Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-6bf8c574d5-8gtf8 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-22T20:57:20.907Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

15 - Blame and Punishment

Two Distinct Mechanisms for Regulating Moral Behavior

from Part III - Behavior

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  20 February 2025

Bertram Malle
Affiliation:
Brown University, Rhode Island
Philip Robbins
Affiliation:
University of Missouri
Get access

Summary

This chapter of the handbook compares the major moral sanctioning behaviors of blame and punishment from two perspectives: their cultural history and their underlying psychology. The author draws a dividing line between two phases of human evolution – before and after human settlement – and proposes that, before that watershed, moral sanctions were informal, nonhierarchical, and often mild, akin to today’s acts of moral blame among intimates. Soon after settlement, hierarchies emerged, in which punishment took hold as a new form of sanctioning, typically exacted by those higher up in the hierarchy, and eventually by institutions of punishment. The author reviews the empirical evidence on the cognitive and social processes underlying each of these sanctioning tools and proposes that their distinct cultural histories are reflected in their psychological properties we can observe today. Whereas blame is, on the whole, flexible, effective, and cognitively sophisticated, punishment is often more damaging, less effective, and can easily be abused – as in past and modern forms of institutional punishment.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2025

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Alicke, M. D. (2000). Culpable control and the psychology of blame. Psychological Bulletin, 126(4), 556574.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alicke, M. D. (2008). Blaming badly. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 8(1–2), 179186.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Alicke, M. D., & Zell, E. (2009). Social attractiveness and blame. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 39(9), 20892105.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Allen, D. S. (2002). The world of Prometheus: The politics of punishing in democratic Athens. Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Amemiya, J., Mortenson, E., & Wang, M.-T. (2020). Minor infractions are not minor: School infractions for minor misconduct may increase adolescents’ defiant behavior and contribute to racial disparities in school discipline. American Psychologist, 75(1), 2336.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ames, D. L., & Fiske, S. T. (2013). Intentional harms are worse, even when they’re not. Psychological Science, 24(9), 17551762.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Andrighetto, G., Brandts, J., Conte, R., Sabater-Mir, J., Solaz, H., & Villatoro, D. (2013). Punish and voice: Punishment enhances cooperation when combined with norm-signalling. PLoS ONE, 8(6), Article e64941.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ask, K., & Pina, A. (2011). On being angry and punitive: How anger alters perception of criminal intent. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 2(5), 494499.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Atkinson, Q. D., Gray, R. D., & Drummond, A. J. (2008). MtDNA variation predicts population size in humans and reveals a major Southern Asian chapter in human prehistory. Molecular Biology and Evolution, 25(2), 468474.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Aurenche, O., Kozłowski, J. K., & Kozłowski, S. K. (2013). To be or not to be… Neolithic: “Failed attempts” at Neolithization in Central and Eastern Europe and in the Near East, and their final success (35,000–7000 BP). Paléorient, 39(2), 545.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Balafoutas, L., & Nikiforakis, N. (2012). Norm enforcement in the city: A natural field experiment. European Economic Review, 56(8), 17731785.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bandy, M. S. (2004). Fissioning, scalar stress, and social evolution in early village societies. American Anthropologist, 106(2), 322333.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baumard, N. (2010). Has punishment played a role in the evolution of cooperation? A critical review. Mind & Society, 9(2), 171192.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baumard, N. (2011). Punishment is not a group adaptation. Mind & Society, 10(1), 126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bell, M. (2012). The standing to blame: A critique. In Coates, D. J. & Tognazzini, N. A. (Eds.), Blame: Its nature and norms (pp. 263281). Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bennett, C. (2002). The varieties of retributive experience. The Philosophical Quarterly, 52(207), 145163.