Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-6bf8c574d5-b4m5d Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-23T01:47:39.900Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Part III - Behavior

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  20 February 2025

Bertram Malle
Affiliation:
Brown University, Rhode Island
Philip Robbins
Affiliation:
University of Missouri
Get access
Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2025

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

References

Anderson, A. K., & Phelps, E. A. (2001). Lesions of the human amygdala impair enhanced perception of emotionally salient events. Nature, 411(6835), 305309.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Axelrod, R., & Hamilton, W. D. (1981). The evolution of cooperation. Science, 211(4489), 13901396.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bailey, P. E., Brady, B., Ebner, N. C., & Ruffman, T. (2020). Effects of age on emotion regulation, emotional empathy, and prosocial behavior. Journals of Gerontology – Series B Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 75(4), 802810.Google ScholarPubMed
Balliet, D., Parks, C., & Joireman, J. (2009). Social value orientation and cooperation in social dilemmas: A meta-analysis. Group Processes and Intergroup Relations, 12(4), 533547.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Balliet, D., Wu, J., & De Dreu, C. K. W. (2014). Ingroup favoritism in cooperation: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 140(6), 15561581.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barfuss, W., Donges, J. F., Vasconcelos, V. V., Kurths, J., & Levin, S. A. (2020). Caring for the future can turn tragedy into comedy for long-term collective action under risk of collapse. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 117(23), 1291512922.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bartal, I. B. A., Decety, J., & Mason, P. (2011). Empathy and pro-social behavior in rats. Science, 334(6061), 14271430.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Basile, B., Mancini, F., Macaluso, E., Caltagirone, C., Frackowiak, R. S. J., & Bozzali, M. (2011). Deontological and altruistic guilt: Evidence for distinct neurobiological substrates. Human Brain Mapping, 32(2), 229239.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Batson, C. D. (2011). Altruism in humans. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Batson, C. D., Fultz, J., & Schoenrade, P. A. (1987). Distress and empathy: Two qualitatively distinct vicarious emotions with different motivational consequences. Journal of Personality, 55(1), 1939.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Batson, C. D., & Moran, T. (1999). Empathy-induced altruism in a prisoner’s dilemma. European Journal of Social Psychology, 29(7), 909924.3.0.CO;2-L>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Batson, C. D., & Powell, A. A. (2003). Altruism and prosocial behavior. In Millon, T. & Lerner, J. M. (Eds.), Handbook of psychology: Personality and social psychology (Vol. 5, pp. 463484). John Wiley & Sons.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bednya, M., Pascual-Leone, A., & Saxe, R. R. (2009). Growing up blind does not change the neural bases of Theory of Mind. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 106(27), 1131211317.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bellucci, G., Camilleri, J. A., Eickhoff, S. B., & Krueger, F. (2020). Neural signatures of prosocial behaviors. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 118, 186195.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berg, J., Dickhaut, J., & McCabe, K. (1995). Trust, reciprocity, and social history. Games and Economic Behavior, 10(1), 122142.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bernhardt, B. C., & Singer, T. (2012). The neural basis of empathy. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 35, 123.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berthoz, S., Armony, J. L., Blair, R. J. R., & Dolan, R. J. (2002). An fMRI study of intentional and unintentional (embarrassing) violations of social norms. Brain, 125(8), 16961708.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berthoz, S., Grèzes, J., Armony, J. L., Passingham, R. E., & Dolan, R. J. (2006). Affective response to one’s own moral violations. NeuroImage, 31(2), 945950.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blair, K., Marsh, A. A., Morton, J., Vythilingam, M., Jones, M., Mondillo, K., Pine, D. C., Drevets, W. C., & Blair, J. R. (2006). Choosing the lesser of two evils, the better of two goods: Specifying the roles of ventromedial prefrontal cortex and dorsal anterior cingulate in object choice. Journal of Neuroscience, 26(44), 1137911386.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blair, R. J. R. (2007). The amygdala and ventromedial prefrontal cortex in morality and psychopathy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(9), 387392.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bohner, G., Crow, K., Erb, H. ‐P, & Schwarz, N. (1992). Affect and persuasion: Mood effects on the processing of message content and context cues and on subsequent behaviour. European Journal of Social Psychology, 22(6), 511530.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boyd, R., Gintis, H., & Bowles, S. (2010). Coordinated punishment of defectors sustains cooperation and can proliferate when rare. Science, 328(5978), 617620.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. J. (1989). The evolution of indirect reciprocity. Social Networks, 11(3), 213236.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bradley, A., Lawrence, C., & Ferguson, E. (2018). Does observability affect prosociality? Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 285(1875), Article 20180116.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brosnan, S. F. (2006). Nonhuman species’ reactions to inequity and their implications for fairness. Social Justice Research, 19(2), 153185.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brosnan, S. F., & De Waal, F. B. M. (2003). Monkeys reject unequal pay. Nature, 425(6955), 297299.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buhle, J. T., Silvers, J. A., Wage, T. D., Lopez, R., Onyemekwu, C., Kober, H., Webe, J., & Ochsner, K. N. (2014). Cognitive reappraisal of emotion: A meta-analysis of human neuroimaging studies. Cerebral Cortex, 24(11), 29812990.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burkett, J. P., Andari, E., Johnson, Z. V., Curry, D. C., De Waal, F. B. M., & Young, L. J. (2016). Oxytocin-dependent consolation behavior in rodents. Science, 351(6271), 375378.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buyalskaya, A., Gallo, M., & Camerer, C. F. (2021). The golden age of social science. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 118(5), Article e2002923118.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Camerer, C. F. (2003). Behavioral game theory: Experiments in strategic interaction. Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Chakroff, A., Dungan, J., Koster-Hale, J., Brown, A., Saxe, R., & Young, L. (2016). When minds matter for moral judgment: Intent information is neurally encoded for harmful but not impure acts. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 11(3), 476484.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chang, L. J., Smith, A., Dufwenberg, M., & Sanfey, A. G. (2011). Triangulating the neural, psychological, and economic bases of guilt aversion. Neuron, 70(3), 560572.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Charness, G., & Rabin, M. (2002). Understanding social preferences with simple tests. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 117(3), 817869.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chavez, A. K., & Bicchieri, C. (2013). Third-party sanctioning and compensation behavior: Findings from the ultimatum game. Journal of Economic Psychology, 39, 268277.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Christakis, N. A. (2019). Blueprint: The evolutionary origins of a good society. Hachette UK.Google Scholar
Cialdini, R. B., Brown, S. L., Lewis, B. P., Luce, C., & Neuberg, S. L. (1997). Reinterpreting the empathy-altruism relationship: When one into one equals oneness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73(3), 481494.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cikara, M., Jenkins, A. C., Dufour, N., & Saxe, R. (2014). Reduced self-referential neural response during intergroup competition predicts competitor harm. NeuroImage, 96, 3643.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohn, A., Maréchal, M. A., Tannenbaum, D., & Zünd, C. L. (2019). Civic honesty around the globe. Science, 365(6448), 7073.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cooper, D. J., & Dutcher, E. G. (2011). The dynamics of responder behavior in ultimatum games: A meta-study. Experimental Economics, 14, 519546.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cooper, D. J., & Kagel, J. H. (2016). Other-regarding preferences. In Kagel, J. H. & Roth, A. E. (Eds.), The handbook of experimental economics (Vol. 2, pp. 217275). Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Coulombe, B. R., Rudd, K. L., & Yates, T. M. (2019). Children’s physiological reactivity in emotion contexts and prosocial behavior. Brain and Behavior, 9(10), Article e01380.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crockett, M. J., Kurth-Nelson, Z., Siegel, J. Z., Dayan, P., & Dolan, R. J. (2014). Harm to others outweighs harm to self in moral decision making. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 111(48), 1732017325.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Curry, O. S., Rowland, L. A., Van Lissa, C. J., Zlotowitz, S., McAlaney, J., & Whitehouse, H. (2018). Happy to help? A systematic review and meta-analysis of the effects of performing acts of kindness on the well-being of the actor. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 76, 320329.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Darby, R. R., Horn, A., Cushman, F., & Fox, M. D. (2018). Lesion network localization of criminal behavior. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 115(3), 601606.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Darley, J. M., & Batson, C. D. (1973). “From Jerusalem to Jericho”: A study of situational and dispositional variables in helping behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 27(1), 100108.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
D’Arms, J., & Jacobson, D. (1994). Expressivism, morality, and the emotions. Ethics, 104(4), 739763.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
De Waal, F. B. M. (2008). Putting the altruism back into altruism: The evolution of empathy. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 279300.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
De Waal, F. B. M., & Suchak, M. (2010). Prosocial primates: Selfish and unselfish motivations. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 365(1553), 27112722.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Decety, J. (2011). Dissecting the neural mechanisms mediating empathy. Emotion Review, 3(1), 92108.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Decety, J., Michalska, K. J., & Akitsuki, Y. (2008). Who caused the pain? An fMRI investigation of empathy and intentionality in children. Neuropsychologia, 46(11), 26072614.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
DeSteno, D., Bartlett, M. Y., Baumann, J., Williams, L. A., & Dickens, L. (2010). Gratitude as moral sentiment: Emotion-guided cooperation in economic exchange. Emotion, 10(2), 289293.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dondi, M., Simion, F., & Caltran, G. (1999). Can newborns discriminate between their own cry and the cry of another newborn infant? Developmental Psychology, 35(2), 418426.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Donohue, M. R., & Tully, E. C. (2019). Reparative prosocial behaviors alleviate children’s guilt. Developmental Psychology, 55(10), 21022113.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Drummond, J. D. K., Hammond, S. I., Satlof-Bedrick, E., Waugh, W. E., & Brownell, C. A. (2017). Helping the one you hurt: Toddlers’ rudimentary guilt, shame, and prosocial behavior after harming another. Child Development, 88(4), 13821397.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eckel, C. C., & Wilson, R. K. (2004). Is trust a risky decision? Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization, 55(4), 447465.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eisenberg, N. (2000). Emotion, regulation, and moral development. Annual Review of Psychology, 51(1), 665697.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eisenberg, N. (2014). Altruistic emotion, cognition, and behavior (PLE: Emotion). Psychology Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., Miller, P. A., Fultz, J., Shell, R., Mathy, R. M., & Reno, R. R. (1989). Relation of sympathy and personal distress to prosocial behavior: A multimethod study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57(1), 5566.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., Murphy, B., Karbon, M., Maszk, P., Smith, M., O’Boyle, C., & Suh, K. (1994). The relations of emotionality and regulation to dispositional and situational empathy-related responding. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66(4), 776797.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eisenberger, N. I., & Lieberman, M. D. (2004). Why rejection hurts: A common neural alarm system for physical and social pain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(7), 294300.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eldar, E., Rutledge, R. B., Dolan, R. J., & Niv, Y. (2016). Mood as representation of momentum. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 20(1), 1524.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Embrey, M., Fréchette, G. R., & Yuksel, S. (2018). Cooperation in the finitely repeated prisoner’s dilemma. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 133(1), 509551.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Engel, C. (2011). Dictator games: A meta study. Experimental Economics, 14, 583610.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Engelmann, J. M., Haux, L. M., & Herrmann, E. (2019). Helping in young children and chimpanzees shows partiality towards friends. Evolution and Human Behavior, 40(3), 292300.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Essler, S., Lepach, A. C., Petermann, F., & Paulus, M. (2020). Equality, equity, or inequality duplication? How preschoolers distribute necessary and luxury resources between rich and poor others. Social Development, 19(1), 110125.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Etkin, A., Büchel, C., & Gross, J. J. (2015). The neural bases of emotion regulation. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 16(11), 693700.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fairley, K., Sanfey, A., Vyrastekova, J., & Weitzel, U. (2016). Trust and risk revisited. Journal of Economic Psychology, 57, 7485.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fehr, E. (2009). On the economics and biology of trust. Journal of the European Economic Association, 7(2–3), 235266.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fehr, E., & Camerer, C. F. (2007). Social neuroeconomics: The neural circuitry of social preferences. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(10), 419427.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fehr, E., & Fischbacher, U. (2003). The nature of human altruism. Nature, 425(6960), 785791.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fehr, E., & Gächter, S. (2000). Cooperation and punishment in public goods experiments. American Economic Review, 90(4), 980994.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fehr, E., Kirchsteiger, G., & Riedl, A. (1993). Does fairness prevent market clearing? An experimental investigation. The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 108(2), 437459.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fehr, E., & Schmidt, K. M. (1999). A theory of fairness, competition, and cooperation. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 114(3), 817868.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
FeldmanHall, O., Dalgleish, T., Evans, D., & Mobbs, D. (2015). Empathic concern drives costly altruism. NeuroImage, 105, 347356.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
FeldmanHall, O., Dalgleish, T., & Mobbs, D. (2013). Alexithymia decreases altruism in real social decisions. Cortex, 49(3), 899904.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
FeldmanHall, O., Dunsmoor, J. E., Tompary, A., Hunter, L. E., Todorov, A., & Phelps, E. A. (2018). Stimulus generalization as a mechanism for learning to trust. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 115(7), E1690E1697.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
FeldmanHall, O., Mobbs, D., Evans, D., Hiscox, L., Navrady, L., & Dalgleish, T. (2012). What we say and what we do: The relationship between real and hypothetical moral choices. Cognition, 123(3), 434441.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
FeldmanHall, O., Otto, A. R., & Phelps, E. A. (2018). Learning moral values: Another’s desire to punish enhances one’s own punitive behavior. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 147(8), 12111224.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
FeldmanHall, O., & Shenhav, A. (2019). Resolving uncertainty in a social world. Nature Human Behaviour, 3(5), 426435.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
FeldmanHall, O., Sokol-Hessner, P., Van Bavel, J. J., & Phelps, E. A. (2014). Fairness violations elicit greater punishment on behalf of another than for oneself. Nature Communications, 5(1), Article 5306.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fellows, L. K., Heberlein, A. S., Morales, D. A., Shivde, G., Waller, S., & Wu, D. H. (2005). Method matters: An empirical study of impact in cognitive neuroscience. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17(6), 850858.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ferguson, T. J., & Stegge, H. (1998). Measuring guilt in children: A rose by any other name still has thorns. In Bybee, J. (Ed.), Guilt and children (pp. 1974). Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fischbacher, U., & Föllmi-Heusi, F. (2013). Lies in disguise: An experimental study on cheating. Journal of the European Economic Association, 11(3), 525547.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Forsythe, R., Horowitz, J. L., Savin, N. E., & Sefton, M. (1994). Fairness in simple bargaining experiments. Games and Economic Behavior, 6(3), 347369.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Galizzi, M. M., & Navarro-Martinez, D. (2019). On the external validity of social preference games: A systematic lab-field study. Management Science, 65(3), 9761002.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gallo, S., Paracampo, R., Müller-Pinzler, L., Severo, M. C., Blömer, L., Fernandes-Henriques, C., Henschel, A., Lammes, B. K., Maskaljunas, T., Suttrup, J., Avenanti, A., Keysers, C., & Gazzola, V. (2018). The causal role of the somatosensory cortex in prosocial behaviour. eLife, 7, Article e32740.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gangopadhyay, P., Chawla, M., Dal Monte, O., & Chang, S. W. C. (2021). Prefrontal–amygdala circuits in social decision-making. Nature Neuroscience, 24(1), 518.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gerlach, P., Teodorescu, K., & Hertwig, R. (2019). The truth about lies: A meta-analysis on dishonest behavior. Psychological Bulletin, 145(1), 144.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Giner-Sorolla, R., & Espinosa, P. (2011). Social cuing of guilt by anger and of shame by disgust. Psychological Science, 22(1), 4953.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Graziano, W. G., & Eisenberg, N. H. (1997). Agreeableness: A dimension of personality. In Hogan, R., Johnson, J., & Briggs, S. (Eds.), Handbook of personality psychology (pp. 795824). Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Güth, W., Schmittberger, R., & Schwarze, B. (1982). An experimental analysis of ultimatum bargaining. Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization, 3(4), 367388.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gweon, H., Dodell-Feder, D., Bedny, M., & Saxe, R. (2012). Theory of mind performance in children correlates with functional specialization of a brain region for thinking about thoughts. Child Development, 83(6), 18531868.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hamilton, W. D. (1964). The genetical evolution of social behaviour. II. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 7(1), 1752.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hamlin, J. K., & Wynn, K. (2012). Who knows what’s good to eat? Infants fail to match the food preferences of antisocial others. Cognitive Development, 27(3), 227239.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hamlin, J. K., Wynn, K., & Bloom, P. (2007). Social evaluation by preverbal infants. Nature, 450(7169), 557559.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haney, C., Banks, C., & Zimbardo, P. (1973). Interpersonal dynamics in a simulated prison. International Journal of Criminology and Penology, 1, 6997.Google Scholar
