicitation and monitoring rules (control). How and when are rules constructed? Are rules more effective when they are collaboratively constructed, rather than parent directives? We suspect that the answer is yes, given what the research on autonomy support has taught us. We also suspect that when rules are established before adolescence, earlier in childhood, they become part of family routines, and are much more likely to be complied with as a result. Measurement should address these nuances.

Other details also are worth addressing. At least a decade ago, a process model of monitoring was proposed (Hayes et al., Reference Hayes, Hudson and Matthews2003), suggesting that monitoring was a series of parent–child interactions, occurring before and after adolescents went about their daily lives. Parents ask about what adolescents are planning to do and then follow up to find out what they actually did. Related to this, we have suspected that rules have a similar process – rules are laid out, but also require parents to follow up, making determinations as to when adolescents have failed to comply and what the consequences should be. To our knowledge, such interactions have never been documented. Because these details are missing from empirical accounts, missing too is how these aspects of monitoring are related to adolescents’ information management and adjustment.

The final concern is about testing direction and causality. From the beginning, we were concerned that too much was inferred about directionality from cross-sectional data and too much about causality was inferred from longitudinal data. Indeed, some of the studies we cite here have used cross-sectional and longitudinal data that, while helpful in some ways, cannot answer some of the questions about causality.

In closing, we note that this volume represents, to a large extent, the current state of the field. The collective body of knowledge has broadened, from questioning what was being assessed in a measure of parental knowledge, to considering the role adolescents play in their own parenting, to viewing parental control and adolescent information management as dynamic and nuanced issues, informed by considering other relationships in and outside of the family. The theoretical framing of the area has grown from its origins in behaviorism to acknowledging the foundational roles of parents’ and adolescents’ cognitions and emotions, motivations and goals. Research has also expanded to include non-Western samples, but more is needed. Recognizing that the field is as dynamic as human development, we believe research can be improved, continuing as it started by attending to the limits of our data. We encourage others to continue the work, as we applaud the efforts made by so many who took this journey with us.

References

Bakken, J. P., & Brown, B. B. (2010). Adolescent secretive behavior: African American and Hmong adolescents’ strategies and justifications for managing parents’ knowledge about peers. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 20(2), 359388. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1532-7795.2010.00642.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barber, B. K. (1996). Parental psychological control: Revisiting a neglected construct. Child Development, 67(6), 32963319. https://doi.org/10.2307/1131780CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barber, B. K. (Ed.). (2002). Intrusive parenting: How psychological control affects children and adolescents. American Psychological Association. https://doi.org/10.1037/10422-000CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barber, B. K., Olsen, J. E., & Shagle, S. C. (1994). Association between parental psychological control and behavioral control and youth internalized and externalized behaviors. Child Development, 65, 11201136. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1994.tb00807.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barber, B. K., Xia, M., Olsen, J. A., McNeely, C. A., & Bose, K. (2012). Feeling disrespected by parents: Refining the measurement and understanding of psychological control. Journal of Adolescence, 35(2), 273287. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2011.10.010CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barnes, G. M., Hoffman, J. H., Welte, J. W., Farrell, M. P., & Dintcheff, B. A. (2006). Effects of parental monitoring and peer deviance on substance use and delinquency. Journal of Marriage and Family, 68(4), 10841104. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1741-3737.2006.00315.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baudat, S., Van Petegem, S., Antonietti, J. P., & Zimmerman, G. (2020). Parental solicitation and adolescents’ information management: The moderating role of autonomy-supportive parenting. Journal of Child & Family Studies, 29, 426441. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10826-019-01687-zCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baumrind, D. (1978). Parental disciplinary patterns and social competence in children. Youth and Society, 9(3), 239276. https://doi.org/10.1177/0044118X7800900302CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bell, R. Q. (1968). A reinterpretation of the direction of effects in studies of socialization. Psychological Review, 75(2), 8195. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0025583CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bowman, M. A., Prelow, H. M., & Weaver, S. R. (2007). Parenting behaviors, association with deviant peers, and delinquency in African American adolescents: A mediated-moderation model. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 36, 517527. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-006-9117-7CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brehm, J. W. (1966). A theory of psychological reactance. Academic Press.Google Scholar