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berger, J., & Hevenstone, D. (2016). Norm enforcement in the city revisited: An international field experiment of altruistic punishment, norm maintenance, and broken windows. Rationality and Society, 28(3), 299319.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bernhard, H., Fischbacher, U., & Fehr, E. (2006). Parochial altruism in humans. Nature, 442(7105), Article 7105.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Binder, G. (2002). Punishment theory: Moral or political? Buffalo Criminal Law Review, 5(2), 321372.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Block, B. P. (1991). Just convictions or just convictions? Issues in Criminological & Legal Psychology, 1(17), 2024.Google Scholar
Boehm, C. (1999). Hierarchy in the forest: The evolution of egalitarian behavior. Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boehm, C. (2000). Conflict and the evolution of social control. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 7(1–2), 79101.Google Scholar
Boehm, C. (2011). Retaliatory violence in human prehistory. The British Journal of Criminology, 51(3), 518534.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boserup, E. (1965). The conditions of agricultural growth: The economics of agrarian change under population pressure. Aldine.Google Scholar
Bozan, J., Shao, H., & Hu, H. (2001). A concise history of China. University Press of the Pacific.Google Scholar
Braithwaite, J. (1999). Restorative justice: Assessing optimistic and pessimistic accounts. Crime and Justice, 25, 1127.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, E. (2013, March 25). Is Adria Richards a bully, or was she bullied by the internet? ZDNet. https://www.zdnet.com/article/is-adria-richards-a-bully-or-was-she-bullied-by-the-internet/Google Scholar
Bucciarelli, M., Khemlani, S., & Johnson-Laird, P. N. (2008). The psychology of moral reasoning. Judgment and Decision Making, 3(2), 121139.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Buckholtz, J. W., Martin, J. W., Treadway, M. T., Jan, K., Zald, D. H., Jones, O., & Marois, R. (2015). From blame to punishment: Disrupting prefrontal cortex activity reveals norm enforcement mechanisms. Neuron, 87(6), 13691380.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buikstra, J. E., Konigsberg, L. W., & Bullington, J. (1986). Fertility and the development of agriculture in the prehistoric Midwest. American Antiquity, 51(3), 528546.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carlsmith, K. M., Darley, J. M., & Robinson, P. H. (2002). Why do we punish?: Deterrence and just deserts as motives for punishment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83(2), 284299.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carneiro, R. L. (1970). A theory of the origin of the state. Science, 169(3947), 733738.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chavez, A. K., & Bicchieri, C. (2013). Third-party sanctioning and compensation behavior: Findings from the ultimatum game. Journal of Economic Psychology, 39, 268277.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ciurria, M. (2019). An intersectional feminist theory of moral responsibility. Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Claessen, H. J. M. (2003). Aspects of law and order in Early State societies. In Feldbrugge, F. J. M. (Ed.), The law’s beginnings (pp. 161179). Martinus Nijhoff Publishers.Google Scholar
Clark, C. J., & Winegard, B. M. (2019). Optimism in unconscious, intuitive morality. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 42, Article e150.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Coates, D. J., & Tognazzini, N. A. (2012). The contours of blame. In Coates, D. J. & Tognazzini, N. A. (Eds.), Blame: Its nature and norms (pp. 326). Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cohen, D., & Nisbett, R. E. (1994). Self-protection and the culture of honor: Explaining Southern violence. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 20(5), 551567.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cottingham, J. (1979). Varieties of retribution. The Philosophical Quarterly, 29(116), 238246.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crockett, M. J. (2017). Moral outrage in the digital age. Nature Human Behaviour, 1(11), 769771.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cusimano, C., Thapa, S., & Malle, B. F. (2017). Judgment before emotion: People access moral evaluations faster than affective states. In Gunzelmann, G., Howes, A., Tenbrink, T., & Davelaar, E. J. (Eds.), Proceedings of the 39th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 18481853). Cognitive Science Society.Google Scholar
