Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-dh8gc Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-09T06:57:11.935Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

19 - Navigational Principles in Multimedia Learning

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 June 2012

Jean-Francois Rouet
Affiliation:
CNRS and University of Poitiers
Hervé Potelle
Affiliation:
CNRS and University of Poitiers
Richard Mayer
Affiliation:
University of California, Santa Barbara
Get access

Summary

Abstract

This chapter discusses the role of navigational aids in the comprehension and use of complex hypermedia documents. Navigational aids constitute a broad category of verbal and graphical devices ranging from local cues, (e.g., headings, introductions and connectors) to global content representations (e.g., tables, outlines and concept maps). Text-processing research has demonstrated that the principled use of cues and content representations improves learners' comprehension and memory for text. Navigational aids are also helpful in hypertext-based learning, provided that the aids are considerate to the users' needs. We review relevant research studies indicating that the benefits of various types of content representations vary as a function of the reader's expertise and purpose. We offer an explanation in terms of conceptual activation in memory, based on behavioral and computational evidence. Research results suggest that any attempt to optimize the design of instructional hypermedia systems should consider readers' prior knowledge, the contents to be presented, and how the contents will be used.

Introduction to Navigational Principles

Information resources, whether in print or online, play an increasing role in learning communities. Students in secondary and higher education are now commonly asked to learn by interacting with vast repositories of documents, in the context of more or less specific study assignments. For example, students may be asked to gather information about the causes of the 19th-century Irish potato famine, or to find out how the sea otter catches her food. Such tasks involve not just reading, but also searching and navigating multiple sources of information.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2005