Harbaugh, W. T., Mayr, U., & Burghart, D. R. (2007). Neural responses to taxation and voluntary giving reveal motives for charitable donations. Science, 316(5831), 16221625.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hare, B. (2017). Survival of the friendliest: Homo sapiens evolved via selection for prosociality. Annual Review of Psychology, 68, 155186.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harenski, C. L., Harenski, K. A., Shane, M. S., & Kiehl, K. A. (2010). Aberrant neural processing of moral violations in criminal psychopaths. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 119(4), 863874.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haruno, M., & Frith, C. D. (2010). Activity in the amygdala elicited by unfair divisions predicts social value orientation. Nature Neuroscience, 13(2), 160161.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hauser, O. P., Rand, D. G., Peysakhovich, A., & Nowak, M. A. (2014). Cooperating with the future. Nature, 511(7508), 220223.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hayashi, N., Ostrom, E., Walker, J., & Yamagishi, T. (1999). Reciprocity, trust, and the sense of control: A cross-societal study. Rationality and Society, 11(1), 2746.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heffner, J., & FeldmanHall, O. (2019). Why we don’t always punish: Preferences for non-punitive responses to moral violations. Scientific Reports, 9(1), 113.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heffner, J., & FeldmanHall, O. (2022). A probabilistic map of emotional experiences during competitive social interactions. Nature Communications, 13(1), Article 1718.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heffner, J., Son, J. Y., & FeldmanHall, O. (2021). Emotion prediction errors guide socially adaptive behaviour. Nature Human Behaviour, 5(10), 13911401.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hein, G., Lamm, C., Brodbeck, C., & Singer, T. (2011). Skin conductance response to the pain of others predicts later costly helping. PLoS ONE, 6(8), Article e22759.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Henrich, J., Boyd, R., Bowles, S., Camerer, C., Fehr, E., Gintis, H., & McElreath, R. (2001). In search of Homo economicus: Behavioral experiments in 15 small-scale societies. American Economic Review, 91(2), 7378.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hepach, R., Engelmann, J. M., Herrmann, E., Gerdemann, S. C., & Tomasello, M. (2022). Evidence for a developmental shift in the motivation underlying helping in early childhood. Developmental Science, 26(1), Article e13253.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hernandez-Lallement, J., Attah, A. T., Soyman, E., Pinhal, C. M., Gazzola, V., & Keysers, C. (2020). Harm to others acts as a negative reinforcer in rats. Current Biology, 30(6), 946961.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hershcovis, M. S., & Bhatnagar, N. (2017). When fellow customers behave badly: Witness reactions to employee mistreatment by customers. Journal of Applied Psychology, 102(11), 15281544.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoffman, M. L. (2000). Introduction and overview. In Empathy and moral development: Implications for caring and justice (pp. 128). Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hu, J., Hu, Y., Li, Y., & Zhou, X. (2021). Computational and neurobiological substrates of cost-benefit integration in altruistic helping decision. Journal of Neuroscience, 41(15), 35453561.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hume, D. (1896). A treatise of human nature. Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
Inman, C. S., Bijanki, K. R., Bass, D. I., Gross, R. E., Hamann, S., & Willie, J. T. (2020). Human amygdala stimulation effects on emotion physiology and emotional experience. Neuropsychologia, 145, Article 106722.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Isen, A. M., & Levin, P. F. (1972). Effect of feeling good on helping: Cookies and kindness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 21(3), 384388.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jolly, E., & Chang, L. J. (2021). Gossip drives vicarious learning and facilitates social connection. Current Biology, 31(12), 25392549.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jordan, J. J., Hoffman, M., Nowak, M. A., & Rand, D. G. (2016). Uncalculating cooperation is used to signal trustworthiness. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 113(31), 86588663.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jordan, J. J., & Rand, D. G. (2020). Signaling when no one is watching: A reputation heuristics account of outrage and punishment in one-shot anonymous interactions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 118(1), 5788.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kédia, G., Berthoz, S., Wessa, M., Hilton, D., & Martinot, J. L. (2008). An agent harms a victim: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study on specific moral emotions. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20(10), 17881798.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keltner, D., Horberg, E. J., & Oveis, C. (2006). Emotions as moral intuitions. In Forgas, J. P. (Ed.), Affect in social thinking and behavior (pp. 161175). Taylor & Francis.Google Scholar
Kemeny, M. E., Foltz, C., Cavanagh, J. F., Cullen, M., Giese-Davis, J., Jennings, P., Rosenberg, E. L., Gillath, O., Shaver, P. R., Wallace, B. A., & Ekman, P. (2012). Contemplative/emotion training reduces negative emotional behavior and promotes prosocial responses. Emotion, 12(2), 338350.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koenigs, M., & Tranel, D. (2007). Irrational economic decision-making after ventromedial prefrontal damage: Evidence from the ultimatum game. Journal of Neuroscience, 27(4), 951956.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koscik, T. R., & Tranel, D. (2011). The human amygdala is necessary for developing and expressing normal interpersonal trust. Neuropsychologia, 49(4), 602611.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koster-Hale, J., Saxe, R., Dungan, J., & Young, L. L. (2013). Decoding moral judgments from neural representations of intentions. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 110(14), 56485653.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kriegeskorte, N., Mur, M., & Bandettini, P. (2008). Representational similarity analysis: Connecting the branches of systems neuroscience. Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience, 2, Article 4.Google ScholarPubMed
Lamba, A., Frank, M. J., & FeldmanHall, O. (2020). Anxiety impedes adaptive social learning under uncertainty. Psychological Science, 31(5), 592603.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Latané, B., & Darley, J. M. (1970). The unresponsive bystander: Why doesn’t he help? Appleton-Century-Crofts.Google Scholar
Levitt, S. D., & List, J. A. (2007). What do laboratory experiments measuring social preferences reveal about the real world? Journal of Economic Perspectives, 21(2), 153174.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lockwood, P. L., Abdurahman, A., Gabay, A. S., Drew, D., Tamm, M., Husain, M., & Apps, M. A. J. (2021). Aging increases prosocial motivation for effort. Psychological Science, 668(681), 3235.Google Scholar
Lockwood, P. L., Apps, M. A. J., & Chang, S. W. C. (2020). Is there a “social” brain? Implementations and algorithms. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 24(10), 802813.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lockwood, P. L., Hamonet, M., Zhang, S. H., Ratnavel, A., Salmony, F. U., Husain, M., & Apps, M. A. J. (2017). Prosocial apathy for helping others when effort is required. Nature Human Behaviour, 1(7), Article 0131.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lucca, K., Capelier-Mourguy, A., Byers-Heinlein, K., Cirelli, L., Dal Ben, R., Frank, M. C., Henderson, A. M. E., Kominsky, J. F., Liberman, Z., Margoni, F., Reschke, P. J., Schlingloff, L., Scott, K., Soderstrom, M., Sommerville, J., Su, Y., Tatone, D., Uzefovsky, F., Wang, Y., & Hamlin, K. (in press). Infants’ social evaluation of helpers and hinderers: A large-scale, multi-lab, coordinated replication study. Developmental Science.Google Scholar
Malti, T., & Krettenauer, T. (2013). The relation of moral emotion attributions to prosocial and antisocial behavior: A meta-analysis. Child Development, 84(2), 397412.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marsh, A. A., Stoycos, S. A., Brethel-Haurwitz, K. M., Robinson, P., VanMeter, J. W., & Cardinale, E. M. (2014). Neural and cognitive characteristics of extraordinary altruists. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 111(42), 1503615041.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Masserman, J. H., Wechkin, S., & Terris, W. (1964). “Altruistic” behavior in rhesus monkeys. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 121(6), 584585.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Masten, C. L., Morelli, S. A., & Eisenberger, N. I. (2011). An fMRI investigation of empathy for “social pain” and subsequent prosocial behavior. NeuroImage, 55(1), 381388.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mattan, B. D., Barth, D. M., Thompson, A., FeldmanHall, O., Cloutier, J., & Kubota, J. T. (2020). Punishing the privileged: Selfish offers from high-status allocators elicit greater punishment from third-party arbitrators. PLoS ONE, 15(5), Article e0232369.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mazar, N., Amir, O., & Ariely, D. (2008). The dishonesty of honest people: A theory of self-concept maintenance. Journal of Marketing Research, 45(6), 633644.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meyza, K. Z., & Knapska, E. (2018). What can rodents teach us about empathy? Current Opinion in Psychology, 24, 1520.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meyza, K. Z., Bartal, I. B. A., Monfils, M. H., Panksepp, J. B., & Knapska, E. (2017). The roots of empathy: Through the lens of rodent models. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 76, 216234.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Milgram, S. (1963). Behavioral study of obedience. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 67(4), 371378.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Milinski, M., Sommerfeld, R. D., Krambeck, H. J., Reed, F. A., & Marotzke, J. (2008). The collective-risk social dilemma and the prevention of simulated dangerous climate change. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 105(7), 22912294.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moll, J., & de Oliveira-Souza, R. (2007). Moral judgments, emotions and the utilitarian brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(8), 319321.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morelli, S. A., Rameson, L. T., & Lieberman, M. D. (2014). The neural components of empathy: Predicting daily prosocial behavior. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 9(1), 3947.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Murphy, R. O., & Ackermann, K. A. (2014). Social value orientation: Theoretical and measurement issues in the study of social preferences. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 18(1), 1341.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ng, S. H. (1986). Equity, intergroup bias and interpersonal bias in reward allocation. European Journal of Social Psychology, 16(3), 239255.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nowak, M. A., & Sigmund, K. (2005). Evolution of indirect reciprocity. Nature, 437(7063), 12911298.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Over, H., Vaish, A., & Tomasello, M. (2016). Do young children accept responsibility for the negative actions of ingroup members? Cognitive Development, 40, 2432.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Penner, L. A., Dovidio, J. F., Piliavin, J. A., & Schroeder, D. A. (2005). Prosocial behavior: Multilevel perspectives. Annual Review of Psychology, 56, 365392.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Phelps, E. A., & LeDoux, J. E. (2005). Contributions of the amygdala to emotion processing: From animal models to human behavior. Neuron, 48(2), 175187.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Quoidbach, J., Taquet, M., Desseilles, M., de Montjoye, Y. A., & Gross, J. J. (2019). Happiness and social behavior. Psychological Science, 30(8), 11111122.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rand, D. G., & Nowak, M. A. (2013). Human cooperation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17(8), 413425.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Range, F., Horn, L., Viranyi, Z., & Huber, L. (2009). The absence of reward induces inequity aversion in dogs. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 106(1), 340345.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Richardson, H., Lisandrelli, G., Riobueno-Naylor, A., & Saxe, R. (2018). Development of the social brain from age three to twelve years. Nature Communications, 9(1), Article 1027.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rilling, J. K., Gutman, D. A., Zeh, T. R., Pagnoni, G., Berns, G. S., & Kilts, C. D. (2002). A neural basis for social cooperation. Neuron, 35(2), 395405.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rilling, J. K., Sanfey, A. G., Aronson, J. A., Nystrom, L. E., & Cohen, J. D. (2004). Opposing BOLD responses to reciprocated and unreciprocated altruism in putative reward pathways. NeuroReport, 15(16), 2539–2243.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sally, D. (1995). Conversation and cooperation in social dilemmas: A meta-analysis of experiments from 1958 to 1992. Rationality and Society, 7(1), 5892.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Saxe, R., & Kanwisher, N. (2003). People thinking about thinking people: The role of the temporo-parietal junction in “theory of mind.” NeuroImage, 19(4), 18351842.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schreuders, E., Klapwijk, E. T., Will, G. J., & Güroğlu, B. (2018). Friend versus foe: Neural correlates of prosocial decisions for liked and disliked peers. Cognitive, Affective and Behavioral Neuroscience, 18, 127142.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shalvi, S., Handgraaf, M. J. J., & De Dreu, C. K. W. (2011). People avoid situations that enable them to deceive others. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 47(6), 10961106.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shaw, L. L., Batson, C. D., & Todd, R. M. (1994). Empathy avoidance: Forestalling feeling for another in order to escape the motivational consequences. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67(5), 879887.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sherif, M., Harvey, O. J., White, B. J., Hood, W. R., & Sherif, C. W. (1961). Intergroup conflict and cooperation: The Robbers Cave experiment. University of Oklahoma Book Exchange.Google Scholar
Shirtcliff, E. A., Vitacco, M. J., Graf, A. R., Gostisha, A. J., Merz, J. L., & Zahn-Waxler, C. (2009). Neurobiology of empathy and callousness: Implications for the development of antisocial behavior. Behavioral Sciences and the Law, 27(2), 137171.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sifneos, P. E. (1973). The prevalence of “alexithymic” characteristics in psychosomatic patients. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 22(2–6), 255262.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Singer, T., Seymour, B., O’Doherty, J., Kaube, H., Dolan, R. J., & Frith, C. D. (2004). Empathy for pain involves the affective but not sensory components of pain. Science, 303(5661), 11571162.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, A. (1776). An inquiry into the nature and causes of the wealth of nations. Methuen.Google Scholar
Smith, A. (1853). The theory of moral sentiments. HG Bohn.Google Scholar
Son, J. Y., Bhandari, A., & FeldmanHall, O. (2019). Crowdsourcing punishment: Individuals reference group preferences to inform their own punitive decisions. Scientific Reports, 9(1), 115.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stallen, M., Rossi, F., Heijne, A., Smidts, A., De Dreu, C. K. W., & Sanfey, A. G. (2018). Neurobiological mechanisms of responding to injustice. Journal of Neuroscience, 38(12), 29442954.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stearns, D. C., & Parrott, W. G. (2012). When feeling bad makes you look good: Guilt, shame, and person perception. Cognition and Emotion, 26(3), 407430.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Svetlova, M., Nichols, S. R., & Brownell, C. A. (2010). Toddlers’ prosocial behavior: From instrumental to empathic to altruistic helping. Child Development, 81(6), 18141827.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Takahashi, H., Yahata, N., Koeda, M., Matsuda, T., Asai, K., & Okubo, Y. (2004). Brain activation associated with evaluative processes of guilt and embarrassment: An fMRI study. NeuroImage, 23(3), 967974.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tangney, J. P., Stuewig, J., & Mashek, D. J. (2007). Moral emotions and moral behavior. Annual Review of Psychology, 58, 345372.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tavoni, A., Dannenberg, A., Kallis, G., & Löschel, A. (2011). Inequality, communication, and the avoidance of disastrous climate change in a public goods game. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 108(29), 1182511829.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Taylor, G. J., & Bagby, R. M. (2000). An overview of the alexithymia construct. In Bar-On, R. & Parker, J. D. A. (Eds.), The handbook of emotional intelligence: Theory, development, assessment, and application at home, school, and in the workplace (pp. 4067). Jossey-Bass/Wiley.Google Scholar
Telzer, E. H., Masten, C. L., Berkman, E. T., Lieberman, M. D., & Fuligni, A. J. (2011). Neural regions associated with self control and mentalizing are recruited during prosocial behaviors towards the family. NeuroImage, 58(1), 242249.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thielmann, I., Spadaro, G., & Balliet, D. (2020). Personality and prosocial behavior: A theoretical framework and meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 146(1), 3090.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Todorov, A., & Engell, A. D. (2008). The role of the amygdala in implicit evaluation of emotionally neutral faces. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 3(4), 303312.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Toi, M., & Batson, C. D. (1982). More evidence that empathy is a source of altruistic motivation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 43(2), 281292.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Trivers, R. L. (1971). The evolution of reciprocal altruism. The Quarterly Review of Biology, 46(1), 3557.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vaish, A. (2018). The prosocial functions of early social emotions: The case of guilt. Current Opinion in Psychology, 20, 2529.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vaish, A., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2016). The early emergence of guilt-motivated prosocial behavior. Child Development, 87(6), 17721782.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Valdesolo, P., & Desteno, D. (2006). Manipulations of emotional context shape moral judgment. Psychological Science, 17(6), 476477.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Baar, J. M., Halpern, D. J., & FeldmanHall, O. (2021). Intolerance of uncertainty modulates brain-to-brain synchrony during politically polarized perception. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 118(20), Article e2022491118.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Hoorn, J., Fuligni, A. J., Crone, E. A., & Galván, A. (2016). Peer influence effects on risk-taking and prosocial decision-making in adolescence: Insights from neuroimaging studies. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 10, 5964.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vives, M.-L., Cikara, M., & FeldmanHall, O. (2022). Following your group or your morals? The in-group promotes immoral behavior while the out-group buffers against it. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 13(1), 139149.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vives, M. L., & FeldmanHall, O. (2018). Tolerance to ambiguous uncertainty predicts prosocial behavior. Nature Communications, 9(1), 19.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Warneken, F., Hare, B., Melis, A. P., Hanus, D., & Tomasello, M. (2007). Spontaneous altruism by chimpanzees and young children. PLoS Biology, 5(7), Article e184.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Warneken, F., & Tomasello, M. (2006). Altruistic helping in human infants and young chimpanzees. Science, 311(5765), 13011303.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weisel, O., & Shalvi, S. (2015). The collaborative roots of corruption. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 112(34), 1065110656.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wichman, H. (1970). Effects of isolation and communication on cooperation in a two-person game. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 16(1), 114120.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wills, J., FeldmanHall, O., Meager, M. R., Van Bavel, J. J., Blackmon, K., Devinsky, O., Doyle, W. K., Luciano, D. J., Kuzniecky, R. I., Nadkarni, S. S., Vazquez, B., Najjar, S., Geller, E., Golfinos, J. G., Placantonakis, D. G., Friedman, D., Wisoff, J. H., & Samadani, U. (2018). Dissociable contributions of the prefrontal cortex in group-based cooperation. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 13(4), 349356.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilson, M. L., & Wrangham, R. W. (2003). Intergroup relations in chimpanzees. Annual Review of Anthropology, 32(1), 363392.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Woo, B. M., Steckler, C. M., Le, D. T., & Hamlin, J. K. (2017). Social evaluation of intentional, truly accidental, and negligently accidental helpers and harmers by 10-month-old infants. Cognition, 168, 154163.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Xiao, E., & Houser, D. (2005). Emotion expression in human punishment behavior. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 102(20), 73987401.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Young, L., & Saxe, R. (2009). An fMRI investigation of spontaneous mental state inference for moral judgment. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21(7), 13961405.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zahn-Waxler, C., Radke-Yarrow, M., & King, R. (1983). Early altruism and guilt. Academic Psychology Bulletin, 5(2), 247259.Google Scholar
Zaki, J. (2014). Empathy: A motivated account. Psychological Bulletin, 140(6), 16081647.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zaki, J., & Ochsner, K. (2012). The neuroscience of empathy: Progress, pitfalls and promise. Nature Neuroscience, 15(5), 675680.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zapparoli, L., Seghezzi, S., Scifo, P., Zerbi, A., Banfi, G., Tettamanti, M., & Paulesu, E. (2018). Dissecting the neurofunctional bases of intentional action. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 115(28), 74407445.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zhu, L., Jenkins, A. C., Set, E., Scabini, D., Knight, R. T., Chiu, P. H., King-Casas, B., & Hsu, M. (2014). Damage to dorsolateral prefrontal cortex affects tradeoffs between honesty and self-interest. Nature Neuroscience, 17(10), 13191321.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