Cullen, F. T., Jonson, C. L., & Nagin, D. S. (2011). Prisons do not reduce recidivism: The high cost of ignoring science. The Prison Journal, 91(3, Suppl), 48S65S.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cushman, F. (2008). Crime and punishment: Distinguishing the roles of causal and intentional analyses in moral judgment. Cognition, 108(2), 353380.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cushman, F. (2015). Punishment in humans: From intuitions to institutions. Philosophy Compass, 10(2), 117133.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Darling-Hammond, S., Ruiz, M., Eberhardt, J. L., & Okonofua, J. A. (2023). The dynamic nature of student discipline and discipline disparities. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 120(17), 110.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Decety, J., & Cacioppo, S. (2012). The speed of morality: A high-density electrical neuroimaging study. Journal of Neurophysiology, 108(11), 30683072.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
De Freitas, J., & Alvarez, G. A. (2018). Your visual system provides all the information you need to make moral judgments about generic visual events. Cognition, 178, 133146.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dersley, I., & Wootton, A. (2000). Complaint sequences within antagonistic argument. Research on Language and Social Interaction, 33(4), 375406.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dhaliwal, N. A., Patil, I., & Cushman, F. (2021). Reputational and cooperative benefits of third-party compensation. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 164, 2751.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dickey, R. (2019, May 28). It’s not that people deny the existence (although some do) it’s just that it’s ridiculous to expect people to keep track of them [Comment]. Quora.com. https://qr.ae/pGBil5Google Scholar
Ditto, P. H. (2009). Passion, reason, and necessity: A quantity-of-processing view of motivated reasoning. In Bayne, T. & Fernández, J. (Eds.), Delusion and self-deception: Affective and motivational influences on belief formation (pp. 2353). Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Drew, P. (1998). Complaints about transgressions and misconduct. Research on Language & Social Interaction, 31(3–4), 295325.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dubreuil, B. (2010). Human evolution and the origins of hierarchies: The state of nature. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dugar, S. (2010). Nonmonetary sanctions and rewards in an experimental coordination game. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 73(3), 377386.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dukes, K. N., & Gaither, S. E. (2017). Black racial stereotypes and victim blaming: Implications for media coverage and criminal proceedings in cases of police violence against racial and ethnic minorities. Journal of Social Issues, 73(4), 789807.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dumond, D. E. (1972). Population growth and political centralization. In Spooner, B. (Ed.), Population growth: Anthropological implications (pp. 286310). MIT Press.Google Scholar
Dunbar, R. I. M. (1996). Grooming, gossip, and the evolution of language. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Eberhardt, J. L., Davies, P. G., Purdie-Vaughns, V. J., & Johnson, S. L. (2006). Looking deathworthy: Perceived stereotypicality of black defendants predicts capital-sentencing outcomes. Psychological Science, 17(5), 383386.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ellsworth, P. C., & Scherer, K. R. (2003). Appraisal processes in emotion. In Davidson, R. J., Scherer, K. R., & Goldsmith, H. H. (Eds.), Handbook of affective sciences (pp. 572595). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Enloe, J. G. (2003). Food sharing past and present: Archaeological evidence for economic and social interactions. Before Farming: The Archaeology and Anthropology of Hunter-Gatherers, 2003(1), 123.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Equal Justice Initiative. (2017). Lynching in America: Confronting the legacy of racial terror. https://eji.org/wp-content/uploads/2019/10/lynching-in-america-3d-ed-080219.pdfGoogle Scholar
Eriksson, K., Andersson, P. A., & Strimling, P. (2017). When is it appropriate to reprimand a norm violation? The roles of anger, behavioral consequences, violation severity, and social distance. Judgment and Decision Making, 12(4), 396407.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Farrington, K. (1996). Dark justice: A history of punishment and torture. Smithmark.Google Scholar