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Bernard, M., Hull, S., & Drake, D. (2001). Where should you put the links? A comparison of four locations. Usability News, 3.2. Retrieved May 10, 2004, from http://psychology.wichita.edu/surl/usabilitynews/3S/links.htmGoogle Scholar
Bock, M. (1980). Some effects of titles on building and recalling text structures. Discourse Processes, 3, 301–311CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brandsford, J. D., & Johnson, M. K. (1972). Contextual prerequisites for understanding: Some investigations in comprehension and recall. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 11, 717–726CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Britt, M. A., Rouet, J.-F., & Perfetti, C. A. (1996). Using hypertext to study and reason about historical evidence. In Rouet, J.-F., Levonen, J. J., Dillon, A. P., and Spiro, R. J. (Eds.), Hypertext and cognition (pp. 43–72). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum AssociatesGoogle Scholar
Carmel, E., Crawford, S., & Chen, H. (1992). Browsing in hypertext: A cognitive study. IEEE Transactions on Systems, Man, and Cybernetics, 22, 865–884CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Charney, D. (1994). The impact of hypertext on processes of reading and writing. In Hilligoss, S. J. & Selfe, C. L. (Eds.), Literacy and computers: The complications of teaching and learning with technology (pp. 238–263). New York: Modern Language AssociationGoogle Scholar
Chen, C., & Rada, R. (1996). Interacting with hypertext: A meta-analysis of experimental studies. Human-Computer Interaction, 11, 125–156CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chiu, C.-H., & Wang, F.-M. (2000). The influence of navigation map scope on disorientation of elementary students in learning a web-based hypermedia course. Journal of Educational Computing Research, 22, 135–144CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Conklin, J. (1987). Hypertext: An introduction and survey. Computer, 20, 17–41CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dallal, N. P., Quibble, Z., & Wyatt, C. (2000). Cognitive design of home pages: An experimental study of comprehension on the World Wide Web. Information Processing and Management, 36, 607–621CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Danielson, D. R. (2002). Web navigation and the behavioral effects of constantly visible site maps. Interacting with Computers, 14, 601–618CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dee-Lucas, D., & Larkin, J. H. (1995). Learning from electronic texts: Effects of interactive overviews for information access. Cognition and Instruction, 13, 431–468CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dillon, A., & Gabbard, R. (1998). Hypermedia as an educational technology: A review of the quantitative research literature on learner comprehension, control and style. Review of Educational Research, 68, 322–349CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dooling, D. J., & Lachman, R. (1971). Effects of retention on the comprehension of prose. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 88, 216–222CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dreher, M. J., & Guthrie, J. T. (1990). Cognitive processes in textbook chapter search tasks. Reading Research Quarterly, 25, 323–339CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Edwards, D., & Hardman, L. (1989). “Lost in hyperspace”: Cognitive mapping and navigation in a hypertext environment. In McAleese, R. (Ed.), Hypertext: Theory into practice (pp. 105–125). Oxford, England: IntellectGoogle Scholar
Foltz, P. W. (1996). Comprehension, coherence and strategies in hypertext and linear text. In Rouet, J.-F., Levonen, J. J., Dillon, A. P., and Spiro, R. J. (Eds.), Hypertext and cognition (pp. 109–136). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum AssociatesGoogle Scholar
Gaddy, M. L., van den Broek, P., & Sung, Y.-C. (2001). The influence of text cues on the allocation of attention during reading. In Sanders, T., Schilperoord, J., & Spooren, W. (Eds.), Cognitive approaches to text coherence (pp. 89–110). Amsterdam/Philadelphia: BenjaminsGoogle Scholar
Giroux, L., Bergeron, G., & Lamarche, J.-P. (1987). Organisation sémantique des menus dans les banques de données [semantic organization of menus in databases], Le Travail Humain, 50, 97–107Google Scholar
Glenberg, A. M., & Langston, W. E. (1992). Comprehension of illustrated text: Pictures help build mental models. Journal of Memory and Language, 31, 129–151CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldman, S. R., & Rakestraw, J. A. Jr. (2000). Structural aspects of constructing meaning from text. In Kamil, M. L., Mosenthal, P. B., Pearson, P. D., and Barr, R. (Eds.), Handbook of reading research. Volume III (pp. 311–335). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum AssociatesGoogle Scholar
Gordon, S., Gustavel, J., Moore, J., & Hankey, J. (1988). The effect of hypertext on reader knowledge representation. Proceedings of the 32nd Annual Meeting of the Human Factors Society (pp. 296–300). Santa Monica, CA: Human Factors SocietyGoogle Scholar
Guthrie, J. T. (1988). Locating information in documents: Examination of a cognitive model. Reading Research Quarterly, 23, 178–199CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Guthrie, J. T., & Kirsch, I. (1987). Distinctions between reading comprehension and locating information in text. Journal of Educational Psychology, 79, 210–228CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hartley, J., & Davies, I. K. (1976). Preinstructional strategies: The role of pretests, behavioral objectives, overviews and advance organizers. Review of Educational Research, 46, 239–265CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hofman, R., & , H. (1999). Cognitive effects of a structural overview in a hypertext. British Journal of Educational Technology, 30, 129–140CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1983). Mental models. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University PressGoogle Scholar
Kieras, D. E. (1980). Initial mention as a signal to thematic content in technical passages. Memory and Cognition, 8, 345–353CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kintsch, W. (1988). The role of knowledge in discourse comprehension: A construction-integration model. Psychological Review, 95, 163–182CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kintsch, W., & Dijk, T. A. (1978). Toward a model of text comprehension and production. Psychological Review, 85, 363–394CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kintsch, W., & Yarbrough, J. C. (1982). Role of rhetorical structure in text comprehension. Journal of Educational Psychology, 74, 828–834CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Koved, L., & Shneiderman, B. (1986). Embedded menus: Selecting items in context. Communications of the ACM, 29, 312–318CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kozminsky, E. (1977). Altering comprehension: The effect of biasing title on text comprehension. Memory and Cognition, 5, 482–490CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Larson, K., & Czerwinski, M. (1998). Web page design: Implications of memory, structure and scent for information retrieval. Proceedings of CHI'98 Conference (pp. 25–32). New York: ACM PressGoogle Scholar