References

Aharoni, E., Antonenko, O., & Kiehl, K. A. (2011). Disparities in the moral intuitions of criminal offenders: The role of psychopathy. Journal of Research in Personality, 45(3), 322327.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aharoni, E., Funk, C., Sinnott-Armstrong, W., & Gazzaniga, M. (2008). Can neurological evidence help courts assess criminal responsibility? Lessons from law and neuroscience. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1124(1), 145160.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aharoni, E., Sinnott-Armstrong, W., & Kiehl, K. A. (2012). Can psychopathic offenders discern moral wrongs? A new look at the moral/conventional distinction. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 121(2), 484497.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Aharoni, E., Sinnott-Armstrong, W., & Kiehl, K. A. (2014). What’s wrong? Moral understanding in psychopathic offenders. Journal of Research in Personality, 53, 175181.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
American Psychiatric Association. (1952). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (1st ed.).Google Scholar
American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.).Google Scholar
Anderson, N. E., Stanford, M. S., Wan, L., & Young, K. A. (2011). High psychopathic trait females exhibit reduced startle potentiation and increased P3 amplitude. Behavioral Sciences and the Law, 29(5), 649666.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aniskiewicz, A. S. (1979). Autonomic components of vicarious conditioning and psychopathy. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 35(1), 6067.3.0.CO;2-R>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bartels, D. M., & Pizarro, D. A. (2011). The mismeasure of morals: Antisocial personality traits predict utilitarian responses to moral dilemmas. Cognition, 121(1), 154161.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baskin-Sommers, A. R., Curtin, J. J., & Newman, J. P. (2015). Altering the cognitive-affective dysfunctions of psychopathic and externalizing offender subtypes with cognitive remediation. Clinical Psychological Science: A Journal of the Association for Psychological Science, 3(1), 4557.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baskin-Sommers, A. R., & Newman, J. P. (2013). Differentiating the cognition-emotion interactions that characterize psychopathy versus externalizing. In Robinson, M. D., Watkins, E., & Harmon-Jones, E. (Eds.), Handbook of cognition and emotion (pp. 501520). Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Berg, J. M., Lilienfeld, S. O., & Waldman, I. D. (2013). Bargaining with the devil: Using economic decision-making tasks to examine the heterogeneity of psychopathic traits. Journal of Research in Personality, 47(5), 472482.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blackburn, R. (2006). Other theoretical models of psychopathy. In Patrick, C. J. (Ed.), Handbook of psychopathy (pp. 3557). Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Blair, R. J. R. (1995). A cognitive developmental approach to morality: Investigating the psychopath. Cognition, 57(1), 129.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blair, R. J. R. (1997). Moral reasoning and the child with psychopathic tendencies. Personality and Individual Differences, 22(5), 731739.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blair, R. J. R. (2003). Neurobiological basis of psychopathy. British Journal of Psychiatry, 182(1), 57.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blair, R. J. R. (2006). The emergence of psychopathy: Implications for the neuropsychological approach to developmental disorders. Cognition, 101(2), 414442.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blair, R. J. R. (2007). The amygdala and ventromedial prefrontal cortex in morality and psychopathy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(9), 387392.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blair, R. J. R. (2008). Fine cuts of empathy and the amygdala: Dissociable deficits in psychopathy and autism. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61(1), 157170.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blair, R. J. R., Budhani, S., Colledge, E., & Scott, S. (2005). Deafness to fear in boys with psychopathic tendencies. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 46(3), 327336.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blair, R. J. R, Jones, L., Clark, F., & Smith, M. (1995). Is the psychopath “morally insane”? Personality and Individual Differences, 19(5), 741752.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blair, R. J. R, Jones, L., Clark, F., & Smith, M. (1997). The psychopathic individual: A lack of responsiveness to distress cures? Psychophysiology, 34(2), 192198.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blair, R. J. R., Mitchell, D. G. V., Peschardt, K. S., Colledge, E., Leonard, R. A., Shine, J. H., Murray, L. K., & Perrett, D. I. (2004). Reduced sensitivity to others’ fearful expressions in psychopathic individuals. Personality and Individual Differences, 37(6), 11111122.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Borg, J. S., & Sinnott-Armstrong, W. P. (2013). Do psychopaths make moral judgments? In Kiehl, K. A. & Sinnott-Armstrong, W. P. (Eds.), Handbook of psychopathy and law (pp. 107128). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Bryant, D. J., Wang, F., Deardeuff, K., Zoccoli, E., & Nam, C. (2016). The neural correlates of moral thinking: A meta-analysis. International Journal of Computational & Neural Engineering, 3(2), 2839.Google Scholar
Caldwell, M., Skeem, J., Salekin, R., & Van Rybroek, G. (2006). Treatment response of adolescent offenders with psychopathy features: A 2-year follow-up. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 33(5), 571596.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Campbell, M. A., Doucette, N. L., & French, S. (2009). Validity and stability of the youth psychopathic traits inventory in a nonforensic sample of young adults. Journal of Personality Assessment, 91(6), 584592.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carlisi, C. O., Moffitt, T. E., Knodt, A. R., Harrington, H., Ireland, D., Melzer, T. R., Poulton, R., Ramrakha, S., Caspi, A., Hariri, A. R., & Viding, E. (2020). Associations between life-course-persistent antisocial behaviour and brain structure in a population-representative longitudinal birth cohort. The Lancet Psychiatry, 7(3), 245253.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carroll, K. M., & Kiluk, B. D. (2017). Cognitive behavioral interventions for alcohol and drug use disorders: Through the stage model and back again. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors: Journal of the Society of Psychologists in Addictive Behaviors, 31(8), 847861.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Caspi, A., Moffitt, T. E., Newman, D. L., & Silva, P. A. (1996). Behavioral observations at age 3 years predict adult psychiatric disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry, 53(11), 10331039.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Choy, O., Raine, A., & Hamilton, R. H. (2018). Stimulation of the prefrontal cortex reduces intentions to commit aggression: A randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled, stratified, parallel-group trial. Journal of Neuroscience, 38(29), 65056512.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cima, M., Tonnaer, F., & Hauser, M. D. (2010). Psychopaths know right from wrong but don’t care. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 5(1), 5967.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Corden, B., Critchley, H. D., Skuse, D., & Dolan, R. J. (2006). Fear recognition ability predicts differences in social cognitive and neural functioning in men. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18(6), 889897.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crockett, M. J., Siegel, J. Z., Kurth-Nelson, Z., Dayan, P., & Dolan, R. J. (2017). Moral transgressions corrupt neural representations of value. Nature Neuroscience, 20(6), 879885.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cushman, F., Gray, K., Gaffey, A., & Mendes, W. B. (2012). Simulating murder: The aversion to harmful action. Emotion, 12(1), 27.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cushman, F., Young, L., & Hauser, M. (2006). The role of conscious reasoning and intuition in moral judgment: Testing three principles of harm. Psychological Science, 17(12), 10821089.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Darby, R. R., Burke, M., & Fox, M. D. (2018). Network localization of disordered free will perception. Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 30(3), E6E6.Google Scholar
Del Gaizo, A. L., & Falkenbach, D. M. (2008). Primary and secondary psychopathic-traits and their relationship to perception and experience of emotion. Personality and Individual Differences, 45(3), 206212.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DeLisi, M., Vaughn, M. G., Gentile, D. A., Anderson, C. A., & Shook, J. J. (2013). Violent video games, delinquency, and youth violence: New evidence. Youth Violence and Juvenile Justice, 11(2), 132142.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eisenbarth, H., Alpers, G. W., Segrè, D., Calogero, A., & Angrilli, A. (2008). Categorization and evaluation of emotional faces in psychopathic women. Psychiatry Research, 159(1–2), 189195.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Endres, M. J., Rickert, M. E., Bogg, T., Lucas, J., & Finn, P. R. (2011). Externalizing psychopathology and behavioral disinhibition: Working memory mediates signal discriminability and reinforcement moderates response bias in approach–avoidance learning. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 120(2), 336351.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fairchild, G., Stobbe, Y., Van Goozen, S. H. M., Calder, A. J., & Goodyer, I. M. (2010). Facial expression recognition, fear conditioning, and startle modulation in female subjects with conduct disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 68(3), 272279.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fairchild, G., Van Goozen, S. H. M., Calder, A. J., Stollery, S. J., & Goodyer, I. M. (2009). Deficits in facial expression recognition in male adolescents with early-onset or adolescence-onset conduct disorder. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 50(5), 627636.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fernandes, S., Aharoni, E., Harenski, C. L., Caldwell, M., & Kiehl, K. A. (2020). Anomalous moral intuitions in juvenile offenders with psychopathic traits. Journal of Research in Personality, 86, Article 103962.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fiedler, S., & Glöckner, A. (2015). Attention and moral behavior. Current Opinion in Psychology, 6, 139144.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fine, C., & Kennett, J. (2004). Mental impairment, moral understanding and criminal responsibility: Psychopathy and the purposes of punishment. International Journal of Law and Psychiatry, 27(5), 425443.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fisher, K. A., & Hany, M. (2021). Antisocial personality disorder. In StatPearls. StatPearls Publishing.Google Scholar
Fodor, E. M. (1973). Moral development and parent behavior antecedents in adolescent psychopaths. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 122(1), 3743.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fridlund, A. J. (1991). Evolution and facial action in reflex, social motive, and paralanguage. Biological Psychology, 32(1), 3100.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Furnham, A., Daoud, Y., & Swami, V. (2009). “How to spot a psychopath”: Lay theories of psychopathy. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 44(6), 464472.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garrigan, B., Adlam, A. L. R., & Langdon, P. E. (2016). The neural correlates of moral decision-making: A systematic review and meta-analysis of moral evaluations and response decision judgements. Brain and Cognition, 108, 8897.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garrigan, B., Adlam, A. L. R., & Langdon, P. E. (2018). Moral decision-making and moral development: Toward an integrative framework. Developmental Review, 49, 80100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Glenn, A. L., Iyer, R., Graham, J., Koleva, S., & Haidt, J. (2009). Are all types of morality compromised in psychopathy? Journal of Personality Disorders, 23(4), 384398.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Graham, J., Nosek, B. A., Haidt, J., Iyer, R., Koleva, S., & Ditto, P. H. (2011). Mapping the moral domain. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 101(2), 366385.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greene, J. D., Sommerville, R. B., Nystrom, L. E., Darley, J. M., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). An fMRI investigation of emotional engagement in moral judgment. Science, 293(5537), 21052108.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haidt, J. (2012). The righteous mind: Why good people are divided by politics and religion. Pantheon.Google Scholar
Haidt, J., & Graham, J. (2007). When morality opposes justice: Conservatives have moral intuitions that liberals may not recognize. Social Justice Research, 20(1), 98116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Han, H. (2017). Neural correlates of moral sensitivity and moral judgment associated with brain circuitries of selfhood: A meta-analysis. Journal of Moral Education, 46(2), 97113.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hansen, A. L., Johnsen, H., Hart, S., Waage, L., & Thayer, J. F. (2008). Brief communication: Psychopathy and recognition of facial expressions of emotion. Journal of Personality Disorders, 22(6), 639645.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hare, R. D. (1991). The Hare Psychopathy Checklist–Revised. Multi-Health Systems.Google Scholar
Hare, R. D. (1999). Without conscience: The disturbing world of the psychopaths among us. Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Hare, R. D. (2003). The Hare Psychopathy Checklist–Revised (2nd ed.). Multi-Health Systems.Google Scholar
Hare, R. D., & Neumann, C. S. (2008). Psychopathy as a clinical and empirical construct. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 4, 217246.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harenski, C. L., Harenski, K. A., Shane, M. S., & Kiehl, K. A. (2010). Aberrant neural processing of moral violations in criminal psychopaths. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 119(4), 863874.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harris, G. T., & Rice, M. E. (2006). Treatment of psychopathy: A review of empirical findings. In Patrick, C. (Ed.), Handbook of psychopathy (pp. 555572). Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Hastings, M. E., Tangney, J. P., & Stuewig, J. (2008). Psychopathy and identification of facial expressions of emotion. Personality and Individual Differences, 44(7), 14741483.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hecht, L. K., Latzman, R. D., & Lilienfeld, S. O. (2018). The psychological treatment of psychopathy: Theory and research. In David, D., Lynn, S. J., & Montgomery, G. H. (Eds.), Evidence-based psychotherapy: The state of the science and practice (pp. 271298). Wiley-Blackwell.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Horne, Z., & Powell, D. (2016). How large is the role of emotion in judgments of moral dilemmas? PLoS ONE, 11(7), Article e0154780.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