Fehr, E., & Gächter, S. (2000). Cooperation and punishment in public goods experiments. The American Economic Review, 90(4), 980994.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Feinberg, M., Cheng, J. T., & Willer, R. (2012). Gossip as an effective and low-cost form of punishment. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 35(1), 2525.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Feinberg, M., Willer, R., & Schultz, M. (2014). Gossip and ostracism promote cooperation in groups. Psychological Science, 25(3), 656664.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fincham, F. D., Beach, S., & Nelson, G. (1987). Attribution processes in distressed and nondistressed couples: III. Causal and responsibility attributions for spouse behavior. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 11(1), 7186.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Flannery, K. V., & Marcus, J. (2012). The creation of inequality: How our prehistoric ancestors set the stage for monarchy, slavery, and empire. Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Forbes, R. C., & Stellar, J. E. (2022). When the ones we love misbehave: Exploring moral processes within intimate bonds. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 122(1), 1633.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Friedman, M. (2013). How to blame people responsibly. Journal of Value Inquiry, 47(3), 271284.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Funk, F., McGeer, V., & Gollwitzer, M. (2014). Get the message: Punishment is satisfying if the transgressor responds to its communicative intent. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 40(8), 986997.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gamblin, B. W., Kehn, A., Vanderzanden, K., Ruthig, J. C., Jones, K. M., & Long, B. L. (2021). A comparison of juror decision making in race-based and sexual orientation–based hate crime cases. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 36(7–8), 32313256.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gamez-Djokic, M., & Molden, D. (2016). Beyond affective influences on deontological moral judgment: The role of motivations for prevention in the moral condemnation of harm. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 42(11), 15221537.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garland, D. (1990). Punishment and modern society: A study in social theory. University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gawronski, B., Conway, P., Armstrong, J., Friesdorf, R., & Hütter, M. (2018). Effects of incidental emotions on moral dilemma judgments: An analysis using the CNI model. Emotion, 18(7), 9891008.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gershoff, E. T., & Lee, S. J. (Eds.). (2020). Ending the physical punishment of children: A guide for clinicians and practitioners. American Psychological Association.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gignoux, C. R., Henn, B. M., & Mountain, J. L. (2011). Rapid, global demographic expansions after the origins of agriculture. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(15), 60446049.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goodwin, G. P., & Gromet, D. M. (2014). Punishment. Cognitive Science, 5(5), 561572.Google ScholarPubMed
Gouldner, A. W. (1960). The norm of reciprocity: A preliminary statement. American Sociological Review, 25(2), 161178.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gray, K., Schein, C., & Ward, A. F. (2014). The myth of harmless wrongs in moral cognition: Automatic dyadic completion from sin to suffering. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143(4), 16001615.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Griffith, A. (2020). God’s law and order: The politics of punishment in evangelical America. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Guala, F. (2012). Reciprocity: Weak or strong? What punishment experiments do (and do not) demonstrate. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 35(1), 115.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Guglielmo, S. (2015). Moral judgment as information processing: An integrative review. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, Article 1637.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Guglielmo, S., & Malle, B. F. (2017). Information-acquisition processes in moral judgments of blame. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 43(7), 957971.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haidt, J. (2001). The emotional dog and its rational tail: A social intuitionist approach to moral judgment. Psychological Review, 108(4), 814834.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haidt, J., & Bjorklund, F. (2008). Social intuitionists answer six questions about moral psychology. In Moral psychology: Vol 2. The cognitive science of morality: Intuition and diversity (pp. 181217). MIT Press.Google Scholar
Haidt, J., Bjorklund, F., & Murphy, S. (2000). Moral dumbfounding: When intuition finds no reason [Unpublished manuscript]. Available at https://pdfs.semanticscholar.org/d415/e7fa2c2df922dac194441516a509ba5eb7ec.pdf]. University of Virginia.Google Scholar
