Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-j824f Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-04T21:32:20.611Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

22 - The Cognitive Aging Principle in Multimedia Learning

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 June 2012

Fred Paas
Affiliation:
Open University of the Netherlands
Pascal W. M. Van Gerven
Affiliation:
Faculty of Psychology, Maastricht University
Huib K. Tabbers
Affiliation:
Tilburg University
Richard Mayer
Affiliation:
University of California, Santa Barbara
Get access

Summary

Abstract

We show that age-related cognitive changes necessitate considerations for the design of multimedia learning environments. These considerations mainly relate to the cognitive aging principle, which states that limited working memory may be effectively expanded by using more than one sensory modality, and some instructional materials with dual-mode presentation may be more efficient than equivalent single-modality formats, especially for older adults. The principle is based on the modality effect and multimedia effect that have been researched extensively in the context of Sweller's (1999) cognitive load theory (CLT) and Mayer's (2001) cognitive theory of multimedia learning (CTML). The research on cognitive aging in relation to multimedia processing is reviewed to explore current understanding of age-related design principles for multimedia learning environments. The potential implications of age-related cognitive changes for the design of multimedia learning environments are highlighted and complemented with important future directions in multimedia learning. The role of CLT and CTML as versatile frameworks for the design of multimedia learning environments for the elderly is discussed.

The Cognitive Aging Principle in the Design of Multimedia Learning

Demographic and technological developments will lead to a growing proportion of independent, active, and eager-to-learn elderly adults who in their everyday lives are more and more confronted with multimedia applications, such as learning environments. Generally, these learning environments consist of many relevant and irrelevant information elements, which are presented together at a fast pace and through different sensory modalities.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2005