House, T. H., & Milligan, W. L. (1976). Autonomic responses to modeled distress in prison psychopaths. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 34(4), 556560.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jambon, M., & Smetana, J. G. (2018). Individual differences in prototypical moral and conventional judgments and children’s proactive and reactive aggression. Child Development, 89(4), 13431359.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jonason, P. K., Strosser, G. L., Kroll, C. H., Duineveld, J. J., & Baruffi, S. A. (2015). Valuing myself over others: The dark triad traits and moral and social values. Personality and Individual Differences, 81, 102106.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Juni, S. (2010). Conceptualizing psychopathy: A psychodynamic approach. Journal of Aggression, Maltreatment & Trauma, 19(7), 777800.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jurkovic, G. J., & Prentice, N. M. (1977). Relation of moral and cognitive development to dimensions of juvenile delinquency. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 86(4), 414420.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kahn, E. (1931). Psychopathic personalities. Yale University Press.Google Scholar
Kiehl, K. A. (2015). The psychopath whisperer: The science of those without conscience. Crown.Google Scholar
Koch, J. L. A. (1891). Die psychopathischen Minderwertigkeiten (3 vols). Maier.Google Scholar
Koenigs, M., Kruepke, M., Zeier, J., & Newman, J. P. (2012). Utilitarian moral judgment in psychopathy. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 7(6), 708714.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kohlberg, L. (1958). The development of modes of moral thinking and choice in the years to sixteen. University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Kohlberg, L. (1963). The development of children’s orientations toward a moral order: I. Sequence in the development of moral thought. Vita Humana, 6(1–2), 1133.Google Scholar
Kraepelin, E. (1913). Clinical psychiatry: A textbook for physicians. Macmillan.Google Scholar
Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., & Cuthbert, B. N. (1990). Emotion, attention, and the startle reflex. Psychological Review, 97(3), 377395.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leist, T., & Dadds, M. R. (2009). Adolescents’ ability to read different emotional faces relates to their history of maltreatment and type of psychopathology. Clinical Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 14(2), 237250.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levenson, R. W. (2014). The autonomic nervous system and emotion. Emotion Review, 6(2), 100112.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levenston, G. K., Patrick, C. J., Bradley, M. M., & Lang, P. J. (2000). The psychopath as observer: Emotion and attention in picture processing. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 109(3), 373385.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levy, N. (2007). The responsibility of the psychopath revisited. Philosophy, Psychiatry, & Psychology, 14(2), 129138.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ling, S., Raine, A., Choy, O., & Hamilton, R. (2020). Effects of prefrontal cortical stimulation on aggressive and antisocial behavior: A double-blind, stratified, randomized, sham-controlled, parallel-group trial. Journal of Experimental Criminology, 16(3), 367387.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Link, N. F., Scherer, S. E., & Byrne, P. N. (1977). Moral judgment and moral conduct in the psychopath. Canadian Psychiatric Association Journal, 22(7), 341346.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Litton, P. (2013). Criminal responsibility and psychopathy: Do psychopaths have a right to excuse? In Kiehl, K. A. & Sinnott-Armstrong, W. P. (Eds.), Handbook of psychopathy and law (pp. 275296). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Livesley, W. J., Schroeder, M. L., Jackson, D. N., & Jang, K. L. (1994). Categorical distinctions in the study of personality disorder: Implications for classification. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 103(1), 617.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lose, C. A. (1977). Level of moral reasoning and psychopathy within a group of federal inmates. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering, 57(7-B), Article 4716.Google Scholar
Malle, B. F. (2021). Moral judgments. Annual Review of Psychology, 72, 293318.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marsh, A. A., & Blair, R. J. R. (2008). Deficits in facial affect recognition among antisocial populations: A meta-analysis. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 32(3), 454465.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marshall, J., Watts, A. L., & Lilienfeld, S. O. (2018). Do psychopathic individuals possess a misaligned moral compass? A meta-analytic examination of psychopathy’s relations with moral judgment. Personality Disorders: Theory, Research, and Treatment, 9(1), 4050.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Montagne, B., van Honk, J., Kessels, R. P. C., Frigerio, E., Burt, M., van Zandvoort, M. J. E., Perrett, D. I., & de Haan, E. H. F. (2005). Reduced efficiency in recognising fear in subjects scoring high on psychopathic personality characteristics. Personality and Individual Differences, 38(1), 511.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morse, S. J. (2008). Psychopathy and criminal responsibility. Neuroethics, 1(3), 205212.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Muñoz, L. C. (2009). Callous-unemotional traits are related to combined deficits in recognizing afraid faces and body poses. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 48(5), 554562.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Newman, J. P., MacCoon, D. G., Vaughn, L. J., & Sadeh, N. (2005). Validating a distinction between primary and secondary psychopathy with measures of Gray’s BIS and BAS constructs. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 114(2), 319323.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nichols, S. (2004). Sentimental rules: On the natural foundations of moral judgment. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nucci, L. P., & Nucci, M. S. (1982). Children’s responses to moral and social conventional transgressions in free-play settings. Child Development, 53(5), 13371342.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nucci, L. P., & Turiel, E. (1978). Social interactions and the development of social concepts in preschool children. Child Development, 49(2), 400407.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
O’Kane, A., Fawcett, D., & Blackburn, R. (1996). Psychopathy and moral reasoning: Comparison of two classifications. Personality and Individual Differences, 20(4), 505514.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Patrick, C. J. (2018). Handbook of psychopathy (2nd ed.). Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Patrick, C. J., Bradley, M. M., & Lang, P. J. (1993). Emotion in the criminal psychopath: Startle reflex modulation. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 102(1), 8292.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pearson, J. M., Heilbronner, S. R., Barack, D. L., Hayden, B. Y., & Platt, M. L. (2011). Posterior cingulate cortex: Adapting behavior to a changing world. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15(4), 143151.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pillsbury, S. H. (2013). Why psychopaths are responsible. In Kieh, K. A. & Sinnott-Armstrong, W. (Eds.), Handbook of psychopathy and law (pp. 297318). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Pinel, P. (1801). Traité médico-philosophique sur l’aliénation mentale, ou la manie. Richard, Caille & Ravier.Google Scholar
Pletti, C., Lotto, L., Buodo, G., & Sarlo, M. (2017). It’s immoral, but I’d do it! Psychopathy traits affect decision-making in sacrificial dilemmas and in everyday moral situations. British Journal of Psychology, 108(2), 351368.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Poon, K. (2016). Understanding risk-taking behavior in bullies, victims, and bully victims using cognitive- and emotion-focused approaches. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, Article 1838.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Poon, K., & Ho, C. S-H. (2015). Contrasting psychosocial outcomes in Chinese delinquent adolescents with attention deficit and hyperactivity disorder symptoms and/or reading disability. The Journal of Forensic Psychiatry & Psychology, 26(1), 3859.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Prichard, J. C. (1835). A treatise on insanity and other disorders affecting the mind. Sherwood, Gilbert, and Piper.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Raine, A. (1993). The psychopathology of crime: Criminal behavior as a clinical disorder. Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Raine, A. (2013). The anatomy of violence: The biological roots of crime. Pantheon Books.Google Scholar
Raine, A. (2019). The neuromoral theory of antisocial, violent, and psychopathic behavior. Psychiatry Research, 277, 6469.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raine, A., Buchsbaum, M., & LaCasse, L. (1997). Brain abnormalities in murderers indicated by positron emission tomography. Biological Psychiatry, 42(6), 495508.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raine, A., Buchsbaum, M. S., Stanley, J., Lottenberg, S., Abel, L., & Stoddard, J. (1994). Selective reductions in prefrontal glucose metabolism in murderers. Biological Psychiatry, 36(6), 365373.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raine, A., Chen, F. R., & Waller, R. (2022). The cognitive, affective and somatic empathy scales for adults. Personality and Individual Differences, 185, Article 111238.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raine, A., & Yang, Y. (2006). Neural foundations to moral reasoning and antisocial behavior. Social, Cognitive, and Affective Neuroscience, 1, 203213.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rebellon, C. J., Piquero, N. L., Piquero, A. R., & Tibbetts, S. G. (2010). Anticipated shaming and criminal offending. Journal of Criminal Justice, 38(5), 988997.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rest, J., Copper, D., Coder, R., Masanz, J., & Anderson, D. (1974). Judging the important issues in moral dilemmas: An objective test of development. Developmental Psychology, 10(4), 491501.Google Scholar
Ribeiro da Silva, D., Rijo, D., & Salekin, R. T. (2012). Child and adolescent psychopathy: A state-of-the-art reflection on the construct and etiological theories. Journal of Criminal Justice, 40(4), 269277.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Romero-Martínez, Á., Lila, M., Gracia, E., & Moya-Albiol, L. (2019). Improving empathy with motivational strategies in batterer intervention programmes: Results of a randomized controlled trial. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 58(2), 125139.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Romero-Martínez, Á., Lila, M., Martínez, M., Pedrón-Rico, V., & Moya-Albiol, L. (2016). Improvements in empathy and cognitive flexibility after court-mandated intervention program in intimate partner violence perpetrators: The role of alcohol abuse. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 13(4), Article 394.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sajous-Turner, A., Anderson, N. E., Widdows, M., Nyalakanti, P., Harenski, K., Harenski, C., Koenigs, M., Decety, J., & Kiehl, K. A. (2020). Aberrant brain gray matter in murderers. Brain Imaging and Behavior, 14(5), 20602061.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Salekin, R. T. (2002). Psychopathy and therapeutic pessimism: Clinical lore or clinical reality? Clinical Psychology Review, 22(1), 79112.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Salekin, R. T., Worley, C., & Grimes, R. D. (2010). Treatment of psychopathy: A review and brief introduction to the mental model approach for psychopathy. Behavioral Sciences and the Law, 28(2), 235266.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sargeant, M. N., Daughters, S. B., Curtin, J. J., Schuster, R. M., & Lejuez, C. W. (2011). Unique roles of antisocial personality disorder and psychopathic traits in distress tolerance. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 120(4), 987992.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schneider, F. (1923). Die Psychopathischen Persönlichkeiten [The psychopathic personalities]. Springer.Google Scholar
Skeem, J. L., Polaschek, D. L. L., Patrick, C. J., & Lilienfeld, S. O. (2011). Psychopathic personality: Bridging the gap between scientific evidence and public policy. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 12(3), 95162.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tangney, J. P., Stuewig, J., & Hafez, L. (2011). Shame, guilt, and remorse: Implications for offender populations. Journal of Forensic Psychiatry and Psychology, 22(5), 706723.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tassy, S., Deruelle, C., Mancini, J., Leistedt, S., & Wicker, B. (2013). High levels of psychopathic traits alters moral choice but not moral judgment. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, Article 229.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thomson, J. J. (1976). Killing, letting die, and the trolley problem. The Monist, 59(2), 204217.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Turiel, E. (1979). Distinct conceptual and developmental domains: Social-convention and morality. Nebraska symposium on motivation. University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
Turiel, E. (1983). The development of social knowledge: Morality and convention. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Tuvblad, C., Bezdjian, S., Raine, A., & Baker, L. A. (2013). Psychopathic personality and negative parent-to-child affect: A longitudinal cross-lag twin study. Journal of Criminal Justice, 41(5), 331341.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vaidyanathan, U., Patrick, C. J., & Bernat, E. M. (2009). Startle reflex potentiation during aversive picture viewing as an indicator of trait fear. Psychophysiology, 46(1), 7585.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vitale, J. E., Smith, S. S., Brinkley, C. A., & Newman, J. P. (2002). The reliability and validity of the Psychopathy Checklist–Revised in a sample of female offenders. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 29(2), 202231.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Volman, I., Toni, I., Verhagen, L., & Roelofs, K. (2011). Endogenous testosterone modulates prefrontal-amygdala connectivity during social emotional behavior. Cerebral Cortex, 21(10), 22822290.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
von Borries, A. K. L, Volman, I., de Bruijn, E. R. A., Bulten, B. H., Verkes, R. J., & Roelofs, K. (2012). Psychopaths lack the automatic avoidance of social threat: Relation to instrumental aggression. Psychiatry Research, 200(2–3), 761766.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Walker, D. W., & Leister, C. (1994). Recognition of facial affect cues by adolescents with emotional and behavioral disorders. Behavioral Disorders, 19(4), 269276.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wilson, K., Juodis, M., & Porter, S. (2011). Fear and loathing in psychopaths: A meta-analytic investigation of the facial affect recognition deficit. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 38(7), 659668.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Woodworth, M., & Waschbusch, D. (2008). Emotional processing in children with conduct problems and callous/unemotional traits. Child: Care, Health and Development, 34(2), 234244.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yang, Y., Raine, A., Colletti, P., Toga, A. W., & Narr, K. L. (2009). Abnormal temporal and prefrontal cortical gray matter thinning in psychopaths. Molecular Psychiatry, 14(6), 561562.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yang, Y., Wang, P., Baker, L. A., Narr, K. L., Joshi, S. H., Hafzalla, G., Raine, A., & Thompson, P. M. (2015). Thicker temporal cortex associates with a developmental trajectory for psychopathic traits in adolescents. PLoS ONE, 10(5), Article e0127025.Google ScholarPubMed