Heffner, J., & FeldmanHall, O. (2019). Why we don’t always punish: Preferences for non-punitive responses to moral violations. Scientific Reports, 9(1), Article 13219.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Henn, B. M., Gignoux, C. R., Jobin, M., Granka, J. M., Macpherson, J. M., Kidd, J. M., Rodríguez-Botigué, L., Ramachandran, S., Hon, L., Brisbin, A., Lin, A. A., Underhill, P. A., Comas, D., Kidd, K. K., Norman, P. J., Parham, P., Bustamante, C. D., Mountain, J. L., & Feldman, M. W. (2011). Hunter-gatherer genomic diversity suggests a southern African origin for modern humans. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(13), 51545162.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Herrmann, B., Thöni, C., & Gächter, S. (2008). Antisocial punishment across societies. Science, 319(5868), 13621367.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hershcovis, M. S., & Bhatnagar, N. (2017). When fellow customers behave badly: Witness reactions to employee mistreatment by customers. Journal of Applied Psychology, 102(11), 15281544.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hopfensitz, A., & Reuben, E. (2009). The importance of emotions for the effectiveness of social punishment. The Economic Journal, 119(540), 15341559.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kansal, T. (2005). Racial disparity in sentencing: A review of the literature. The Sentencing Project. https://www.opensocietyfoundations.org/publications/racial-disparity-sentencingGoogle Scholar
Kelly, R. C. (2000). Warless societies and the origin of war. University of Michigan Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kennedy, J. L. D., Tuliao, A. P., Flower, K. N., Tibbs, J. J., & McChargue, D. E. (2019). Long-term effectiveness of a brief restorative justice intervention. International Journal of Offender Therapy and Comparative Criminology, 63(1), 317.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Klonick, K. (2015). Re-shaming the debate: Social norms, shame, and regulation in an internet age. Maryland Law Review, 75(4), 10291065.Google Scholar
Knauft, B. M. (1994). Culture and cooperation in human evolution. In Sponsel, L. & Gregor, T. (Eds.), The anthropology of peace and nonviolence (pp. 3767). Lynne Rienner.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kneer, M., & Machery, E. (2019). No luck for moral luck. Cognition, 182, 331348.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kochanska, G., & Aksan, N. (2006). Children’s conscience and self-regulation. Journal of Personality, 74(6), 15871618.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kriss, P. H., Weber, R. A., & Xiao, E. (2016). Turning a blind eye, but not the other cheek: On the robustness of costly punishment. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 128, 159177.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuo, S.-Y., Longmire, D., & Cuvelier, S. J. (2010). An empirical assessment of the process of restorative justice. Journal of Criminal Justice, 38(3), 318328.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Laforest, M. (2002). Scenes of family life: Complaining in everyday conversation. Journal of Pragmatics, 34(10–11), 15951620.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Larsen, C. S. (1995). Biological changes in human populations with agriculture. Annual Review of Anthropology, 24(1), 185213.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lee, R. B. (1972). Population growth and the beginnings of sedentary life among the !Kung bushmen. In Spooner, B. (Ed.), Population growth: Anthropological implications (pp. 329342). MIT Press.Google Scholar
Lee, R. B. (1979). The !Kung San: Men, women, and work in a foraging society. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Lee, R. B., & Daly, R. H. (1999). The Cambridge encyclopedia of hunters and gatherers. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Lerner, J. S., Goldberg, J. H., & Tetlock, P. E. (1998). Sober second thought: The effects of accountability, anger, and authoritarianism on attributions of responsibility. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24(6), 563574.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MacManus, D., Dean, K., Al Bakir, M., Iversen, A. C., Hull, L., Fahy, T., Wessely, S., & Fear, N. T. (2012). Violent behaviour in UK military personnel returning home after deployment. Psychological Medicine, 42(8), 16631673.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Malle, B. F. (2021). Moral judgments. Annual Review of Psychology, 72, 293318.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Malle, B. F., Guglielmo, S., & Monroe, A. E. (2014). A theory of blame. Psychological Inquiry, 25(2), 147186.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Malle, B. F., Guglielmo, S., Voiklis, J., & Monroe, A. E. (2022). Cognitive blame is socially shaped. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 31(2), 169176.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Márquez-Reiter, R., & Haugh, M. (2019). Denunciation, blame and the moral turn in public life. Discourse, Context & Media, 28, 3543.