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Adam, J. J., Paas, F., Teeken, J. C., Loon, E. M., Boxtel, M. P. J., Houx, P. J., & Jolles, J. (1998). Effects of age on performance in a finger-precuing task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 870–883Google Scholar
Baddeley, A. (1992). Working memory. Science, 255, 556–559CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baddeley, A. (2003). Working memory: Looking back and looking forward. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 4, 829–839CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chandler, P., & Sweller, J. (1992). The spit-attention effect as a factor in the design of instruction. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 62, 233–246CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, J. M., & Paivio, A. (1991). Dual coding theory and education. Educational Psychology Review, 3, 149–210CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Constantinidou, F., & Baker, S. (2002). Stimulus modality and verbal learning performance in normal aging. Brain and Language, 82, 296–311CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Czaja, S. J. (1996). Aging and the acquisition of computer skills. In Rogers, W. A., Fisk, A. D., & Walker, N. (Eds.), Aging and skilled performance: Advances in theory and applications (pp. 201–220). Mahwah, NJ: ErlbaumGoogle Scholar
Czaja, S. J., & Sharit, J. (1993). Age differences in the performance of computer-based work. Psychology and Aging, 8, 59–67CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ericsson, K. A., & Kintsch, W. (1995). Long-term working memory. Psychological Review, 102, 211–245CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fisk, J. E., & Warr, P. (1996). Age and working memory: The role of perceptual speed, the central executive, and the phonological loop. Psychology and Aging, 11, 316–323CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gilinski, A. S., & Judd, B. B. (1994). Working memory and bias in reasoning across the life span. Psychology and Aging, 9, 356–371CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hartman, M., & Hasher, L. (1991). Aging and suppression: Memory for previously relevant information. Psychology and Aging, 6, 587–594CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hasher, L., Stoltzfus, E. R., Zacks, R. T., & Rypma, B. (1991). Age and inhibition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 17, 163–169Google Scholar
Hasher, L., & Zacks, R. T. (1988). Working memory, comprehension, and aging: A review and a new view. In Bower, G. G. (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 22, pp. 193–225). San Diego, CA: Academic PressGoogle Scholar
Jamieson, B. A., & Rogers, W. A. (2000). Age-related effects of blocked and random practice schedules on learning a new technology. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 55B, P343–P353CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jeung, H., Chandler, P., & Sweller, J. (1997). The role of visual indicators in dual sensory mode instruction. Educational Psychology, 17, 329–343CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kalyuga, S., Chandler, P., & Sweller, J. (1999). Managing split-attention and redundancy in multimedia instruction. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 13, 351–3713.0.CO;2-6>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kester, L. (2003). Timing of information presentation and the acquisition of complex skills. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Open University of the Netherlands, Heerlen, the Netherlands
Kester, L., Kirschner, P. A., Merriënboer, J. J. G., & Baumer, A. (2001). Just-in-time information presentation and the acquisition of complex cognitive skills. Computers in Human Behavior, 17, 373–391CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kirasic, K. C., Allen, G. L., & Haggerty, D. (1992). Age-related differences in adults' macrospatial processes. Experimental Aging Research, 18, 33–40CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leahy, W., Chandler, P., & Sweller, J. (2003). When auditory presentations should and should not be a component of multimedia instructions. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 17, 401–418CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Light, L. L., Zelinski, E. M., & Moore, M. G. (1982). Adult age differences in inferential reasoning from new information. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 8, 435–447Google Scholar
Lipman, P. D., & Caplan, L. J. (1992). Adult age differences in memory for routes: Effects of instruction and spatial diagram. Psychology and Aging, 7, 435–441CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mautone, P. D., & Mayer, R. E. (2001). Signaling as a cognitive guide in multimedia learning. Journal of Educational Psychology, 93, 377–389CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mayer, R. E. (2001). Multimedia learning. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University PressCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mayer, R. E., & Chandler, P. (2001). When learning is just a click away: Does simple user interaction foster deeper understanding of multimedia messages?Journal of Educational Psychology, 93, 390–397CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mayer, R. E., Dow, G. T., & Mayer, S. (2003). Multimedia learning in an interactive self-explaining environment: What works in the design of agent-based microworlds?Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 806–813CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mayer, R. E., Heiser, J., & Lonn, S. (2001). Cognitive constraints on multimedia learning: When presenting more material results in less understanding. Journal of Educational Psychology, 93, 187–198CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mayer, R. E., Mathias, A., & Wetzell, K. (2002). Fostering understanding of multimedia messages through pre-training: Evidence for a two-stage theory of mental model construction. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 8, 147–154Google ScholarPubMed
Mayer, R. E., & Moreno, R. (2003). Nine ways to reduce cognitive load in multimedia learning. Educational Psychologist, 38, 43–52CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mayer, R. E., Moreno, R., Boire, M., & Vagge, S. (1999). Maximizing constructivist learning from multimedia communications by minimizing cognitive load. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 638–643CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mayr, U., & Kliegl, R. (1993). Sequential and coordinative complexity: Age-based processing limitations in figural transformations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 19, 1297–1320Google ScholarPubMed