References

Appiah, A. (2006). Cosmopolitanism: Ethics on a world of strangers. Norton.Google Scholar
Bain, P., Vaes, J., Haslam, N., Kashima, Y., & Guan, Y. (2012). Folk psychologies of humanness: Beliefs about distinctive and core human characteristics in Australia, Italy, and China. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 43(1), 5358.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bandura, A. (1999). Moral disengagement in the perpetration of inhumanities. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 3(3), 193209.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bandura, A., Underwood, B., & Fromson, M. E. (1975). Disinhibition of aggression through diffusion of responsibility and dehumanization of victims. Journal of Research in Personality, 9(4), 253269.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bar-Tal, D. (1989). Delegitimization: The extreme case of stereotyping. In Bar-Tal, D., Grauman, C., Kruglanski, A., & Stroebe, W. (Eds.), Stereotyping and prejudice: Changing conceptions (pp. 169188). Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bastian, B., & Haslam, N. (2010). Excluded from humanity: Ostracism and dehumanization. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 46(1), 107113.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bastian, B., Jetten, J., & Radke, H. R. (2012). Cyber-dehumanization: Violent video game play diminishes our humanity. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48(2), 486491.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bastian, B., Laham, S. M., Wilson, S., Haslam, N., & Koval, P. (2011). Blaming, praising, and protecting our humanity: The implications of everyday dehumanization for judgments of moral status. British Journal of Social Psychology, 50(3), 469483.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brandt, M. J., & Reyna, C. (2011). The chain of being: A hierarchy of morality. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 6(5), 428446.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brewer, M. B. (1999). The psychology of prejudice: Ingroup love and outgroup hate? Journal of Social Issues, 55(3), 429444.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brison, N., Stinglhamber, F., & Caesens, G. (2022). Organizational dehumanization. In Oxford research encyclopedia of psychology. https://doi.org/10.1093/acrefore/9780190236557.013.902CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brudholm, T., & Lang, J. (2021). On hatred and dehumanization. In Kronfeldner, M. (Ed.), The Routledge handbook of dehumanization (pp. 341354). Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bruneau, E., Jacoby, N., Kteily, N., & Saxe, R. (2018). Denying humanity: The distinct neural correlates of blatant dehumanization. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 147(7), 10781093.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Castano, E., & Giner-Sorolla, R. (2006). Not quite human: Infrahumanization in response to collective responsibility for intergroup killing. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 90(5), 804818.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cehajic, S., Brown, R., & Gonzalez, R. (2009). What do I care? Perceived ingroup responsibility and dehumanization as predictors of empathy felt for the victim group. Group Processes and Intergroup Relations, 12(6), 715729.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Demoulin, S., Leyens, J-P., Paladino, M., Rodriguez‐Torres, R., Rodriguez‐Perez, A., & Dovidio, J. (2004). Dimensions of “uniquely” and “non‐uniquely” human emotions. Cognition and Emotion, 18(1), 7196.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Enock, F. E., Flavell, J. C., Tipper, S. P., & Over, H. (2021). No convincing evidence outgroups are denied uniquely human characteristics: Distinguishing intergroup preference from trait-based dehumanization. Cognition, 212, Article 104682.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goff, P., Eberhardt, J., Williams, M., & Jackson, M. (2008). Not yet human: Implicit knowledge, historical dehumanization, and contemporary consequences. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94(2), 292306.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gray, H., Gray, K., & Wegner, D. (2007). Dimensions of mind perception. Science, 315(5812), Article 619.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haagensen, L., & Croes, M. (2012). Thy brother’s keeper? The relationship between social distance and intensity of dehumanization during genocide. Genocide Studies and Prevention: An International Journal, 7(2), Article 7.Google Scholar
Haque, O., & Waytz, A. (2012). Dehumanization in medicine: Causes, solutions, and functions. Perspectives on Psychological Sciences, 7(2), 176186.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harris, L., & Fiske, S. (2006). Dehumanizing the lowest of the low: Neuroimaging responses to extreme out-groups. Psychological Science, 17(10), 847853.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haslam, N. (2006). Dehumanization: An integrative review. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 10(3), 252264.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haslam, N. (2013). What is dehumanization? In Bain, P., Vaes, J., & Leyens, J. P. (Eds.), Humanness and dehumanization (pp. 3448). Psychology Press.Google ScholarPubMed
Haslam, N. (2019). The many roles of dehumanization in genocide. In Newman, L. S. (Ed.), Confronting humanity at its worst: The social psychology of genocide and extreme intergroup violence (pp. 119138). Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haslam, N. (2021). The social psychology of dehumanization. In Kronfeldner, M. (Ed.), The Routledge handbook of dehumanization (pp. 127144). Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haslam, N., Bain, P., Douge, L., Lee, M., & Bastian, B. (2005). More human than you: Attributing humanness to self and others. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89(6), 937950.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haslam, N., Bastian, B., & Bissett, M. (2004). Essentialist beliefs about personality and their implications. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30(12), 16611673.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haslam, N., & Loughnan, S. (2014). Dehumanization and infrahumanization. Annual Review of Psychology, 65, 399423.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haslam, N., Loughnan, S., & Sun, P. (2011). Beastly: What makes animal metaphors offensive? Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 30(3), 311325.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jack, A. I., Dawson, A. J., & Norr, M. E. (2013). Seeing human: Distinct and overlapping neural signatures associated with two forms of dehumanization. Neuroimage, 79, 313328.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kelman, H. (1976). Violence without restraint: Reflections on the dehumanization of victims and victimizers. In Kren, G. & Rappoport, L. (Eds.), Varieties of psychohistory (pp. 282314). Springer.Google Scholar
Kouchaki, M., Dobson, K. S. H., Waytz, A., & Kteily, N. S. (2018). The link between self-dehumanization and immoral behavior. Psychological Science, 29(8), 12341246.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kronfeldner, M. (2021a). Psychological essentialism and dehumanization. In Kronfeldner, M. (Ed.), The Routledge handbook of dehumanization (pp. 362377). Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kronfeldner, M. (Ed.). (2021b). The Routledge handbook of dehumanization. Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kteily, N., & Bruneau, E. (2017). Backlash: The politics and real-world consequences of minority group dehumanization. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 43(1), 87104.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kteily, N., Bruneau, E., Waytz, A., & Cotterill, S. (2015). The ascent of man: Theoretical and empirical evidence for blatant dehumanization. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 109(5), 901931.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kteily, N., Hodson, G., & Bruneau, E. (2016). They see us as less than human: Metadehumanization predicts intergroup conflict via reciprocal dehumanization. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 110(3), 343370.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kteily, N. S., & Landry, A. P. (2022). Dehumanization: Trends, insights, and challenges. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 26(3), 222240.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Landry, A. P., Ihm, E., & Schooler, J. W. (2021). Hated but still human: Meta-dehumanization leads to greater hostility than metaprejudice. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 25(2), 315334.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lang, J. (2010). Questioning dehumanization: Intersubjective dimensions of violence in the Nazi concentration and death camps. Holocaust and Genocide Studies, 24(2), 225246.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lang, J. (2020). The limited importance of dehumanization in collective violence. Current Opinion in Psychology, 35, 1720.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leyens, J-P., Demoulin, S., Vaes, J., Gaunt, R., & Paladino, M. (2007). Infra-humanization: The wall of group differences. Social Issues and Policy Review, 1(1), 139172.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leyens, J-P., Rodriguez-Torres, R., Rodríguez-Pérez, A., Gaunt, R., Paladino, M, Vaes, J., & Demoulin, S. (2001). Psychological essentialism and the attribution of uniquely human emotions to ingroups and outgroups. European Journal of Social Psychology, 81(4), 395411.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Loughnan, S., & Haslam, N. (2007). Animals and androids: Implicit associations between social categories and nonhumans. Psychological Science, 18(2), 116121.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Loughnan, S., Haslam, N., Murnane, T., Vaes, J., Reynolds, C., & Suitner, C. (2010). Objectification leads to depersonalization: The denial of mind and moral concern to objectified others. European Journal of Social Psychology, 40(5), 709717.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Malle, B. F. (2019). How many dimensions of mind perception really are there? In Goel, A. K., Seifert, C. M., & Freksa, C. (Eds.), Proceedings of the 41st Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 22682274). Cognitive Science Society.Google Scholar
Manne, K. (2016). Humanism: A critique. Social Theory and Practice, 42(2), 389415.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maoz, I., & McCauley, C. (2008). Threat, dehumanization, and support for retaliatory aggressive policies in asymmetric conflict. Journal of Conflict Resolution, 52(1), 93116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Opotow, S. (1990). Moral exclusion and injustice: An introduction. Journal of Social Issues, 46(1), 173182.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Over, H. (2021). Seven challenges for the dehumanization hypothesis. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 16(1), 313.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pavetich, M., & Stathi, S. (2021). Meta-humanization reduces prejudice, even under high intergroup threat. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 20(3), 651671.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pizzirani, B., Karantzas, G. C., Roisman, G. I., & Simpson, J. A. (2021). Early childhood antecedents of dehumanization perpetration in adult romantic relationships. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 12(7), 11751183.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, D. L. (2011). Less than human: Why we demean, enslave, and exterminate others. St Martin’s Press.Google Scholar
Smith, D. L. (2020). On inhumanity: Dehumanization and how to resist it. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, D. L. (2021). Making monsters: The uncanny power of dehumanization. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Stanton, G. H. (1998). The 8 stages of genocide. Genocide Watch, 1. https://www.genocidewatch.com/tenstagesGoogle Scholar
Staub, E. (1989). The roots of evil: The origins of genocide and other group violence. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Tam, T., Hewstone, M., Cairns, E., Tausch, N., Maio, G., & Kenworthy, J. (2007). The impact of intergroup emotions on forgiveness in Northern Ireland. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 10(1), 119136.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vaes, J., Paladino, M., & Puvia, E. (2011). Are sexualized females complete human beings? Why males and females dehumanize sexually objectified women. European Journal of Social Psychology, 41(6), 774785.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Waytz, A., Gray, K., Epley, N., & Wegner, D. (2010). Causes and consequences of mind perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14(8), 383388.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weisman, K., Dweck, C. S., & Markman, E. M. (2017). Rethinking people’s conceptions of mental life. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 114(43), 1137411379.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