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Masclet, D., Noussair, C., Tucker, S., & Villeval, M.-C. (2003). Monetary and nonmonetary punishment in the voluntary contributions mechanism. American Economic Review, 93(1), 366381.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mazzocco, P. J., Alicke, M. D., & Davis, T. L. (2004). On the robustness of outcome bias: No constraint by prior culpability. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 26(2–3), 131146.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McGeer, V. (2012). Civilizing blame. In Coates, D. J. & Tognazzini, N. A. (Eds.), Blame: Its nature and norms (pp. 162188). Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McHugh, C., McGann, M., Igou, E. R., & Kinsella, E. L. (2017). Searching for moral dumbfounding: Identifying measurable indicators of moral dumbfounding. Collabra: Psychology, 3(1, Art. 23), 124.Google Scholar
McHugh, C., McGann, M., Igou, E. R., & Kinsella, E. L. (2020). Reasons or rationalizations: The role of principles in the moral dumbfounding paradigm. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 33(3), 376392.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Messerschmidt, M. (2005). Die Wehrmachtjustiz 1933–1945. Schöningh.Google Scholar
Miller, G. P. (2003). Norm enforcement in the public sphere: The case of handicapped parking. George Washington Law Review, 71, 895933.Google Scholar
Milner, G. R., Anderson, E., & Smith, V. G. (1991). Warfare in Late Prehistoric West-Central Illinois. American Antiquity, 56(4), 581603.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mirabito, L. A., & Lecci, L. (2021). The impact of anti-gay bias on verdicts and sentencing with gay defendants. Journal of Gay & Lesbian Social Services: The Quarterly Journal of Community & Clinical Practice, 33(1), 3255.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mitchell, T. L., Haw, R. M., Pfeifer, J. E., & Meissner, C. A. (2005). Racial bias in mock juror decision-making: A meta-analytic review of defendant treatment. Law and Human Behavior, 29(6), 621637.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Molho, C., Tybur, J. M., Van Lange, P. A. M., & Balliet, D. (2020). Direct and indirect punishment of norm violations in daily life. Nature Communications, 11(1), Article 1.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Monin, B., Pizarro, D. A., & Beer, J. S. (2007). Deciding versus reacting: Conceptions of moral judgment and the reason-affect debate. Review of General Psychology, 11(2), 99111.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Monroe, A. E., & Malle, B. F. (2017). Two paths to blame: Intentionality directs moral information processing along two distinct tracks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 146(1), 123133.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Monroe, A. E., & Malle, B. F. (2019). People systematically update moral judgments of blame. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 116(2), 215236.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mooijman, M., & Graham, J. (2018). Unjust punishment in organizations. Research in Organizational Behavior, 38, 95106.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moore, B. (2001). Cruel and unusual punishment in the Roman Empire and dynastic China. International Journal of Politics, Culture, and Society, 14(4), 729772.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morris, G. H. (1988). Finding fault. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 7(1), 125.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Muhammad, K. G., Gottschalk, M., & Thompson, H. A. (2015). The underlying causes of rising incarceration: Crime, politics, and social change. In Travis, J., Western, B., & Redburn, S. (Eds.), The growth of incarceration in the United States: Exploring causes and consequences (pp. 104129). National Academies Press.Google Scholar
Nadelhoffer, T., Heshmati, S., Kaplan, D., & Nichols, S. (2013). Folk retributivism and the communication confound. Economics and Philosophy, 29(2), 235261.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nadler, J. (2012). Blaming as a social process: The influence of character and moral emotion on blame. Law and Contemporary Problems, 75(2), 131.Google Scholar
Nadler, J., & McDonnell, M.-H. (2012). Moral character, motive, and the psychology of blame. Cornell Law Review, 97, 255304.Google Scholar