Lee, M. J., & Tedder, M. L. (2003). The effects of three different computer texts on readers' recall: Based on working memory capacity. Computers in Human Behavior, 19, 767–783CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Loman, N. L., & Mayer, R. E. (1983). Signalling techniques to increase the understandability of expository prose. Journal of Educational Psychology, 75, 402–412CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lorch, R. F. (1989). Text-signaling devices and their effects on reading and memory processes. Educational Psychology Review, 1, 209–234CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lorch, R. F., & Lorch, E. P. (1996). Effects of organizational signals on free recall of expository text. Journal of Educational Psychology, 88, 38–48CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lorch, R. F. Jr., Lorch, E. P., Ritchey, K., McGovern, L., & Coleman, D. (2001). Effects of headings on text sumarization. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 26, 171–191CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Macedo-Rouet, M., Rouet, J.-F., Epstein, I., & Fayard, P. (2003). Reading and understanding a science report through paper and hypertext – an experimental study. Science Communication, 25, 99–128CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mannes, S. (1994). Strategic processing of text. Journal of Educational Psychology, 88, 577–588CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McDonald, S., & Stevenson, R. J. (1998). An evaluation of the effects of navigational tools and subject matter expertise on browsing and information retrieval in hypertext. Interacting with Computers, 10, 129–142CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meyer, B. J. F. (1985). Prose analysis: Purposes, procedures, and problems. In Britton, B. K. & Black, J. B. (Eds.), Understanding expository text (pp. 11–64). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum AssociatesGoogle Scholar
Miller, D. P. (1981). The depth/breadth tradeoff in hierarchical computer menus. Proceedings of the Human Factors Society, 296–300CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mohageg, M. F. (1992). The influence of hypertext linking structures on the efficiency of information retrieval. Human Factors, 34, 351–367CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nielsen, J. (1999). Designing Web Usability: The Practice of Simplicity. Indianapolis: New RidersGoogle Scholar
Nilsson, R. M., & Mayer, R. E. (2002). The effects of graphical organizers giving cues to the structure of a hypertext document on users' navigation strategies and performance. International Journal of Human-Computer Studies, 57, 1–26CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Norman, K. (Ed.). (1991). The psychology of menu selection. Norwood, NJ: Ablex Publishing CoGoogle Scholar
Potelle, H., & Rouet, J.-F. (2003). Effects of content representation and readers' prior knowledge on the comprehension of hypertext. International Journal of Human-Computer Studies, 58, 327–345CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rouet, J.-F., Favart, M., Britt, M. A., & Perfetti, C. A. (1997). Studying and using multiple documents in history: Effects of discipline expertise. Cognition and Instruction, 15, 85–106CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rouet, J.-F., & Levonen, J. J. (1996). Studying and learning with nonlinear documents: Empirical studies and their implications. In Rouet, J.-F., Levonen, J. J., Dillon, A. P., and Spiro, R. J. (Eds.). Hypertext and cognition (pp. 9–24). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum AssociatesGoogle Scholar
Rouet, J.-F., & Vidal-Abarca, E. (2002). “Mining for meaning”: A cognitive examination of inserted questions in learning from scientific text. In Otero, J., Leon, J. A., & Graesser, A. C. (Eds.), The psychology of science text comprehension (pp. 417–436). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum AssociatesGoogle Scholar
Rouet, J.-F., Vidal-Abarca, E., Bert-Erboul, A., & Millogo, V. (2001). Effects of information search tasks on the comprehension of instructional text. Discourse Processes, 31, 163–186CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schwartz, M. N. K., & Flammer, A. (1981). Text structure and title-effect on comprehension and recall. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 20, 61–66CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shapiro, A. M. (1999). The relationship between prior knowledge and interactive overviews during hypermedia-aided learning. Journal of Educational Computing Research, 20, 143–163CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, E. E., & Swinney, D. A. (1992). The role of schemas in reading text: A real-time examination. Discourse Processes, 15, 303–316CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Snowberry, C., Parkinson, S., & Sisson, N. (1983). Computer display menus. Ergonomics, 26, 699–712CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spyridakis, J. H., & Standal, T. C. (1987). Signals in expository prose: Effects on reading. Reading Research Quarterly, 22, 285–298CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stanton, N. A., Taylor, R. G., & Tweedie, L. A. (1992). Maps as navigational aids in hypertext environments: An empirical evaluation. Journal of Educational Multimedia and Hypermedia, 1, 431–444Google Scholar
Tung, L. L., Debreceny, R., Chan, Y. G., Chan, A. T., & Le, S. E. (2003). Interacting with hypertext: An experimental investigation of navigation tools. Electronic Commerce Research and Applications, 2, 61–72CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Trumpower, D. L., & Goldsmith, T. E. (2004). Structural enhancement of learning. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 29, 426–446CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wiley, J., & Rayner, K. (2000). Effects of titles on the processing of text and lexically ambiguous words: Evidence from eye movements. Memory and Cognition, 28, 1011–1021CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wright, P. (1991). Cognitive overheads and prostheses: Some issues in evaluating hypertexts. In Furuta, R. & Stotts, D. (Eds.), Proceedings of the Third ACM Conference on Hypertext (pp. 1–12). New York: ACM PressGoogle Scholar
Yu, B.-M., & Roh, S.-Z. (2002). The effects of menu-design on informationseeking performance and users' attitude on the World Wide Web. Journal of the American Society for Information Science and Technology, 53, 923–933CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zaphiris, P., Shneiderman, B., & Norman, K. L. (2002). Expandable indexes versus sequential menus for searching hierarchies on the World Wide Web. Behaviour and Information Technology, 21, 201–207. Retrieved May 17, 2004 from http://www.cs.umd.edu/hcil/pubs/tech-reports.shtml#1999CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zhang, H., & Hoosain, R. (2001). The influence of narrative text characteristics on thematic inference during reading. Journal of Research in Reading, 24, 173–186CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×