Caldwell, R. M., Beutler, L. E., Ross, S. A., & Silver, N. C. (2006). Brief report: An examination of the relationships between parental monitoring, self-esteem and delinquency among Mexican-American male adolescents. Journal of Adolescence, 29(3), 459464. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2005.07.005CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Campione-Barr, N., Lindell, A. K., Giron, S. E., Killoren, S. E., & Greer, K. B. (2015). Domain differentiated disclosure to mothers and siblings and associations with sibling relationship quality and youth emotional adjustment. Developmental Psychology, 51(9), 12781291. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000036CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chan, H. Y., Brown, B. B., & Von Bank, H. (2015). Adolescent disclosure of information about peers: The mediating role of perceptions of parents’ right to know. Journal of Youth & Adolescence, 44, 10481065. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-015-0261-9CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cronbach, L. J. (1955). Processes affecting scores on “understanding of others” and “assumed similarity.” Psychological Bulletin, 52(3), 177193. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0044919CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crouter, A. C., MacDermid, S. M., McHale, S. M., & Perry-Jenkins, M. (1990). Parental monitoring and perceptions of children’s school performance and conduct in dual-and single-earner families. Developmental Psychology, 26(4), 649657. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.26.4.649CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cumsille, P., Darling, N., & Martínez, M. L. (2010). Shading the truth: The patterning of adolescents’ decisions to avoid issues, disclose, or lie to parents. Journal of Adolescence, 33(2), 285296. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2009.10.008CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Darling, N., Cumsille, P., Caldwell, L. L., & Dowdy, B. (2006). Predictors of adolescents’ disclosure to parents and perceived parental knowledge: Between-and within-person differences. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 35, 659670. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-006-9058-1CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Darling, N., Cumsille, P., Peña‐Alampay, L., & Coatsworth, D. (2009). Individual and issue‐specific differences in parental knowledge and adolescent disclosure in Chile, the Philippines, and the United States. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 19(4), 715740. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1532-7795.2009.00608.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
De Kemp, R. A., Scholte, R. H., Overbeek, G., & Engels, R. C. (2006). Early adolescent delinquency: The role of parents and best friends. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 33(4), 488510. https://doi.org/10.1177/0093854806286208CrossRefGoogle Scholar
De Los Reyes, A., Goodman, K. L., Kliewer, W., & Reid-Quinones, K. (2010). The longitudinal consistency of mother–child reporting discrepancies of parental monitoring and their ability to predict child delinquent behaviors two years later. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 39, 14171430. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-009-9496-7CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dishion, T. J., & McMahon, R. J. (1998). Parental monitoring and the prevention of child and adolescent problem behavior: A conceptual and empirical formulation. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 1, 6175. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1021800432380CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dishion, T. J., Nelson, S. E., & Kavanagh, K. (2003). The family check-up with high-risk young adolescents: Preventing early-onset substance use by parent monitoring. Behavior Therapy, 34(4), 553571. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0005-7894(03)80035-7CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellis, D. A., Templin, T. N., Naar-King, S., & Frey, M. A. (2008). Toward conceptual clarity in a critical parenting construct: Parental monitoring in youth with chronic illness. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 33(8), 799808. https://doi.org/10.1093/jpepsy/jsn044CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fiese, B. H., & Sameroff, A. J. (1989). Family context in pediatric psychology: A transactional perspective. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 14(2), 293314. https://doi.org/10.1093/jpepsy/14.2.293CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Finkenauer, C., Engels, R. C., & Meeus, W. (2002). Keeping secrets from parents: Advantages and disadvantages of secrecy in adolescence. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 31, 123136. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1014069926507CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fletcher, A. C., Darling, N. E., & Steinberg, L. (1995). Parental monitoring and peer influences on adolescent substance use. In McCord, J. (Ed.), Coercion and punishment in long-term perspectives (pp. 259271). Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fletcher, A. C., Steinberg, L., & Williams‐Wheeler, M. (2004). Parental influences on adolescent problem behavior: Revisiting Stattin and Kerr. Child Development, 75(3), 781796. