Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-dh8gc Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-09T21:53:35.890Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

7 - Intelligence in Infancy

from Part II - Development of Intelligence

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  13 December 2019

Robert J. Sternberg
Affiliation:
Cornell University, New York
Get access

Summary

This chapter addresses the compelling questions of whether infants are intelligent and whether infant intelligence predicts future mental development. The chapter first tackles the perennially intransigent challenges of defining infancy and intelligence. The chapter next reviews the history of infancy study from the point of view of what we thought we knew about infant intelligence. The chapter then draws the reader into an intuitive perspective on what might be everyday intelligent behaviors on the part of infants; that perspective is buttressed with scientific investigations. That laboratory work is subsequently elaborated on with reference to new paradigms followed by a focus on two prominent interrelated methods and measures of studying cognition in infants: habituation and novelty preference. Their interpretation as measures of cognition in infancy is supported with evidence from studies of concurrent and predictive validity. The chapter concludes with comments and thoughts about the future promise of a new view on infant intelligence.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2020

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Abelson, R. (1985). A variance explanation paradox: When a little is a lot. Psychological Bulletin, 97, 129133.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ahadi, S., & Diener, E. (1989). Multiple determinants and effect sizes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 398406.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Alexander, K. L., Entwisle, D. R., & Dauber, S. L. (1993). First-grade behavior: Its short-and long-term consequences for school performance. Child Development, 64, 801814.Google Scholar
Amso, D., & Johnson, S. P. (2006). Learning by selection: Visual search and object perception in young infants. Developmental Psychology, 42, 12361245.Google Scholar
Anastasi, A., & Urbina, S. (1997). Psychological testing. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
Arterberry, M. E., & Bornstein, M. H. (2002). Variability and its sources in infant categorization. Infant Behavior and Development, 25, 515528.Google Scholar
Baillargeon, R. (2004). Infants’ physical world. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 13, 8994.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bauer, P. J. (2007). Recall in infancy: A neurodevelopmental account. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16, 142146.Google Scholar
Bayley, N. (1949). Consistency and variability in the growth of intelligence from birth to eighteen yearsThe Pedagogical Seminary and Journal of Genetic Psychology75(2), 165196.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bayley, N. (1955). On the growth of intelligenceAmerican Psychologist10(12), 805818.Google Scholar
Bellinger, D. (1980). Consistency in the pattern of change in mother’s speech: Some discriminant analyses. Journal of Child Language, 7, 469487.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Binet, A. (1905). On double consciousness: Experimental psychological studies. Chicago: Open Court Publishing Company.Google Scholar
Bjorklund, D. F., & Myers, A. (2019). The evolution of parenting. In Bornstein, M. H. (Ed.), Handbook of parenting (3rd ed., vol. 2, pp. 329). New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Bjorklund, D. F., & Schneider, W. (1996). The interaction of knowledge, aptitude, and strategies in children’s memory performance. Advances in Child Development and Behavior, 26, 5989.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blaga, O. M., Anderson, C. J., Shaddy, D. J., Kannass, K. N., Little, T. D., & Colombo, J. (2009). Structure and continuity of intelligence during early childhood. Intelligence, 37, 106113.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Block, J., & Block, J. H. (2006). Venturing a 30-year longitudinal studyAmerican Psychologist61(4), 315327.Google Scholar
Bornstein, M. H. (1985). How infant and mother jointly contribute to developing cognitive competence in the child. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 82, 74707473.Google Scholar
Bornstein, M. H. (1989a). Sensitive periods in development: Structural characteristics and causal interpretationsPsychological Bulletin105(2), 179197.Google Scholar
Bornstein, M. H. (1989b). Between caretakers and their young: Two modes of interaction and their consequences for cognitive growth. In Bornstein, M. H. & Bruner, J. S. (Eds.), Interaction in human development (pp. 197214). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Bornstein, M. H. (2013). Mother-infant attunement: A multilevel approach via body, brain, and behavior. In Legerstee, M., Haley, D. W., & Bornstein, M. H. (Eds.), The infant mind: Origins of the social brain (pp. 266298). New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