Mayr, U., Kliegl, R., & Krampe, R. T. (1996). Sequential and coordinative processing dynamics in figural transformations across the life span. Cognition, 59, 61–90CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moreno, R., & Mayer, R. E. (1999). Cognitive principles of multimedia learning: The role of modality and contiguity. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 358–368CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morrell, R. W., & Echt, K. V. (1996). Instructional design for older computer users: The influence of cognitive factors. In Rogers, W. A., Fisk, A. D., & Walker, N. (Eds.), Aging and skilled performance: Advances in theory and applications (pp. 241–265). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum AssociatesGoogle Scholar
Mousavi, S. Y., Low, R., & Sweller, J. (1995). Reducing cognitive load by mixing auditory and visual presentation modes. Journal of Educational Psychology, 87, 319–334CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paas, F., Camp, G., & Rikers, R. (2001). Instructional compensation for age-related cognitive declines: Effects of goal specificity in maze learning. Journal of Educational Psychology, 93, 181–186CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paas, F., Renkl, A., & Sweller, J. (2003). Cognitive load theory and instructional design: Recent developments. Educational Psychologist, 38, 1–4CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paas, F., Renkl, A., & Sweller, J. (2004). Cognitive load theory: Instructional implications of the interaction between information structures and cognitive architecture. Instructional Science, 32, 1–8CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paas, F., Tuovinen, J., Tabbers, H., & Gerven, P. W. M. (2003). Cognitive load measurement as a means to advance cognitive load theory. Educational Psychologist, 38, 63–71CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paivio, A. (1986). Mental representations: A dual coding approach. New York: Oxford University PressGoogle Scholar
Pollock, E., Chandler, P., & Sweller, J. (2002). Assimilating complex information. Learning and Instruction, 12, 61–86CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reuter-Lorenz, P. A., Stanczak, L., & Miller, A. C. (1999). Neural recruitment and cognitive aging: Two hemispheres are better than one, especially as you age. Psychological Science, 10, 494–500CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Salthouse, T. A. (1994). Aging associations: Influence of speed on adult age differences in associative learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 20, 1486–1503Google ScholarPubMed
Salthouse, T. A. (1996). The processing-speed theory of adult age differences in cognition. Psychological Review, 103, 403–428CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Salthouse, T. A., & Babcock, R. L. (1991). Decomposing adult age differences in working memory. Developmental Psychology, 27, 763–776CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Salthouse, T. A., Mitchell, D. R. D., Skovronek, E., & Babcock, R. L. (1989). Effects of adult age and working memory on reasoning and spatial abilities. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 15, 507–516Google ScholarPubMed
Spieler, D. H., Balota, D. A., & Faust, M. E. (1996). Stroop performance in healthy younger and older adults and in individuals with dementia of the Alzheimer's type. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 22, 461–479Google ScholarPubMed
Stolzfus, E. R., Hasher, L., Zacks, R. T., Ulivi, M. S., & Goldstein, D. (1993). Investigations of inhibition and interference in younger and older adults. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 48, 179–188CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sweller, J. (1999). Instructional design in technical areas. Camberwell, Australia: ACER PressGoogle Scholar
Sweller, J., & Chandler, P. (1994). Why some material is difficult to learn. Cognition and Instruction, 12, 185–233CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sweller, J., Merriënboer, J. J. G., & Paas, F. (1998). Cognitive architecture and instructional design. Educational Psychology Review, 10, 251–296CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tabbers, H. K., Martens, R. L., & van Merriënboer, J. J. G. (2001). The modality effect in multimedia instructions. In Moore, J. D. & Stenning, K. (Eds.), Proceedings of the Twenty-Third Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 1024–1029). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum AssociatesGoogle Scholar
Tabbers, H. K., Martens, R. L., & Merriënboer, J. J. G. (2004). Multimedia instructions and cognitive load theory: Effects of modality and cueing. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 74, 71–81CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tindall-Ford, S., Chandler, P., & Sweller, J. (1997). When two sensory modes are better than one. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 3, 257–287Google Scholar
Tversky, B., Morrison, J. B., & Bétrancourt, M. (2002). Animation: Can it facilitate?International Journal of Human-Computer Studies, 57, 247–262CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gerven, P. W. M., Paas, F., Merriënboer, J. J. G., Hendriks, M., & Schmidt, H. G. (2003). The efficiency of multimedia learning into old age. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 73, 489–505CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gerven, P. W. M., Paas, F., Merriënboer, J. J. G., & Schmidt, H. G. (2000). Cognitive load theory and the acquisition of complex cognitive skills in the elderly: Towards an integrative framework. Educational Gerontology, 26, 503–521Google Scholar
Gerven, P. W. M., Paas, F., Merriënboer, J. J. G., & Schmidt, H. G. (2002). Cognitive load theory and aging: Effects of worked examples on training efficiency. Learning and Instruction, 38, 87–107CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Gerven, P. W. M., Paas, F., van Merriënboer, J. J. G., & Schmidt, H. G. (2004a). Modality and variability as factors in training the elderly. Manuscript submitted for publication
Gerven, P. W. M., Paas, F., Merriënboer, J. J. G., & Schmidt, H. G. (2004b). Memory load and the cognitive pupillary response in aging. Psychophysiology, 41, 167–174CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Williams, M. D. (1996). Learner-control and instructional techniques. In Jonassen, D. H. (Ed.), Handbook of research for educational communications and technology (pp. 957–983). New York: MacMillanGoogle Scholar
Wingfield, A., Stine, E. A., Lahar, C. J., & Aberdeen, J. S. (1988). Does the capacity of working memory change with age?Experimental Aging Research, 14, 103–107CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zacks, R., & Hasher, L. (1997). Cognitive gerontology and attentional inhibition: A reply to Burke and McDowd. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 52B, P274–P283CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×