Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-4rdpn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-12T22:18:23.675Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

5 - Irony as Social Work: Opposition, Expectation Violation, and Contrast

from Part III - Irony’s Impact

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  20 December 2023

Herbert L. Colston
Affiliation:
University of Alberta
Get access

Summary

This chapter argues that irony in language emerges as an exquisite form of social work, through the operations of opposition, expectation violation, and contrast. Among all the different and varied figurative forms, irony may be particularly well suited in helping us form a sense-of-self that aligns with other people’s expectations, connect with other people, and manage our positions in social networks and hierarchies. Verbal irony’s oppositionality can lead to an expression of a violation of expectations on a speaker’s part through various methods (e.g., echo, pretense, allusional pretense, salience, contrast). But irony does more by providing speakers with a way to express their attitudes about different situations (e.g., agreeing or disagreeing with some other person’s attitude). Hearing irony may also help people form attitudes about the ironic speakers (e.g., finding the use of sarcasm as funny, clever, boorish). Thinking of irony as social work highlights the utility of this figure in delicately dealing with a wide range of interpersonal circumstances in everyday life.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2023

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Baumeister, R. F., & Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to belong: Desire for interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human motivation. Psychological Bulletin, 117(3), 497.Google Scholar
Colston, H. L. (2015). Using figurative language. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Colston, H. L. (2019). How language makes meaning: Embodiment and conjoined antonymy. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Colston, H. L. (2021). Humor and figurative language: Good for a laugh, and more. In Strick, M. & Ford, T. (Eds.), The social psychology of humor (pp. 92–108). Routledge.Google Scholar
Colston, H. L., & O’Brien, J. (2000a). Contrast and pragmatics in figurative language: Anything understatement can do, irony can do better. Journal of Pragmatics, 32, 15571583.Google Scholar
Colston, H. L., & O’Brien, J. (2000b). Contrast of kind vs. contrast of magnitude: The pragmatic accomplishments of irony and hyperbole. Discourse Processes, 30(2), 179199.Google Scholar
Colston, H. L., & Rasse, C. (2023). Embodiment across Englishes: Comprehension of popular song lyric metaphors in Canadian, Austrian, and American English. In Degani, M. & Cailles, M. (Eds.), Advances in world Englishes. Bloomsbury.Google Scholar
Denny, B. T., Kober, H., Wager, T. D., & Ochsner, K. N. (2012). A meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies of self and other judgments reveals a spatial gradient for mentalizing in medial prefrontal cortex. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 24(8), 17421752.Google Scholar
Dunbar, R. (1998). The social brain hypothesis. Evolutionary Anthropology, 6, 178190.Google Scholar
Eisenberger, N. I. (2012). The pain of social disconnection: Examining the shared neural underpinnings of physical and social pain. Nature Reviews, Neuroscience, 13(6), 421434.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eisenberger, N. I., & Cole, S. W. (2012). Social neuroscience and health: Neuropsychological mechanisms linking social ties with physical health. Nature Neuroscience, 15, 669674.Google Scholar
Eisenberger, N. I., Lieberman, M. D., & Williams, K. D. (2003). Does rejection hurt? An fMRI study of social exclusion. Science, 302, 290292.Google Scholar
Everett, D. L. (2012). Language: The cultural tool. Pantheon Books.Google Scholar
Gibbs, R. W. (2000). Irony in talk among friends. Metaphor and Symbol, 15(1–2), 527.Google Scholar
Kelley, W. M., Macrae, C. N., Wyland, C. L., Caglar, S., Inati, S., & Heatherton, T. F. (2002). Finding the self? An event-related fMRI study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14(5), 785794.Google Scholar
Lieberman, M. D. (2010). Social cognitive neuroscience. In Fiske, S. T., Gilbert, D. T., & Lindsey, G. (Eds.), Handbook of Social Psychology (5th ed., pp. 143193). McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Lieberman, M. D. (2013). Social: Why our brains are wired to connect. Broadway Books.Google Scholar
Lieberman, M. D., & Eisenberger, N. I. (2009). Pains and pleasures of social life. Science, 323, 890891.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McKiernan, K. A., Kaufman, J. N., Kucera-Thompson, J., & Binder, J. R. (2003). A parametric manipulation of factors affecting task-induced deactivation in functional neuroimaging. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 15(3), 394408.Google Scholar
Nowak, M., & Highfield, R. (2012). SuperCooperators: Altruism, evolution, and why we need each other to succeed. Free Press.Google Scholar
Pexman, P., & Olineck, K. M. (2002a). Does sarcasm always sting? Investigating the impact of ironic insults and ironic compliments. Discourse Processes, 33, 199217.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pexman, P., & Olineck, K. M. (2002b). Understanding irony: How to stereotypes cue ironic intent? Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 21, 245274.Google Scholar
Pexman, P. M., & Zvaigzne, M. T. (2004). Does irony go better with friends. Metaphor and Symbol, 19(2), 143163.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pinker, S. (2000). The language instinct: How the mind creates language. Harper Perennial Modern Classics.Google Scholar
Semendeferi, K., Teffer, K., Buxhoeveden, D. P., Park, M. S., Bludau, S., Amunts, K., Travis, K., & Buckwalter, J. (2011). Spatial organization of neurons in the frontal pole sets humans apart from great apes. Cerebral Cortex, 21(7), 14851497.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shulman, G. L., Corbetta, M., Buckner, R. L., Fiez, J. A., Miezin, F. M., Raichle, M. E., & Petersen, S. E. (1997a). Common blood flow changes across visual tasks I: Increases in subcortical structures and cerebellum but not in nonvisual cortex. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 9(5), 624647.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shulman, G. L., Fiez, J. A., Corbetta, M., Buckner, R. L., Miezin, F. M., Raichle, M. E., & Petersen, S. E. (1997b). Common blood flow changes across visual tasks II: Decreases in cerebral cortex. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 9(5), 648663.Google Scholar
Spunt, R. P., & Lieberman, M. D. (2012). Dissociating modality-specific and supramodal neural systems for action understanding. Journal of Neuroscience, 32, 35753583.Google Scholar
Stravinsky, A., & Boyer, R. (2001). Loneliness in relation to suicide ideation and parasuicide: A population-wide study. Suicide and Life-Threatening Behavior, 31(1), 3240.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. (2020). The role of roles in uniquely human cognition and sociality. Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour, 50, 219.Google Scholar
Williams, K. D. (2001). Ostracism: The power of silence. Guilford Press.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×