References

Alicke, M. D. (2000). Culpable control and the psychology of blame. Psychological Bulletin, 126(4), 556574.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alicke, M. D. (2008). Blaming badly. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 8(1–2), 179186.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Alicke, M. D., & Zell, E. (2009). Social attractiveness and blame. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 39(9), 20892105.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Allen, D. S. (2002). The world of Prometheus: The politics of punishing in democratic Athens. Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Amemiya, J., Mortenson, E., & Wang, M.-T. (2020). Minor infractions are not minor: School infractions for minor misconduct may increase adolescents’ defiant behavior and contribute to racial disparities in school discipline. American Psychologist, 75(1), 2336.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ames, D. L., & Fiske, S. T. (2013). Intentional harms are worse, even when they’re not. Psychological Science, 24(9), 17551762.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Andrighetto, G., Brandts, J., Conte, R., Sabater-Mir, J., Solaz, H., & Villatoro, D. (2013). Punish and voice: Punishment enhances cooperation when combined with norm-signalling. PLoS ONE, 8(6), Article e64941.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ask, K., & Pina, A. (2011). On being angry and punitive: How anger alters perception of criminal intent. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 2(5), 494499.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Atkinson, Q. D., Gray, R. D., & Drummond, A. J. (2008). MtDNA variation predicts population size in humans and reveals a major Southern Asian chapter in human prehistory. Molecular Biology and Evolution, 25(2), 468474.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Aurenche, O., Kozłowski, J. K., & Kozłowski, S. K. (2013). To be or not to be… Neolithic: “Failed attempts” at Neolithization in Central and Eastern Europe and in the Near East, and their final success (35,000–7000 BP). Paléorient, 39(2), 545.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Balafoutas, L., & Nikiforakis, N. (2012). Norm enforcement in the city: A natural field experiment. European Economic Review, 56(8), 17731785.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bandy, M. S. (2004). Fissioning, scalar stress, and social evolution in early village societies. American Anthropologist, 106(2), 322333.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baumard, N. (2010). Has punishment played a role in the evolution of cooperation? A critical review. Mind & Society, 9(2), 171192.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baumard, N. (2011). Punishment is not a group adaptation. Mind & Society, 10(1), 126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bell, M. (2012). The standing to blame: A critique. In Coates, D. J. & Tognazzini, N. A. (Eds.), Blame: Its nature and norms (pp. 263281). Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bennett, C. (2002). The varieties of retributive experience. The Philosophical Quarterly, 52(207), 145163.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berger, J., & Hevenstone, D. (2016). Norm enforcement in the city revisited: An international field experiment of altruistic punishment, norm maintenance, and broken windows. Rationality and Society, 28(3), 299319.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bernhard, H., Fischbacher, U., & Fehr, E. (2006). Parochial altruism in humans. Nature, 442(7105), Article 7105.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Binder, G. (2002). Punishment theory: Moral or political? Buffalo Criminal Law Review, 5(2), 321372.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Block, B. P. (1991). Just convictions or just convictions? Issues in Criminological & Legal Psychology, 1(17), 2024.Google Scholar
Boehm, C. (1999). Hierarchy in the forest: The evolution of egalitarian behavior. Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boehm, C. (2000). Conflict and the evolution of social control. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 7(1–2), 79101.Google Scholar
Boehm, C. (2011). Retaliatory violence in human prehistory. The British Journal of Criminology, 51(3), 518534.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boserup, E. (1965). The conditions of agricultural growth: The economics of agrarian change under population pressure. Aldine.Google Scholar
Bozan, J., Shao, H., & Hu, H. (2001). A concise history of China. University Press of the Pacific.Google Scholar
Braithwaite, J. (1999). Restorative justice: Assessing optimistic and pessimistic accounts. Crime and Justice, 25, 1127.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, E. (2013, March 25). Is Adria Richards a bully, or was she bullied by the internet? ZDNet. https://www.zdnet.com/article/is-adria-richards-a-bully-or-was-she-bullied-by-the-internet/Google Scholar
Bucciarelli, M., Khemlani, S., & Johnson-Laird, P. N. (2008). The psychology of moral reasoning. Judgment and Decision Making, 3(2), 121139.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Buckholtz, J. W., Martin, J. W., Treadway, M. T., Jan, K., Zald, D. H., Jones, O., & Marois, R. (2015). From blame to punishment: Disrupting prefrontal cortex activity reveals norm enforcement mechanisms. Neuron, 87(6), 13691380.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buikstra, J. E., Konigsberg, L. W., & Bullington, J. (1986). Fertility and the development of agriculture in the prehistoric Midwest. American Antiquity, 51(3), 528546.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carlsmith, K. M., Darley, J. M., & Robinson, P. H. (2002). Why do we punish?: Deterrence and just deserts as motives for punishment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83(2), 284299.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carneiro, R. L. (1970). A theory of the origin of the state. Science, 169(3947), 733738.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chavez, A. K., & Bicchieri, C. (2013). Third-party sanctioning and compensation behavior: Findings from the ultimatum game. Journal of Economic Psychology, 39, 268277.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ciurria, M. (2019). An intersectional feminist theory of moral responsibility. Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Claessen, H. J. M. (2003). Aspects of law and order in Early State societies. In Feldbrugge, F. J. M. (Ed.), The law’s beginnings (pp. 161179). Martinus Nijhoff Publishers.Google Scholar
Clark, C. J., & Winegard, B. M. (2019). Optimism in unconscious, intuitive morality. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 42, Article e150.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Coates, D. J., & Tognazzini, N. A. (2012). The contours of blame. In Coates, D. J. & Tognazzini, N. A. (Eds.), Blame: Its nature and norms (pp. 326). Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cohen, D., & Nisbett, R. E. (1994). Self-protection and the culture of honor: Explaining Southern violence. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 20(5), 551567.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cottingham, J. (1979). Varieties of retribution. The Philosophical Quarterly, 29(116), 238246.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crockett, M. J. (2017). Moral outrage in the digital age. Nature Human Behaviour, 1(11), 769771.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cusimano, C., Thapa, S., & Malle, B. F. (2017). Judgment before emotion: People access moral evaluations faster than affective states. In Gunzelmann, G., Howes, A., Tenbrink, T., & Davelaar, E. J. (Eds.), Proceedings of the 39th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 18481853). Cognitive Science Society.Google Scholar
Cullen, F. T., Jonson, C. L., & Nagin, D. S. (2011). Prisons do not reduce recidivism: The high cost of ignoring science. The Prison Journal, 91(3, Suppl), 48S65S.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cushman, F. (2008). Crime and punishment: Distinguishing the roles of causal and intentional analyses in moral judgment. Cognition, 108(2), 353380.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cushman, F. (2015). Punishment in humans: From intuitions to institutions. Philosophy Compass, 10(2), 117133.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Darling-Hammond, S., Ruiz, M., Eberhardt, J. L., & Okonofua, J. A. (2023). The dynamic nature of student discipline and discipline disparities. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 120(17), 110.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Decety, J., & Cacioppo, S. (2012). The speed of morality: A high-density electrical neuroimaging study. Journal of Neurophysiology, 108(11), 30683072.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
De Freitas, J., & Alvarez, G. A. (2018). Your visual system provides all the information you need to make moral judgments about generic visual events. Cognition, 178, 133146.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dersley, I., & Wootton, A. (2000). Complaint sequences within antagonistic argument. Research on Language and Social Interaction, 33(4), 375406.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dhaliwal, N. A., Patil, I., & Cushman, F. (2021). Reputational and cooperative benefits of third-party compensation. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 164, 2751.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dickey, R. (2019, May 28). It’s not that people deny the existence (although some do) it’s just that it’s ridiculous to expect people to keep track of them [Comment]. Quora.com. https://qr.ae/pGBil5Google Scholar
Ditto, P. H. (2009). Passion, reason, and necessity: A quantity-of-processing view of motivated reasoning. In Bayne, T. & Fernández, J. (Eds.), Delusion and self-deception: Affective and motivational influences on belief formation (pp. 2353). Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Drew, P. (1998). Complaints about transgressions and misconduct. Research on Language & Social Interaction, 31(3–4), 295325.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dubreuil, B. (2010). Human evolution and the origins of hierarchies: The state of nature. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dugar, S. (2010). Nonmonetary sanctions and rewards in an experimental coordination game. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 73(3), 377386.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dukes, K. N., & Gaither, S. E. (2017). Black racial stereotypes and victim blaming: Implications for media coverage and criminal proceedings in cases of police violence against racial and ethnic minorities. Journal of Social Issues, 73(4), 789807.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dumond, D. E. (1972). Population growth and political centralization. In Spooner, B. (Ed.), Population growth: Anthropological implications (pp. 286310). MIT Press.Google Scholar
Dunbar, R. I. M. (1996). Grooming, gossip, and the evolution of language. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Eberhardt, J. L., Davies, P. G., Purdie-Vaughns, V. J., & Johnson, S. L. (2006). Looking deathworthy: Perceived stereotypicality of black defendants predicts capital-sentencing outcomes. Psychological Science, 17(5), 383386.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ellsworth, P. C., & Scherer, K. R. (2003). Appraisal processes in emotion. In Davidson, R. J., Scherer, K. R., & Goldsmith, H. H. (Eds.), Handbook of affective sciences (pp. 572595). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Enloe, J. G. (2003). Food sharing past and present: Archaeological evidence for economic and social interactions. Before Farming: The Archaeology and Anthropology of Hunter-Gatherers, 2003(1), 123.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Equal Justice Initiative. (2017). Lynching in America: Confronting the legacy of racial terror. https://eji.org/wp-content/uploads/2019/10/lynching-in-america-3d-ed-080219.pdfGoogle Scholar
Eriksson, K., Andersson, P. A., & Strimling, P. (2017). When is it appropriate to reprimand a norm violation? The roles of anger, behavioral consequences, violation severity, and social distance. Judgment and Decision Making, 12(4), 396407.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Farrington, K. (1996). Dark justice: A history of punishment and torture. Smithmark.Google Scholar
Fehr, E., & Gächter, S. (2000). Cooperation and punishment in public goods experiments. The American Economic Review, 90(4), 980994.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Feinberg, M., Cheng, J. T., & Willer, R. (2012). Gossip as an effective and low-cost form of punishment. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 35(1), 2525.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Feinberg, M., Willer, R., & Schultz, M. (2014). Gossip and ostracism promote cooperation in groups. Psychological Science, 25(3), 656664.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fincham, F. D., Beach, S., & Nelson, G. (1987). Attribution processes in distressed and nondistressed couples: III. Causal and responsibility attributions for spouse behavior. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 11(1), 7186.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Flannery, K. V., & Marcus, J. (2012). The creation of inequality: How our prehistoric ancestors set the stage for monarchy, slavery, and empire. Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Forbes, R. C., & Stellar, J. E. (2022). When the ones we love misbehave: Exploring moral processes within intimate bonds. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 122(1), 1633.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Friedman, M. (2013). How to blame people responsibly. Journal of Value Inquiry, 47(3), 271284.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Funk, F., McGeer, V., & Gollwitzer, M. (2014). Get the message: Punishment is satisfying if the transgressor responds to its communicative intent. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 40(8), 986997.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gamblin, B. W., Kehn, A., Vanderzanden, K., Ruthig, J. C., Jones, K. M., & Long, B. L. (2021). A comparison of juror decision making in race-based and sexual orientation–based hate crime cases. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 36(7–8), 32313256.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gamez-Djokic, M., & Molden, D. (2016). Beyond affective influences on deontological moral judgment: The role of motivations for prevention in the moral condemnation of harm. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 42(11), 15221537.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garland, D. (1990). Punishment and modern society: A study in social theory. University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gawronski, B., Conway, P., Armstrong, J., Friesdorf, R., & Hütter, M. (2018). Effects of incidental emotions on moral dilemma judgments: An analysis using the CNI model. Emotion, 18(7), 9891008.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gershoff, E. T., & Lee, S. J. (Eds.). (2020). Ending the physical punishment of children: A guide for clinicians and practitioners. American Psychological Association.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gignoux, C. R., Henn, B. M., & Mountain, J. L. (2011). Rapid, global demographic expansions after the origins of agriculture. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(15), 60446049.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goodwin, G. P., & Gromet, D. M. (2014). Punishment. Cognitive Science, 5(5), 561572.Google ScholarPubMed
Gouldner, A. W. (1960). The norm of reciprocity: A preliminary statement. American Sociological Review, 25(2), 161178.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gray, K., Schein, C., & Ward, A. F. (2014). The myth of harmless wrongs in moral cognition: Automatic dyadic completion from sin to suffering. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143(4), 16001615.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Griffith, A. (2020). God’s law and order: The politics of punishment in evangelical America. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Guala, F. (2012). Reciprocity: Weak or strong? What punishment experiments do (and do not) demonstrate. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 35(1), 115.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Guglielmo, S. (2015). Moral judgment as information processing: An integrative review. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, Article 1637.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Guglielmo, S., & Malle, B. F. (2017). Information-acquisition processes in moral judgments of blame. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 43(7), 957971.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haidt, J. (2001). The emotional dog and its rational tail: A social intuitionist approach to moral judgment. Psychological Review, 108(4), 814834.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haidt, J., & Bjorklund, F. (2008). Social intuitionists answer six questions about moral psychology. In Moral psychology: Vol 2. The cognitive science of morality: Intuition and diversity (pp. 181217). MIT Press.Google Scholar
Haidt, J., Bjorklund, F., & Murphy, S. (2000). Moral dumbfounding: When intuition finds no reason [Unpublished manuscript]. Available at https://pdfs.semanticscholar.org/d415/e7fa2c2df922dac194441516a509ba5eb7ec.pdf]. University of Virginia.Google Scholar
Heffner, J., & FeldmanHall, O. (2019). Why we don’t always punish: Preferences for non-punitive responses to moral violations. Scientific Reports, 9(1), Article 13219.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Henn, B. M., Gignoux, C. R., Jobin, M., Granka, J. M., Macpherson, J. M., Kidd, J. M., Rodríguez-Botigué, L., Ramachandran, S., Hon, L., Brisbin, A., Lin, A. A., Underhill, P. A., Comas, D., Kidd, K. K., Norman, P. J., Parham, P., Bustamante, C. D., Mountain, J. L., & Feldman, M. W. (2011). Hunter-gatherer genomic diversity suggests a southern African origin for modern humans. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(13), 51545162.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Herrmann, B., Thöni, C., & Gächter, S. (2008). Antisocial punishment across societies. Science, 319(5868), 13621367.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hershcovis, M. S., & Bhatnagar, N. (2017). When fellow customers behave badly: Witness reactions to employee mistreatment by customers. Journal of Applied Psychology, 102(11), 15281544.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hopfensitz, A., & Reuben, E. (2009). The importance of emotions for the effectiveness of social punishment. The Economic Journal, 119(540), 15341559.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kansal, T. (2005). Racial disparity in sentencing: A review of the literature. The Sentencing Project. https://www.opensocietyfoundations.org/publications/racial-disparity-sentencingGoogle Scholar