Bornstein, M. H. (2014). Human infancy … and the rest of the lifespan. Annual Review of Psychology, 65, 121158.Google Scholar
Bornstein, M. H. (2015). Children’s parents. In Bornstein, M. H. & Leventhal, T. (Vol. Eds.), Ecological settings and processes in developmental systems. In Lerner, R. M. (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology and developmental science (7th ed., vol. 4, pp. 55132). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.Google Scholar
Bornstein, M. H. (Ed.) (2018). SAGE encyclopedia of lifespan human development. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
Bornstein, M. H., & Arterberry, M. E. (2003). Recognition, categorization, and apperception of the facial expression of smiling by 5-month-old infants. Developmental Science, 6, 585599.Google Scholar
Bornstein, M. H., Arterberry, M. E., & Lamb, M. (2014). Development in infancy: A contemporary introduction (5th ed.). New York,: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Bornstein, M. H., Arterberry, M. E., & Mash, C. (2004). Long‐term memory for an emotional interpersonal interaction occurring at 5 months of ageInfancy6(3), 407416.Google Scholar
Bornstein, M. H., Arterberry, M. E., & Mash, C. (2013). Differentiated brain activity in response to faces of “own” versus “unfamiliar” babies in primipara mothers: An electrophysiological study. Developmental Neuropsychology, 38(6), 365385.Google Scholar
Bornstein, M. H., & Benasich, A. A. (1986). Infant habituation: Assessments of short-term reliability and individual differences at five months. Child Development, 57, 8799.Google Scholar
Bornstein, M. H., & Colombo, J. (2012). Infant cognitive functioning and mental development. In Pauen, S. (Ed.), Early childhood development and later achievement (pp. 118147). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Bornstein, M. H., Hahn, C.-S., Bell, C., Haynes, O. M., Slater, A., Golding, J., Wolke, D., & ALSPAC Study Team. (2006). Stability in cognition from early infancy: A developmental cascade. Psychological Science, 17, 151158.Google Scholar
Bornstein, M. H., Hahn, C.-H., & Putnick, D. L. (2016). Stability of core language skill across the first decade of life in children at biological and social risk. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 57, 14341443.Google Scholar
Bornstein, M. H., Hahn, C.-S., & Wolke, D. (2013). Systems and cascades in cognitive development and academic achievement. Child Development, 84, 154162.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bornstein, M. H., & Ludemann, P. L. (1989). Habituation at home. Infant Behavior and Development, 12, 525529.Google Scholar
Bornstein, M. H., & Putnick, D. L. (2012). Stability of language in childhood: A multiage, multidomain, multimeasure, and multisource study. Developmental Psychology, 48, 477491.Google Scholar
Bornstein, M. H., & Putnick, D. L. (2019). The architecture of the child mind: g, F, and their hierarchy. New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Bornstein, M. H., Putnick, D. L., & Esposito, G. (2017). Continuity and stability in development. Child Development Perspectives, 11, 113119.Google Scholar
Bornstein, M. H., & Sigman, M. D. (1986). Continuity in mental development from infancy. Child Development, 57, 251274.Google Scholar
Bornstein, M. H., Tal, J., Rahn, C., Galperín, C. Z., Pêcheux, M.-G., Lamour, M., et al. (1992). Functional analysis of the contents of maternal speech to infants of 5 and 13 months in four cultures: Argentina, France, Japan, and the United States. Developmental Psychology, 28, 593603.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bornstein, M. H., & Tamis-LeMonda, C. S. (1990). Activities and interactions of mothers and their firstborn infants in the first six months of life: Covariation, stability, continuity, correspondence, and prediction. Child Development, 61, 12061217.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bornstein, M. H., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., & Haynes, O. M. (1999). First words in the second year: Continuity, stability, and models of concurrent and predictive correspondence in vocabulary and verbal responsiveness across age and context. Infant Behavior and Development, 22, 6585.Google Scholar
Brody, N. (1992). Intelligence (2nd ed.). New York: Academic.Google Scholar
Broman, S. H. (1989). Infant physical status and later cognitive development. In Bornstein, M. H. & Krasnegor, N. A. (Eds.), Stability and continuity in mental development: Behavioral and biological perspectives (pp. 4562). Hillsdale, NJLawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Broman, S. H., Nichols, P. L., & Kennedy, W. A. (1975). Preschool IQ: Prenatal and early developmental correlates. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Bruer, J. (2002). The myth of the first three years. New York: The Free Press.Google Scholar
Butz, W. P., & Torrey, B. B. (2006). Some frontiers in social scienceScience312(5782), 18981900.Google Scholar