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00706.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frijns, T., Keijsers, L., Branje, S., & Meeus, W. (2010). What parents don’t know and how it may affect their children: Qualifying the disclosure–adjustment link. Journal of Adolescence, 33(2), 261270. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2009.05.010CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grolnick, W. S., Gurland, S. T., DeCourcey, W., & Jacob, K. (2002). Antecedents and consequences of mothers’ autonomy support: An experimental investigation. Developmental Psychology, 38(1), 143155. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.38.1.143CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Guo, Y., Killoren, S., & Campione-Barr, N. (2022). Strategies and reasons for nondisclosure in close relationships during adolescence. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 51(9), 18411857. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-022-01634-0CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hawk, S. T., Becht, A., & Branje, S. (2016). “Snooping” as a distinct parental monitoring strategy: Comparisons with overt solicitation and control. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 26(3), 443458. https://doi.org/10.1111/jora.12204CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hawk, S. T., Hale, W. W. III, Raaijmakers, Q. A., & Meeus, W. (2008). Adolescents’ perceptions of privacy invasion in reaction to parental solicitation and control. The Journal of Early Adolescence, 28(4), 583608. https://doi.org/10.1177/0272431608317611CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hawk, S. T., Keijsers, L., Hale, W. W. III, & Meeus, W. (2009). Mind your own business! Longitudinal relations between perceived privacy invasion and adolescent–parent conflict. Journal of Family Psychology, 23(4), 511520. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0015426CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hayes, L., Hudson, A., & Matthews, J. (2003). Parental monitoring: A process model of parent–adolescent interaction. Behaviour Change, 20(1), 1324. https://doi.org/10.1375/bech.20.1.13.24844CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hilliard, M. E., Holmes, C. S., Chen, R., Maher, K., Robinson, E., & Streisand, R. (2013). Disentangling the roles of parental monitoring and family conflict in adolescents’ management of type 1 diabetes. Health Psychology, 32(4), 388396. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0027811CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hougaard, B. (2004). Curlingföräldrar och servicebarn [Curling parents and service children]. Prisma.Google Scholar
Jacobson, K. C., & Crockett, L. J. (2000). Parental monitoring and adolescent adjustment: An ecological perspective. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 10(1), 6597. https://doi.org/10.1207/sjra1001_4CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kakihara, F., & Tilton‐Weaver, L. (2009). Adolescents’ interpretations of parental control: Differentiated by domain and types of control. Child Development, 80(6), 17221738. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01364.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kakihara, F., Tilton-Weaver, L., Kerr, M., & Stattin, H. (2010). The relationship of parental control to youth adjustment: Do youths’ feelings about their parents play a role? Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 39, 14421456. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-009-9479-8CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kandel, D. B. (1996). The parental and peer contexts of adolescent deviance: An algebra of interpersonal influences. Journal of Drug Issues, 26(2), 289315. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022042696026002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kearney, J., & Bussey, K. (2015). The longitudinal influence of self‐efficacy, communication, and parenting on spontaneous adolescent disclosure. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 25(3), 506523. https://doi.org/10.1111/jora.12148CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keijsers, L., Frijns, T., Branje, S. J. T., & Meeus, W. (2009). Developmental links of adolescent disclosure, parental solicitation, and control with delinquency: Moderation by parental support. Developmental Psychology, 45(5), 13141327. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0016693CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kerr, M., & Stattin, H. (2000). What parents know, how they know it, and several forms of adolescent adjustment: Further support for a reinterpretation of monitoring. Developmental Psychology, 36(3), 366380. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kerr, M., & Stattin, H. (2003). Parenting of adolescents: Action or reaction? In Crouter, A. & Booth, A. (Eds.), Children’s influence on family dynamics (pp. 131162). Routledge.Google Scholar