Nemeth, C., & Sosis, R. H. (1973). A simulated jury study: Characteristics of the defendant and the jurors. The Journal of Social Psychology, 90(2), 221229.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Neumann, R., Seibt, B., & Strack, F. (2001). The influence of mood on the intensity of emotional responses: Disentangling feeling and knowing. Cognition and Emotion, 15(6), 725747.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Niemi, L., & Young, L. (2016). When and why we see victims as responsible: The impact of ideology on attitudes toward victims. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 42(9), 12271242.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Okonofua, J. A., & Eberhardt, J. L. (2015). Two strikes: Race and the disciplining of young students. Psychological Science, 26(5), 617624.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pedersen, E. J., McAuliffe, W. H. B., Shah, Y., Tanaka, H., Ohtsubo, Y., & McCullough, M. E. (2020). When and why do third parties punish outside of the lab? A cross-cultural recall study. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 11(6), 846853.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peregrine, P. N., Ember, C. R., & Ember, M. (2007). Modeling state origins using cross-cultural data. Cross-Cultural Research, 41(1), 7586.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pesta, R. (2022). School punishment, deterrence, and race: A partial test of defiance theory. Crime & Delinquency, 68(3), 463494.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pinker, S. (2011). The better angels of our nature: Why violence has declined. Viking.Google Scholar
Pratt, J., Brown, D., Brown, M., Wallsworth, S., & Morrison, W. (Eds.). (2005). The new punitiveness: Trends, theories, perspectives. Willan.Google Scholar
Princing, M. (2019, September 3). What microaggressions are and how to prevent them. Right as Rain by UW Medicine. https://rightasrain.uwmedicine.org/life/relationships/microaggressionsGoogle Scholar
Przepiorka, W., & Berger, J. (2016). The sanctioning dilemma: A quasi-experiment on social norm enforcement in the train. European Sociological Review, 32(3), 439451.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Quigley, B. M., & Tedeschi, J. T. (1996). Mediating effects of blame attributions on feelings of anger. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22(12), 12801288.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rand, D. G., Armao, J. J. IV, Nakamaru, M., & Ohtsuki, H. (2010). Anti-social punishment can prevent the co-evolution of punishment and cooperation. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 265(4), 624632.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Redford, L., & Ratliff, K. A. (2016). Hierarchy-legitimizing ideologies reduce behavioral obligations and blame for implicit attitudes and resulting discrimination. Social Justice Research, 29(2), 159185.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Redford, L., & Ratliff, K. A. (2018). Pride and punishment: Entitled people’s self‐promoting values motivate hierarchy‐restoring retribution. European Journal of Social Psychology, 48(3), 303319.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Redman, C. L. (1978). The rise of civilization: From early farmers to urban society in the ancient Near East. W. H. Freeman.Google Scholar
Republican National Committee. (2020). Resolution upholding the first amendment to the constitution of the United States of America in the response to the coronavirus pandemic and the cancel culture movement. https://bit.ly/4gpQLVsGoogle Scholar
Robinson, P. H., & Darley, J. M. (1995). Justice, liability, and blame: Community views and the criminal law. Westview Press.Google Scholar
Royzman, E. B., Kim, K., & Leeman, R. F. (2015). The curious tale of Julie and Mark: Unraveling the moral dumbfounding effect. Judgment and Decision Making, 10(4), 296313.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sasse, J., Halmburger, A., & Baumert, A. (2020). The functions of anger in moral courage—Insights from a behavioral study. Emotion, 22(6), 13211335.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scherer, K. R. (2013). The nature and dynamics of relevance and valence appraisals: Theoretical advances and recent evidence. Emotion Review, 5(2), 150162.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Seidel, A., & Prinz, J. (2013). Mad and glad: Musically induced emotions have divergent impact on morals. Motivation and Emotion, 37(3), 629637.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Service, E. R. (1966). The hunters. Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
Shoemaker, D. (2012). Blame and punishment. In Coates, D. J. & Tognazzini, N. A. (Eds.), Blame: Its nature and norms (pp. 100118). Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shoemaker, D., & Vargas, M. (2021). Moral torch fishing: A signaling theory of blame. Noûs, 55(3), 581602.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sidanius, J., Mitchell, M., Haley, H., & Navarrete, C. D. (2006). Support for harsh criminal sanctions and criminal justice beliefs: A social dominance perspective. Social Justice Research, 19(4), 433449.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sidanius, J., & Pratto, F. (1999). Social dominance: An intergroup theory of social hierarchy and oppression. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Silberbauer, G. (1982). Political process in G/wi bands. In Leacock, E. & Lee, R. (Eds.), Politics and history in band societies (pp. 2336). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Sood, A. M. (2019). Attempted justice: Misunderstanding and bias in psychological constructions of critical attempt. Stanford Law Review, 71(3), 593686.Google Scholar