Chapman, R. S. (1981). Cognitive development and language comprehension in 10 to 21-month-olds. In Stark, R. E. (Ed.), Language behavior in infancy and early childhood (pp. 359394). New York: Elsevier North Holland.Google Scholar
Chen, Z., & Siegler, R. (2000). Intellectual development in childhood. In Sternberg, R. J. (Ed.), Handbook of intelligence (pp. 92116). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Choudhury, N., & Benasich, A. A. (2011). Maturation of auditory evoked potentials from 6 to 48 months: Prediction to 3 and 4 year language and cognitive abilitiesClinical Neurophysiology122(2), 320338.Google Scholar
Clarke, A. M., & Clarke, A. D. B. (Eds.) (1976). Early experience: Myth and evidence. New York: Free Press.Google Scholar
Coates, D. L., & Lewis, M. (1984). Early mother-infant interaction and infant cognitive status as predictors of school performance and cognitive behavior in six-year-oldsChild Development, 55, 12191230.Google Scholar
Colombo, J. (1993). Infant cognition: Predicting later intellectual functioning. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
Cooper, L. A., & Regan, D. T. (1986). Attention, perception, and intelligence. In Sternberg, R. J. (Ed.), Handbook of human intelligence (pp. 123169). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Cortina, J. M., & Landis, R. S. (2009). When small effect sizes tell a big story, and when large effect sizes don’t. In Lance, C. E. & Vandenberg, R. J. (Eds.), Statistical and methodological myths and urban legends: Doctrine, verity and fable in the organizational and social sciences (pp. 287308). New York: Taylor & Francis.Google Scholar
Courage, M. L., Howe, M. L., & Squires, S. E. (2004). Individual differences in 3.5-month-olds’ visual attention: What do they predict at 1 year?Infant Behavior and Development27(1), 1930.Google Scholar
Cuevas, K., & Bell, M. A. (2014). Infant attention and early childhood executive functionChild Development85(2), 397404.Google Scholar
Cummings, M. E., & Warmuth, K. M. (2019). Parenting and attachment. In Bornstein, M. H. (Ed.), Handbook of parenting (3rd ed., vol. 4, pp. 374400). New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Darwin, C. R. (1859). The origin of speciesNew York: Modern Library.Google Scholar
Darwin, C. R. (1877). A biographical sketch of an infant. Mind, 2, 286294.Google Scholar
Davis, K., Christodoulou, J. A., Seider, S., & Gardner, H. (2011). The theory of multiple intelligences. In Sternberg, R. J. & Kaufman, S. B. (Eds.), Cambridge handbook of intelligence (pp. 485503). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
de Boysson-Bardies, B., & Vihman, M. M. (1991). Adaptation to language: Evidence from babbling and first words in four languagesLanguage, 67(2), 297319.Google Scholar
Deary, I. J. (1995). Auditory inspection time and intelligence: What is the causal direction? Developmental Psychology, 31, 237250.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DeCasper, A. J., & Spence, M. J. (1986). Prenatal maternal speech influences newborns’ perception of speech sounds. Infant Behavior and Development, 9, 133150.Google Scholar
Demiris, Y., & Meltzoff, A. (2008). The robot in the crib: A developmental analysis of imitation skills in infants and robots. Infant and Child Development, 17, 4353.Google Scholar
Delauney-El Allam, M., Marlier, L., & Schaal, B. (2006). Learning at the breast: Preference formation for an artificial scent and its attraction against the odor of maternal milk. Infant Behavior and Development, 29, 308321.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Delauney-El Allam, M., Soussignan, R., Patris, B., Marlier, L., & Schaal, B. (2010). Long‐lasting memory for an odor acquired at the mother’s breast. Developmental Science, 13(6), 849863.Google Scholar
Dempster, F. N. (1991). Inhibitory processes: A neglected dimension of intelligence. Intelligence, 15, 157173.Google Scholar
Detterman, D. K. (1987). Theoretical notions of intelligence and mental retardation. American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 92, 211.Google Scholar
Dickstein, S., & Parke, R. D. (1988). Social referencing in infancy: A glance at fathers and marriage. Child Development, 59, 506511.Google Scholar
Dixon, W. E., & Smith, P. H. (2008). Attentional focus moderates habituation–language relationships: Slow habituation may be a good thingInfant and Child Development17(2), 95108.Google Scholar
Dollard, J., & Miller, N. (1950). Personality and psychotherapy. New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Domsch, H., Lohaus, A., & Thomas, H. (2009). Prediction of childhood cognitive abilities from a set of early indicators of information processing capabilitiesInfant Behavior and Development32(1), 91102.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dougherty, T. M., & Haith, M. (1997). Infant expectations and reaction time as predictors of childhood speed of processing and IQ. Developmental Psychology, 33, 146155.Google Scholar