Kiesner, J., Poulin, F., & Dishion, T. J. (2010). Adolescent substance use with friends: Moderating and mediating effects of parental monitoring and peer activity contexts. Merrill Palmer Quarterly, 56(4), 529556. https://doi.org/10.1353/mpq.2010.0002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuczynski, L., & Kochanska, G. (1990). Development of children’s noncompliance strategies from toddlerhood to age 5. Developmental Psychology, 26(3), 398408. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.26.3.398CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Laird, R. D., Marrero, M. D., Melching, J. A., & Kuhn, E. S. (2013). Information management strategies in early adolescence: Developmental change in use and transactional associations with psychological adjustment. Developmental Psychology, 49(5), 928937. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0028845CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Laird, R. D., Marrero, M. D., & Sentse, M. (2010). Revisiting parental monitoring: Evidence that parental solicitation can be effective when needed most. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 39, 14311441. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-009-9453-5CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lollis, S., & Kuczynski, L. (1997). Beyond one hand clapping: Seeing bidirectionality in parent–child relations. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 14(4), 441461. https://doi.org/10.1177/0265407597144002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marshall, S. K., Tilton-Weaver, L. C., & Bosdet, L. (2005). Information management: Considering adolescents’ regulation of parental knowledge. Journal of Adolescence, 28(5), 633647. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2005.08.008CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McCann, M., Jordan, J. A., Higgins, K., & Moore, L. (2019). Longitudinal social network analysis of peer, family, and school contextual influences on adolescent drinking frequency. Journal of Adolescent Health, 65(3), 350358. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jadohealth.2019.03.004CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Montano, G. T., McCauley, H. L., Miller, E., Chisolm, D. J., & Marshal, M. P. (2017). Differences in parental monitoring components and their associated health indicators between sexual-minority and heterosexual girls. Journal of GLBT Family Studies, 13(3), 211235. https://doi.org/10.1080/1550428X.2016.1200507CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mounts, N. S. (2002). Parental management of adolescent peer relationships in context: The role of parenting style. Journal of Family Psychology, 16(1), 5869. https://doi.org/10.1037/0893-3200.16.1.58CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nucci, L., Smetana, J., Araki, N., Nakaue, M., & Comer, J. (2014). Japanese adolescents’ disclosure and information management with parents. Child Development, 85(3), 901907. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12174CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Padilla-Walker, L. M., & Nelson, L. J. (2012). Black hawk down? Establishing helicopter parenting as a distinct construct from other forms of parental control during emerging adulthood. Journal of Adolescence, 35(5), 11771190. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2012.03.007CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Patterson, G. R., & Dishion, T. J. (1985). Contributions of families and peers to delinquency. Criminology, 23(1), 6379. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-9125.1985.tb00326.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pedersen, J. (2013). The rise of the millennial parents: Parenting yesterday and today. R&L Education.Google Scholar
Peterson, G. W. (1986). Parent–youth power dimensions and the behavioral autonomy of adolescents. Journal of Adolescent Research, 1(2), 231249. https://doi.org/10.1177/074355488612008CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Petronio, S. (1994). Privacy binds in family interactions: The case of parental privacy invasion. In Cupach, W. R. & Spitzberg, B. H. (Eds.), The dark side of interpersonal communication (pp. 241257). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pomerantz, E. M., & Eaton, M. M. (2000). Developmental differences in children’s conceptions of parental control: “They love me, but they make me feel incompetent.” Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 46(1), 140167. www.jstor.org/stable/23093346Google Scholar
Rodríguez-Meirinhos, A., Vansteenkiste, M., Soenens, B., Oliva, A., Brenning, K., & Antolín-Suárez, L. (2020). When is parental monitoring effective? A person-centered analysis of the role of autonomy-supportive and psychologically controlling parenting in referred and non-referred adolescents. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 49, 352368. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-019-01151-7CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rote, W. M., & Smetana, J. G. (2016). Beliefs about parents’ right to know: Domain differences and associations with change in concealment. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 26(2), 334344. https://doi.org/10.1111/jora.12194CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000). Self-determination theory and the facilitation of intrinsic motivation, social development, and well-being. American Psychologist, 55(1), 6878. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.55.1.68CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ryan, R. M., Deci, E. L., Grolnick, W. S., & La Guardia, J. G. (2015). The significance of autonomy and autonomy support in psychological development and psychopathology. In Ciccheti, D. & Cohen, D. J. (Eds.), Developmental psychopathology: Vol. 1. Theory and method (pp. 795849). Wiley.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Simmons, R. G., & Blyth, D. A. (1987). Moving into adolescence: The impact of pubertal change and school context. DeGruter.Google Scholar