Kelly, R. C. (2000). Warless societies and the origin of war. University of Michigan Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kennedy, J. L. D., Tuliao, A. P., Flower, K. N., Tibbs, J. J., & McChargue, D. E. (2019). Long-term effectiveness of a brief restorative justice intervention. International Journal of Offender Therapy and Comparative Criminology, 63(1), 317.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Klonick, K. (2015). Re-shaming the debate: Social norms, shame, and regulation in an internet age. Maryland Law Review, 75(4), 10291065.Google Scholar
Knauft, B. M. (1994). Culture and cooperation in human evolution. In Sponsel, L. & Gregor, T. (Eds.), The anthropology of peace and nonviolence (pp. 3767). Lynne Rienner.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kneer, M., & Machery, E. (2019). No luck for moral luck. Cognition, 182, 331348.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kochanska, G., & Aksan, N. (2006). Children’s conscience and self-regulation. Journal of Personality, 74(6), 15871618.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kriss, P. H., Weber, R. A., & Xiao, E. (2016). Turning a blind eye, but not the other cheek: On the robustness of costly punishment. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 128, 159177.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuo, S.-Y., Longmire, D., & Cuvelier, S. J. (2010). An empirical assessment of the process of restorative justice. Journal of Criminal Justice, 38(3), 318328.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Laforest, M. (2002). Scenes of family life: Complaining in everyday conversation. Journal of Pragmatics, 34(10–11), 15951620.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Larsen, C. S. (1995). Biological changes in human populations with agriculture. Annual Review of Anthropology, 24(1), 185213.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lee, R. B. (1972). Population growth and the beginnings of sedentary life among the !Kung bushmen. In Spooner, B. (Ed.), Population growth: Anthropological implications (pp. 329342). MIT Press.Google Scholar
Lee, R. B. (1979). The !Kung San: Men, women, and work in a foraging society. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Lee, R. B., & Daly, R. H. (1999). The Cambridge encyclopedia of hunters and gatherers. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Lerner, J. S., Goldberg, J. H., & Tetlock, P. E. (1998). Sober second thought: The effects of accountability, anger, and authoritarianism on attributions of responsibility. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24(6), 563574.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MacManus, D., Dean, K., Al Bakir, M., Iversen, A. C., Hull, L., Fahy, T., Wessely, S., & Fear, N. T. (2012). Violent behaviour in UK military personnel returning home after deployment. Psychological Medicine, 42(8), 16631673.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Malle, B. F. (2021). Moral judgments. Annual Review of Psychology, 72, 293318.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Malle, B. F., Guglielmo, S., & Monroe, A. E. (2014). A theory of blame. Psychological Inquiry, 25(2), 147186.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Malle, B. F., Guglielmo, S., Voiklis, J., & Monroe, A. E. (2022). Cognitive blame is socially shaped. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 31(2), 169176.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Márquez-Reiter, R., & Haugh, M. (2019). Denunciation, blame and the moral turn in public life. Discourse, Context & Media, 28, 3543.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Masclet, D., Noussair, C., Tucker, S., & Villeval, M.-C. (2003). Monetary and nonmonetary punishment in the voluntary contributions mechanism. American Economic Review, 93(1), 366381.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mazzocco, P. J., Alicke, M. D., & Davis, T. L. (2004). On the robustness of outcome bias: No constraint by prior culpability. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 26(2–3), 131146.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McGeer, V. (2012). Civilizing blame. In Coates, D. J. & Tognazzini, N. A. (Eds.), Blame: Its nature and norms (pp. 162188). Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McHugh, C., McGann, M., Igou, E. R., & Kinsella, E. L. (2017). Searching for moral dumbfounding: Identifying measurable indicators of moral dumbfounding. Collabra: Psychology, 3(1, Art. 23), 124.Google Scholar
McHugh, C., McGann, M., Igou, E. R., & Kinsella, E. L. (2020). Reasons or rationalizations: The role of principles in the moral dumbfounding paradigm. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 33(3), 376392.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Messerschmidt, M. (2005). Die Wehrmachtjustiz 1933–1945. Schöningh.Google Scholar
Miller, G. P. (2003). Norm enforcement in the public sphere: The case of handicapped parking. George Washington Law Review, 71, 895933.Google Scholar
Milner, G. R., Anderson, E., & Smith, V. G. (1991). Warfare in Late Prehistoric West-Central Illinois. American Antiquity, 56(4), 581603.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mirabito, L. A., & Lecci, L. (2021). The impact of anti-gay bias on verdicts and sentencing with gay defendants. Journal of Gay & Lesbian Social Services: The Quarterly Journal of Community & Clinical Practice, 33(1), 3255.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mitchell, T. L., Haw, R. M., Pfeifer, J. E., & Meissner, C. A. (2005). Racial bias in mock juror decision-making: A meta-analytic review of defendant treatment. Law and Human Behavior, 29(6), 621637.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Molho, C., Tybur, J. M., Van Lange, P. A. M., & Balliet, D. (2020). Direct and indirect punishment of norm violations in daily life. Nature Communications, 11(1), Article 1.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Monin, B., Pizarro, D. A., & Beer, J. S. (2007). Deciding versus reacting: Conceptions of moral judgment and the reason-affect debate. Review of General Psychology, 11(2), 99111.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Monroe, A. E., & Malle, B. F. (2017). Two paths to blame: Intentionality directs moral information processing along two distinct tracks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 146(1), 123133.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Monroe, A. E., & Malle, B. F. (2019). People systematically update moral judgments of blame. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 116(2), 215236.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mooijman, M., & Graham, J. (2018). Unjust punishment in organizations. Research in Organizational Behavior, 38, 95106.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moore, B. (2001). Cruel and unusual punishment in the Roman Empire and dynastic China. International Journal of Politics, Culture, and Society, 14(4), 729772.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morris, G. H. (1988). Finding fault. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 7(1), 125.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Muhammad, K. G., Gottschalk, M., & Thompson, H. A. (2015). The underlying causes of rising incarceration: Crime, politics, and social change. In Travis, J., Western, B., & Redburn, S. (Eds.), The growth of incarceration in the United States: Exploring causes and consequences (pp. 104129). National Academies Press.Google Scholar
Nadelhoffer, T., Heshmati, S., Kaplan, D., & Nichols, S. (2013). Folk retributivism and the communication confound. Economics and Philosophy, 29(2), 235261.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nadler, J. (2012). Blaming as a social process: The influence of character and moral emotion on blame. Law and Contemporary Problems, 75(2), 131.Google Scholar
Nadler, J., & McDonnell, M.-H. (2012). Moral character, motive, and the psychology of blame. Cornell Law Review, 97, 255304.Google Scholar
Nemeth, C., & Sosis, R. H. (1973). A simulated jury study: Characteristics of the defendant and the jurors. The Journal of Social Psychology, 90(2), 221229.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Neumann, R., Seibt, B., & Strack, F. (2001). The influence of mood on the intensity of emotional responses: Disentangling feeling and knowing. Cognition and Emotion, 15(6), 725747.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Niemi, L., & Young, L. (2016). When and why we see victims as responsible: The impact of ideology on attitudes toward victims. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 42(9), 12271242.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Okonofua, J. A., & Eberhardt, J. L. (2015). Two strikes: Race and the disciplining of young students. Psychological Science, 26(5), 617624.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pedersen, E. J., McAuliffe, W. H. B., Shah, Y., Tanaka, H., Ohtsubo, Y., & McCullough, M. E. (2020). When and why do third parties punish outside of the lab? A cross-cultural recall study. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 11(6), 846853.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peregrine, P. N., Ember, C. R., & Ember, M. (2007). Modeling state origins using cross-cultural data. Cross-Cultural Research, 41(1), 7586.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pesta, R. (2022). School punishment, deterrence, and race: A partial test of defiance theory. Crime & Delinquency, 68(3), 463494.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pinker, S. (2011). The better angels of our nature: Why violence has declined. Viking.Google Scholar
Pratt, J., Brown, D., Brown, M., Wallsworth, S., & Morrison, W. (Eds.). (2005). The new punitiveness: Trends, theories, perspectives. Willan.Google Scholar
Princing, M. (2019, September 3). What microaggressions are and how to prevent them. Right as Rain by UW Medicine. https://rightasrain.uwmedicine.org/life/relationships/microaggressionsGoogle Scholar
Przepiorka, W., & Berger, J. (2016). The sanctioning dilemma: A quasi-experiment on social norm enforcement in the train. European Sociological Review, 32(3), 439451.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Quigley, B. M., & Tedeschi, J. T. (1996). Mediating effects of blame attributions on feelings of anger. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22(12), 12801288.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rand, D. G., Armao, J. J. IV, Nakamaru, M., & Ohtsuki, H. (2010). Anti-social punishment can prevent the co-evolution of punishment and cooperation. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 265(4), 624632.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Redford, L., & Ratliff, K. A. (2016). Hierarchy-legitimizing ideologies reduce behavioral obligations and blame for implicit attitudes and resulting discrimination. Social Justice Research, 29(2), 159185.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Redford, L., & Ratliff, K. A. (2018). Pride and punishment: Entitled people’s self‐promoting values motivate hierarchy‐restoring retribution. European Journal of Social Psychology, 48(3), 303319.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Redman, C. L. (1978). The rise of civilization: From early farmers to urban society in the ancient Near East. W. H. Freeman.Google Scholar
Republican National Committee. (2020). Resolution upholding the first amendment to the constitution of the United States of America in the response to the coronavirus pandemic and the cancel culture movement. https://bit.ly/4gpQLVsGoogle Scholar
Robinson, P. H., & Darley, J. M. (1995). Justice, liability, and blame: Community views and the criminal law. Westview Press.Google Scholar
Royzman, E. B., Kim, K., & Leeman, R. F. (2015). The curious tale of Julie and Mark: Unraveling the moral dumbfounding effect. Judgment and Decision Making, 10(4), 296313.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sasse, J., Halmburger, A., & Baumert, A. (2020). The functions of anger in moral courage—Insights from a behavioral study. Emotion, 22(6), 13211335.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scherer, K. R. (2013). The nature and dynamics of relevance and valence appraisals: Theoretical advances and recent evidence. Emotion Review, 5(2), 150162.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Seidel, A., & Prinz, J. (2013). Mad and glad: Musically induced emotions have divergent impact on morals. Motivation and Emotion, 37(3), 629637.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Service, E. R. (1966). The hunters. Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
Shoemaker, D. (2012). Blame and punishment. In Coates, D. J. & Tognazzini, N. A. (Eds.), Blame: Its nature and norms (pp. 100118). Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shoemaker, D., & Vargas, M. (2021). Moral torch fishing: A signaling theory of blame. Noûs, 55(3), 581602.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sidanius, J., Mitchell, M., Haley, H., & Navarrete, C. D. (2006). Support for harsh criminal sanctions and criminal justice beliefs: A social dominance perspective. Social Justice Research, 19(4), 433449.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sidanius, J., & Pratto, F. (1999). Social dominance: An intergroup theory of social hierarchy and oppression. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Silberbauer, G. (1982). Political process in G/wi bands. In Leacock, E. & Lee, R. (Eds.), Politics and history in band societies (pp. 2336). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Sood, A. M. (2019). Attempted justice: Misunderstanding and bias in psychological constructions of critical attempt. Stanford Law Review, 71(3), 593686.Google Scholar
Sood, A. M., & Darley, J. M. (2012). The plasticity of harm in the service of criminalization goals. California Law Review, 100(5), 13131358.Google Scholar
Sorial, S. (2016). Performing anger to signal injustice: The expression of anger in victim impact statements. In Abell, C., Smith, J., Abell, C., & Smith, J. (Eds.), The expression of emotion: Philosophical, psychological and legal perspectives (pp. 287310). Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stanley, M. L., Yin, S., & Sinnott-Armstrong, W. (2019). A reason-based explanation for moral dumbfounding. Judgment and Decision Making, 14(2), 120129.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stewart, J. E. (1985). Appearance and punishment: The attraction-leniency effect in the courtroom. The Journal of Social Psychology, 125(3), 373378.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Strohminger, N. (2017). Four unwarranted assumptions about the role of emotion in moral judgment. In Price, C. & Walle, E. (Eds.), Emotion Researcher: ISRE’s sourcebook for research on emotion and affect. http://emotionresearcher.com/four-unwarranted-assumptions-about-the-role-of-emotion-in-moral-judgment/Google Scholar
Sutton, J. R. (2013). Structural bias in the sentencing of felony defendants. Social Science Research, 42(5), 12071221.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tata, C. (1997). Conceptions and representations of the sentencing decision process. Journal of Law and Society, 24(3), 395420.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tonry, M. (2009). Explanations of American punishment policies: A national history. Punishment & Society, 11(3), 377394.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Voiklis, J., Kim, B., Cusimano, C., & Malle, B. F. (2016). Moral judgments of human vs. Robot agents. In Proceedings of the 25th IEEE International Symposium on Robot and Human Interactive Communication (RO-MAN) (pp. 486491). IEEE.Google Scholar
Voiklis, J., & Malle, B. F. (2018). Moral cognition and its basis in social cognition and social regulation. In Gray, K. & Graham, J. (Eds.), Atlas of moral psychology (pp. 108120). Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Walker, M. U. (2006). Moral repair: Reconstructing moral relations after wrongdoing. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Watanabe, S., & Laurent, S. M. (2021). Volition speaks louder than action: Offender atonement, forgivability, and victim valuation in the minds of perceivers. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 47(6), 10201036.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weiss, A., & Burgmer, P. (2021). Other-serving double standards: People show moral hypercrisy in close relationships. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 38(11), 31983218.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
White, L. A. (2007). The evolution of culture: The development of civilization to the fall of Rome. Left Coast Press.Google Scholar
Wiessner, P. (2005). Norm enforcement among the Ju/’hoansi Bushmen. Human Nature, 16(2), 115145.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilson, P. J. (1988). The domestication of the human species. Yale University Press.Google Scholar
Woodburn, J. (1982). Egalitarian societies. Man, 17(3), 431451.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wu, J., Balliet, D., & Van Lange, P. A. M. (2016). Gossip versus punishment: The efficiency of reputation to promote and maintain cooperation. Scientific Reports, 6, Article 23919.Google ScholarPubMed
Xiao, E. (2018). Punishment, social norms, and cooperation. In Teitelbaum, J. C. & Zeiler, K. (Eds.), Research handbook on behavioral law and economics (pp. 155173). Edward Elgar Publishing.Google Scholar
Xiao, E., & Houser, D. (2005). Emotion expression in human punishment behavior. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 102(20), 73987401.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yamagishi, T. (1986). The provision of a sanctioning system as a public good. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51(1), 110116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yukhnenko, D., Sridhar, S., & Fazel, S. (2020). A systematic review of criminal recidivism rates worldwide: 3-year update. Wellcome Open Research, 4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zajenkowska, A., Prusik, M., Jasielska, D., & Szulawski, M. (2021). Hostile attribution bias among offenders and non-offenders: Making social information processing more adequate. Journal of Community & Applied Social Psychology, 31(2), 241256.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