Sood, A. M., & Darley, J. M. (2012). The plasticity of harm in the service of criminalization goals. California Law Review, 100(5), 13131358.Google Scholar
Sorial, S. (2016). Performing anger to signal injustice: The expression of anger in victim impact statements. In Abell, C., Smith, J., Abell, C., & Smith, J. (Eds.), The expression of emotion: Philosophical, psychological and legal perspectives (pp. 287310). Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stanley, M. L., Yin, S., & Sinnott-Armstrong, W. (2019). A reason-based explanation for moral dumbfounding. Judgment and Decision Making, 14(2), 120129.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stewart, J. E. (1985). Appearance and punishment: The attraction-leniency effect in the courtroom. The Journal of Social Psychology, 125(3), 373378.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Strohminger, N. (2017). Four unwarranted assumptions about the role of emotion in moral judgment. In Price, C. & Walle, E. (Eds.), Emotion Researcher: ISRE’s sourcebook for research on emotion and affect. http://emotionresearcher.com/four-unwarranted-assumptions-about-the-role-of-emotion-in-moral-judgment/Google Scholar
Sutton, J. R. (2013). Structural bias in the sentencing of felony defendants. Social Science Research, 42(5), 12071221.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tata, C. (1997). Conceptions and representations of the sentencing decision process. Journal of Law and Society, 24(3), 395420.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tonry, M. (2009). Explanations of American punishment policies: A national history. Punishment & Society, 11(3), 377394.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Voiklis, J., Kim, B., Cusimano, C., & Malle, B. F. (2016). Moral judgments of human vs. Robot agents. In Proceedings of the 25th IEEE International Symposium on Robot and Human Interactive Communication (RO-MAN) (pp. 486491). IEEE.Google Scholar
Voiklis, J., & Malle, B. F. (2018). Moral cognition and its basis in social cognition and social regulation. In Gray, K. & Graham, J. (Eds.), Atlas of moral psychology (pp. 108120). Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Walker, M. U. (2006). Moral repair: Reconstructing moral relations after wrongdoing. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Watanabe, S., & Laurent, S. M. (2021). Volition speaks louder than action: Offender atonement, forgivability, and victim valuation in the minds of perceivers. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 47(6), 10201036.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weiss, A., & Burgmer, P. (2021). Other-serving double standards: People show moral hypercrisy in close relationships. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 38(11), 31983218.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
White, L. A. (2007). The evolution of culture: The development of civilization to the fall of Rome. Left Coast Press.Google Scholar
Wiessner, P. (2005). Norm enforcement among the Ju/’hoansi Bushmen. Human Nature, 16(2), 115145.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilson, P. J. (1988). The domestication of the human species. Yale University Press.Google Scholar
Woodburn, J. (1982). Egalitarian societies. Man, 17(3), 431451.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wu, J., Balliet, D., & Van Lange, P. A. M. (2016). Gossip versus punishment: The efficiency of reputation to promote and maintain cooperation. Scientific Reports, 6, Article 23919.Google ScholarPubMed
Xiao, E. (2018). Punishment, social norms, and cooperation. In Teitelbaum, J. C. & Zeiler, K. (Eds.), Research handbook on behavioral law and economics (pp. 155173). Edward Elgar Publishing.Google Scholar
Xiao, E., & Houser, D. (2005). Emotion expression in human punishment behavior. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 102(20), 73987401.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yamagishi, T. (1986). The provision of a sanctioning system as a public good. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51(1), 110116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yukhnenko, D., Sridhar, S., & Fazel, S. (2020). A systematic review of criminal recidivism rates worldwide: 3-year update. Wellcome Open Research, 4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zajenkowska, A., Prusik, M., Jasielska, D., & Szulawski, M. (2021). Hostile attribution bias among offenders and non-offenders: Making social information processing more adequate. Journal of Community & Applied Social Psychology, 31(2), 241256.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×