Smetana, J. G., & Daddis, C. (2002). Domain‐specific antecedents of parental psychological control and monitoring: The role of parenting beliefs and practices. Child Development, 73(2), 563580. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00424CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smetana, J. G., Metzger, A., Gettman, D. C., & Campione‐Barr, N. (2006). Disclosure and secrecy in adolescent–parent relationshipsChild Development77(1), 201217. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00865.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smetana, J. G., Villalobos, M., Tasopoulos-Chan, M., Gettman, D. C., & Campione-Barr, N. (2009). Early and middle adolescents’ disclosure to parents about activities in different domains. Journal of Adolescence, 32(3), 693713. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2008.06.010CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Soenens, B., & Vansteenkiste, M. (2010). A theoretical upgrade of the concept of parental psychological control: Proposing new insights on the basis of self-determination theory. Developmental Review, 30(1), 7499. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2009.11.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stattin, H., & Kerr, M. (2000). Parental monitoring: A reinterpretation. Child Development, 71(4), 10721085. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00210CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Steinberg, L., Elmen, J., & Mounts, N. (1989). Authoritative parenting, psychosocial maturity, and academic success among adolescents. Child Development, 60(6), 14241436. https://doi.org/10.2307/1130932CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Steinberg, L., Fletcher, A., & Darling, N. (1994). Parental monitoring and peer influences on adolescent substance use. Pediatrics, 93(6), 10601064. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.93.6.1060CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Steinberg, L., Mounts, N., Lamborn, S., & Dornbusch, S. (1991). Authoritative parenting and adolescent adjustment across varied ecological niches. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 1(l), 1936. https://doi.org/10.1111/1532-7795.ep11522650Google Scholar
Tasopoulos-Chan, M., Smetana, J. G., & Yau, J. P. (2009). How much do I tell thee? Strategies for managing information to parents among American adolescents from Chinese, Mexican, and European backgrounds. Journal of Family Psychology, 23(3), 364374https://doi.org/10.1037/a0015816CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tilton-Weaver, L. C., Burk, W. J., Kerr, M., & Stattin, H. (2013). Can parental monitoring and peer management reduce the selection or influence of delinquent peers? Testing the question using a dynamic social network approach. Developmental Psychology, 49(11), 20572070. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0031854CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tilton‐Weaver, L. C., & Galambos, N. L. (2003). Adolescents’ characteristics and parents’ beliefs as predictors of parents’ peer management behaviors. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 13(3), 269300. https://doi.org/10.1111/1532-7795.1303002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tilton-Weaver, L. C., Kerr, M., Pakalniskeine, V., Tokic, A., Salihovic, S., & Stattin, H. (2010). Open up or close down: How do parental reactions affect youth information management? Journal of Adolescence, 33(2), 333346. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2009.07.011CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tilton-Weaver, L. C., & Marshall, S. K. (2008). Adolescents’ agency in information management. In Kerr, M., Stattin, H., & Engels, R. C. M. E. (Eds.), What can parents do? New insights into the role of parents in adolescent problem behavior (pp. 1141). John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Tilton-Weaver, L. C., & Marshall, S. K. (2017). Governance transfer: A new perspective on adolescent behavioral autonomy and parental control. In Soenens, B., Vansteenkiste, M., & Van Petegem, S. (Eds.), The meaning and the role of autonomy in adolescent development. Toward conceptual clarity. Studies in Adolescent Development (pp. 7493). Psychological Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tilton‐Weaver, L. C., Marshall, S. K., & Darling, N. (2014). What’s in a name? Distinguishing between routine disclosure and self‐disclosure. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 24(4), 551563. https://doi.org/10.1111/jora.12090CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tilton-Weaver, L. C., & Trost, K. (2012, August). Privacy in the family: Adolescents’ views on their needs and their parents’ behaviors [Paper presentation]. Biennial meeting of the European Association for Research on Adolescence, Spetses, Greece.Google Scholar
Turiel, E., Smetana, J. G., & Killen, M. (1991). Social contexts in social-cognitive development. In Kurtines, W. M. & Gewirtz, J. L. (Eds.), Handbook of moral behavior and development: Vol. 2. Research (pp. 307332). Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Van Petegem, S., Soenens, B., Vansteenkiste, M., & Beyers, W. (2015). Rebels with a cause? Adolescent defiance from the perspective of reactance theory and self‐determination theory. Child Development, 86(3), 903918. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12355CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Waizenhofer, R. N., Buchanan, C. M., & Jackson-Newsom, J. (2004). Mothers’ and fathers’ knowledge of adolescents’ daily activities: Its sources and its links with adolescent adjustment. Journal of Family Psychology, 18(2), 348360. https://doi.org/10.1037/0893-3200.18.2.348CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zimmer-Gembeck, M. J., & Collins, W. A. (2008). Autonomy development during adolescence. In Adams, G. & Berzonsky, M. (Eds.), Blackwell handbook of adolescence (pp. 175204). Blackwell.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×