References

Baumard., N., André, J.-B., & Sperber, D. (2013). A mutualistic approach to morality: The evolution of fairness by partner choice. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36(1), 5978.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bennett, C. (2008). The apology ritual: A philosophical theory of punishment. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bernhard, R. M., Martin, J. W., & Warneken, F. (2020). Why do children punish? Fair outcomes matter more than intent in children’s second- and third-party punishment. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 200, Article 104909.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berniūnas, R., Dranseika, V., & Sousa, P. (2016). Are there different moral domains? Evidence from Mongolia. Asian Journal of Social Psychology, 19(3), 275282.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boon, S. D., & Yoshimura, S. M. (2020). Revenge as social interaction: Merging social psychological and interpersonal communication approaches to the study of vengeful behavior. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 14(9), Article e12554.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Braithwaite, J. (2002). Restorative justice and responsive regulation. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Braithwaite, J., & Pettit, P. (1990). Not just deserts: A republican theory of criminal justice. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Braun, J., & Gollwitzer, M. (2012). Leniency for out-group offenders. European Journal of Social Psychology, 42(7), 883892.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carlsmith, K. M. (2006). The roles of retribution and utility in determining punishment. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 42(4), 437451.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carlsmith, K. M., Darley, J. M., & Robinson, P. H. (2002). Why do we punish? Deterrence and just deserts as motives for punishment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83(2), 284299.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carlsmith, K. M., Wilson, T. D., & Gilbert, D. T. (2008). The paradoxical consequences of revenge. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95(6), 13161324.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chavez, A. K., & Bicchieri, C. (2013). Third-party sanctioning and compensation behavior: Findings from the ultimatum game. Journal of Economic Psychology, 39, 268277.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Corwin, E. P., Cramer, R. J., Griffin, D. A., & Brodsky, S. L. (2012). Defendant remorse, need for affect, and juror sentencing decisions. Journal of the American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law Online, 40(1), 4149.Google ScholarPubMed
Crockett, M. J., Özdemir, Y., & Fehr, E. (2014). The value of vengeance and the demand for deterrence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143(6), 22792286.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crombag, H., Rassin, E., & Horselenberg, R. (2003). On vengeance. Psychology, Crime & Law, 9(4), 333344.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Duff, R. A. (2001). Punishment, communication and community. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Eadeh, F. R., Peak, S. A., & Lambert, A. J. (2017). The bittersweet taste of revenge: On the negative and positive consequences of retaliation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 68, 2739.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fehr, E., & Fischbacher, U. (2004). Third-party punishment and social norms. Evolution and Human Behavior, 25(2), 6387.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Feinberg, J. (1965). The expressive function of punishment. The Monist, 49(3), 397423.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fischer, M., Twardawski, M., Strelan, P., & Gollwitzer, M. (2022). Victims need more than power: Empowerment and moral change independently predict victims’ satisfaction and willingness to reconcile. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 123(3), 518536.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fitness, J., & Peterson, J. (2008). Punishment and forgiveness in close relationships: An evolutionary, social-psychological perspective. In Forgas, J. P. & Fitness, J. (Eds.), Social relationships: Cognitive, affective, and motivational processes (pp. 255269). Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Fudenberg, D., & Pathak, P. A. (2010). Unobserved punishment supports cooperation. Journal of Public Economics, 94(1–2), 7886.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Funk, F., McGeer, V., & Gollwitzer, M. (2014). Get the message: Punishment is satisfying if the transgressor responds to its communicative intent. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 40(8), 986997.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gergely, G., & Király, I. (2019). Natural pedagogy of social emotions. In Dukes, D. & Clément, F. (Eds.), Foundations of affective social learning: Conceptualizing the social transmission of value (pp. 87114). Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gilbert, M. (1994). Me, you, and us: Distinguishing “egoism,” “altruism,” and “groupism.” Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 17(4), 621622.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gollwitzer, M., Meder, M., & Schmitt, M. (2011). What gives victims satisfaction when they seek revenge? European Journal of Social Psychology, 41(3), 364374.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goodall, J. (1986). The chimpanzees of Gombe: Patterns of behavior. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Goodwin, G. P., & Benforado, A. (2015). Judging the goring ox: Retribution directed toward animals. Cognitive Science, 39(3), 619646.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goodwin, G. P., & Darley, J. M. (2008). The psychology of meta-ethics: Exploring objectivism. Cognition, 106(3), 13391366.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gray, K., Waytz, A., & Young, L. (2012). The moral dyad: A fundamental template unifying moral judgment. Psychological Inquiry, 23(2), 206215.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grice, H. P. (1969). Utterer’s meaning and intentions. The Philosophical Review, 78(2), 147177.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gromet, D. M., & Darley, J. M. (2009). Punishment and beyond: Achieving justice through the satisfaction of multiple goals. Law & Society Review, 43(1), 138.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heffner, J., & FeldmanHall, O. (2019). Why we don’t always punish: Preferences for non-punitive responses to moral violations. Scientific Reports, 9(1), 113.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Henrich, J., McElreath, R., Barr, A., Ensminger, J., Barrett, C., Botyanatz, A., Cardenas, J. C., Gurven, M., Gwako, E., Henrich, N., Lesorogol, C., Marlowe, F., Tracer, D., & Ziker, J. (2006). Costly punishment across human societies. Science, 312, 17671770.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ho, M. K., MacGlashan, J., Littman, M. L., & Cushman, F. (2017). Social is special: A normative framework for teaching with and learning from evaluative feedback. Cognition, 167, 91106.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoehl, S., Keupp, S., Schleihauf, H., McGuigan, N., Buttelmann, D., & Whiten, A. (2019). ‘Over-imitation’: A review and appraisal of a decade of research. Developmental Review, 51, 90108.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Horner, V., & Whiten, A. (2005). Causal knowledge and imitation/emulation switching in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and children (Homo sapiens). Animal Cognition, 8(3), 164181.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hutchins, E. (2014). The cultural ecosystem of human cognition. Philosophical Psychology, 27(1), 3449.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnstone, G. (2013). Restorative justice: Ideas, values, debates. Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jordan, J. J., Hoffman, M., Bloom, P., & Rand, D. G. (2016). Third-party punishment as a costly signal of trustworthiness. Nature, 530(7591), 473476.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jordan, J. J., McAuliffe, K., & Warneken, F. (2014). Development of in-group favoritism in children’s third-party punishment of selfishness. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 111(35), 1271012715.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jordan, J. J., & Rand, D. G. (2020). Signaling when no one is watching: A reputation heuristics account of outrage and punishment in one-shot anonymous interactions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 118(1), 5788.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaminski, J., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2004). Body orientation and face orientation: Two factors controlling apes’ begging behavior from humans. Animal Cognition, 7(4), 216223.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keller, L. B., Oswald, M. E., Stucki, I., & Gollwitzer, M. (2010). A closer look at an eye for an eye: Laypersons’ punishment decisions are primarily driven by retributive motives. Social Justice Research, 23(2–3), 99116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kelly, D., & Stich, S. (2007). Two theories about the cognitive architecture underlying morality. In Carruthers, P., Laurence, S., & Stich, S. (Eds.), The innate mind: Vol. 3. Foundations and the future (pp. 348366). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Kenward, B. (2012). Over-imitating preschoolers believe unnecessary actions are normative and enforce their performance by a third party. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 112(2), 195207.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keupp, S., Bancken, C., Schillmöller, J., Rakoczy, H., & Behne, T. (2016). Rational over-imitation: Preschoolers consider material costs and copy causally irrelevant actions selectively. Cognition, 147, 8592.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keupp, S., Behne, T., & Rakoczy, H. (2013). Why do children overimitate? Normativity is crucial. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 116(2), 392406.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Király, I., Csibra, G., & Gergely, G. (2013). Beyond rational imitation: Learning arbitrary means actions from communicative demonstrations. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 116(2), 471486.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Krasnow, M. M., Cosmides, L., Pedersen, E. J., & Tooby, J. (2012). What are punishment and reputation for? PLoS ONE, 7(9), Article e45662.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lacey, N. (1988). State punishment. Routledge.Google Scholar
Lee, Y., & Warneken, F. (2020). Children’s evaluations of third-party responses to unfairness: Children prefer helping over punishment. Cognition, 205, Article 104374.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marques, J. M., Yzerbyt, V. Y., & Leyens, J.-P. (1988). The ‘Black Sheep Effect’: Extremity of judgments towards ingroup members as a function of group identification. European Journal of Social Psychology, 18(1), 116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McAuliffe, K., Jordan, J. J., & Warneken, F. (2015). Costly third-party punishment in young children. Cognition, 134, 110.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McGeer, V. (2008). Varieties of moral agency: Lessons from autism (and psychopathy). In Sinnott-Armstrong, W. (Ed.), Moral psychology: Vol. 3. The neuroscience of morality: Emotion, brain disorders, and development (pp. 227257). MIT Press.Google Scholar
McGeer, V. (2013). Civilizing blame. In Coates, J. D. & Tognazzini, N. A. (Eds.), Blame: Its nature and norms (pp. 162188). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
McGeer, V. (2020). Enculturating folk psychologists. Synthese, 199(1–2), 10391063.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McGeer, V., & Funk, F. (2017). Are ‘optimistic’ theories of criminal justice psychologically feasible? The probative case of civic republicanism. Criminal Law and Philosophy, 11(3), 523544.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Molnar, A., Chaudhry, S., & Loewenstein, G. F. (2023). “It’s not about the money. It’s about sending a message!” Avengers want offenders to understand the reason for revenge. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 174, Article 104207.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moore, R. (2016). Meaning and ostension in great ape gestural communication. Animal Cognition, 19(1), 223231.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nadelhoffer, T., Heshmati, S., Kaplan, D., & Nichols, S. (2013). Folk retributivism and the communication confound. Economics and Philosophy, 29(2), 235261.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nahmias, E., & Aharoni, E. (2018). Communicative theories of punishment and the impact of apology. In Surprenant, C. W. (Ed.), Rethinking punishment in the era of mass incarceration (pp. 144161). Routledge.Google Scholar
Nucci, L. P. (2001). Education in the moral domain. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nucci, L. P., & Nucci, M. S. (1982). Children’s social interactions in the context of moral and conventional transgressions. Child Development, 53(2), 403412.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Okimoto, T. G., & Wenzel, M. (2008). The symbolic meaning of transgressions: Towards a unifying framework of justice restoration. Advances in Group Processes, 25, 291326.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Piazza, J., Sousa, P., Rottman, J., & Syropoulos, S. (2019). Which appraisals are foundational to moral judgment? Harm, injustice, and beyond. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 10(7), 903913.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Raihani, N. J., & Bshary, R. (2019). Punishment: One tool, many uses. Evolutionary Human Sciences, 1, Article e12.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raihani, N. J., Thornton, A., & Bshary, R. (2012). Punishment and cooperation in nature. Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 27(5), 288295.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rakoczy, H., & Tomasello, M. (2007). The ontogeny of social ontology: Steps to shared intentionality and status functions. In Tsohatzidis, S. L. (Ed.), Intentional acts and institutional facts: Essays on John Searle’s social ontology (pp. 113137). Springer Netherlands.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Riedl, K., Jensen, K., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2015). Restorative justice in children. Current Biology, 25(13), 17311735.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rossner, M., & Bruce, J. (2016). Community participation in restorative justice: Rituals, reintegration, and quasi-professionalization. Victims & Offenders, 11(1), 107125.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Salali, G. D., Juda, M., & Henrich, J. (2015). Transmission and development of costly punishment in children. Evolution and Human Behavior, 36(2), 8694.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sarin, A., Ho, M., Martin, J., & Cushman, F. A. (2021). Punishment is organized around principles of communicative inference. Cognition, 208, Article 104544.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shapland, J., Atkinson, A., Atkinson, H., Colledge, E., Dignan, J., Howes, M., Johnstone, J., Robinson, G., & Sorsby, A. (2006). Situating restorative justice within criminal justice. Theoretical Criminology, 10(4), 505532.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shnabel, N., & Nadler, A. (2008). A needs-based model of reconciliation: Satisfying the differential emotional needs of victim and perpetrator as a key to promoting reconciliation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94(1), 116132.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shoemaker, D., & Vargas, M. (2019). Moral torch fishing: A signaling theory of blame. Noûs, 55(3), 581602.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shweder, R. A., Much, N. C., Mahapatra, M., & Park, L. (1997). The “big three” of morality (autonomy, community, divinity) and the “big three” explanations of suffering. In Brandt, A. & Rozin, P. (Eds.), Morality and health (pp. 119169). Routledge.Google Scholar
Sjöström, A., & Gollwitzer, M. (2015). Displaced revenge: Can revenge taste “sweet” if it aims at a different target? Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 56, 191202.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Skitka, L. J., Hanson, B. E., Morgan, G. S., & Wisneski, D. C. (2021). The psychology of moral conviction. Annual Review of Psychology, 72, 347366.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smetana, J. G. (1993). Understanding of social rules. In Bennett, M. (Ed.), Development of social cognition: The child as psychologist (pp. 111141). The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Smetana, J. G., & Braeges, J. L. (1990). The development of toddlers’ moral and conventional judgments. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 36(3), 329346.Google Scholar
Sommers, S. R., & Ellsworth, P. C. (2000). Race in the courtroom: Perceptions of guilt and dispositional attributions. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26(11), 13671379.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Southgate, V., Van Maanen, C., & Csibra, G. (2007). Infant pointing: Communication to cooperate or communication to learn? Child Development, 78(3), 735740.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Southwood, N. (2011). The moral/conventional distinction. Mind, 120(479), 761802.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sripada, C., & Stich, S. (2006). A framework for the psychology of norms. In Carruthers, P., Laurence, S., & Stich, S. (Eds.), The innate mind: Culture and cognition (Vol. 2, pp. 280301). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Sterelny, K. (2012). The evolved apprentice. MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Strang, H. (2002). Repair or revenge: Victims and restorative justice. Clarendon Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Strawson, P. F. (1962). Freedom and resentment. Proceedings of the British Academy, 48, 187211.Google Scholar
Sunstein, C. R. (1996). On the expressive function of law. University of Pennsylvania Law Review, 144(5), 20212053.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M., & Call, J. (2019). Thirty years of great ape gestures. Animal Cognition, 22(4), 461469.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., Call, J., Behne, T., & Moll, H. (2005). Understanding and sharing intentions: The origins of cultural cognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28(5), 675691.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., & Liszkowski, U. (2007). A new look at infant pointing. Child Development, 78(3), 705722.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Turiel, E. (1983). The development of social knowledge: Morality and convention. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Twardawski, M., Tang, K. T. Y., & Hilbig, B. E. (2020). Is it all about retribution? The flexibility of punishment goals. Social Justice Research, 33(2), 195218.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van Prooijen, J.-W. (2018). The moral punishment instinct. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Voiklis, J., & Malle, B. F. (2017). Moral cognition and its basis in social cognition and social regulation. In Gray, K. & Graham, J. (Eds.), Atlas of moral psychology (pp. 108120). The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
von Hirsch, A. (1993). Censure and sanctions. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Washburn, A., & Skitka, L. J. (2015). Motivated and displaced revenge: Remembering 9/11 suppresses opposition to military intervention in Syria (for some). Analyses of Social Issues and Public Policy (ASAP), 15(1), 89104.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wenzel, M., & Okimoto, T. G. (2016). Retributive justice. In Sabbagh, C. & Schmitt, M. (Eds.), Handbook of social justice theory and research (pp. 237256). Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wu, Z., & Gao, X. (2018). Preschoolers’ group bias in punishing selfishness in the Ultimatum Game. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 166, 280292.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Xiao, E., & Houser, D. (2005). Emotion expression in human punishment behavior. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 102(20), 73987401.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yamamoto, S., Humle, T., & Tanaka, M. (2012). Chimpanzees’ flexible targeted helping based on an understanding of conspecifics’ goals. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109(9), 35883592.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yang, X., Wu, Z., & Dunham, Y. (2021). Children’s restorative justice in an intergroup context. Social Development, 30(3), 663683.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

  • Behavior
  • Edited by Bertram Malle, Brown University, Rhode Island, Philip Robbins, University of Missouri
  • Book: The Cambridge Handbook of Moral Psychology
  • Online publication: 20 February 2025
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108894357.015
Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

  • Behavior
  • Edited by Bertram Malle, Brown University, Rhode Island, Philip Robbins, University of Missouri
  • Book: The Cambridge Handbook of Moral Psychology
  • Online publication: 20 February 2025
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108894357.015
Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

  • Behavior
  • Edited by Bertram Malle, Brown University, Rhode Island, Philip Robbins, University of Missouri
  • Book: The Cambridge Handbook of Moral Psychology
  • Online publication: 20 February 2025
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108894357.015
Available formats
×