Duncan, G. J., Dowsett, C. J., Claessens, A., Magnuson, K., Huston, A. C., Klebanov, P., et al. (2007). School readiness and later achievement. Developmental Psychology, 43, 14281446.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Elder, G. H., Shanahan, M. J., & Jennings, J. A. (2015). Human development in time and place. In Bornstein, M. H. & Leventhal, T. (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology and developmental science, vol. 4, Ecological settings and processes in developmental systems (7th ed., pp. 654). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.Google Scholar
Emmerich, W. (1964). Continuity and stability in early social developmentChild Development, 35(2), 311332.Google Scholar
Erikson, E. H. (1950). Childhood and society. New York: W. W. Norton and CompanyGoogle Scholar
Fagan, J. F., Holland, C. R., & Wheeler, K. (2007). The prediction, from infancy, of adult IQ and achievementIntelligence35(3), 225231.Google Scholar
Fagan, J. F., & Singer, L. T. (1983). Infant recognition memory as a measure of intelligence. In Lipsitt, L. P. (Ed.), Advances in infancy research (vol. 2, pp. 3179). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Fernald, A., Perfors, A., & Marchman, V. A. (2006). Picking up speed in understanding: Speech processing efficiency and vocabulary growth across the 2nd yearDevelopmental Psychology42(1), 98116.Google Scholar
Ferri, E., Bynner, J., & Wadsworth, M. (2003). Changing Britain, changing lives. London: Institute of Education, University of London.Google Scholar
Freud, S. (1949). An outline of psycho-analysis. New York: NortonGoogle Scholar
Frick, J. E., & Colombo, J. (1996). Individual differences in infant visual attention: Recognition of degraded visual forms by four-month-olds. Child Development, 67, 188204.Google Scholar
Frick, J. E., & Richards, J. E. (2001). Individual differences in infants’ recognition of briefly presented visual stimuli. Infancy, 2, 331352.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garcia-Sierra, A., Rivera-Gaxiola, M., Percaccio, C. R., Conboy, B. T., Romo, H., Klarman, L., et al. (2011). Bilingual language learning: An ERP study relating early brain responses to speech, language input, and later word productionJournal of Phonetics39(4), 546557.Google Scholar
Geangu, E., Benga, O., Stahl, D., & Striano, T. (2010). Contagious crying beyond the first days of life. Infant Behavior and Development, 33, 279288.Google Scholar
Geangu, E., Benga, O., Stahl, D., & Striano, T. (2011). Individual differences in infants’ emotional resonance to a peer in distress: Self-other awareness and emotion regulation. Social Development, 20, 450470.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gottfredson, L. S. (1997). Mainstream science on intelligence: An editorial with 52 signatories, history, and bibliography. Intelligence, 24(1), 1323.Google Scholar
Gottfried, A. W. (Ed.) (1984). Home environment and early cognitive development. Orlando, FL: Academic.Google Scholar
Goubet, N., Rattaz, C., Pierrat, V., Allemann, E., Bullinger, A., & Lequien, P. (2002). Olfactory familiarization and discrimination in preterm and full-term newborns. Infancy, 3, 5376.Google Scholar
Goubet, N., Strasbaugh, K., & Chesney, J. (2007). Familiarity breeds content? Soothing effect of a familiar odor on full-term newborns. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 28, 189194.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gould, S. J. (1985). Ontogeny and phylogeny. Cambridge, MA: Belknap.Google Scholar
Guerin, D. W., Gottfried, A. W., Oliver, P. H., & Thomas, C. W. (1994). Temperament and school functioning during early adolescence. Journal of Early Adolescence, 14, 200225.Google Scholar
Hall, G. S. (1891). Notes on the study of infants. Pedagogical Seminary, 1, 127138.Google Scholar
Hart, B., & Risley, T. R. (1995). Meaningful differences in the everyday experience of young American children. New York: Paul H. Brookes.Google Scholar
Hart, B., & Risley, T. R. (1999). The social world of children learning to talk. New York: Paul H. Brookes.Google Scholar
Hauser, R. M. (2009). The Wisconsin longitudinal study. In Elder, G. H. & Giele, J. Z. (Eds.), The craft of life course research (pp. 2950). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Hedges, L. L., & Oklin, I. (1985). Statistical methods for meta-analysis. Orlando, FL: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Hespos, S. J., & Baillargeon, R. (2008). Young infants’ actions reveal their developing knowledge of support variables: Converging evidence for violation-of-expectation findings. Cognition, 107, 304316.Google Scholar
Hespos, S. J., Ferry, A. L., & Rips, L. J. (2009). Five-month-old infants have different expectations for solids and liquids. Psychological Science, 20, 603611.Google Scholar
Hetherington, E. M., Parke, R. D., Gauvain, M., & Locke, V. O. (2006). Childhood psychology: A contemporary viewpoint (6th ed.). Boston: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Hirshberg, L. M., & Svejda, M. (1990). When infants look to their parents: I. Infants’ social referencing of mothers compared to fathers. Child Development, 61, 11751186.Google Scholar
Hornik, R., Risenhoover, N., & Gunnar, M. (1987). The effects of maternal positive, neutral, and negative affective communications on infant responses to new toys. Child Development, 58, 937944.Google Scholar
Humphreys, L. G., & Davey, T. C. (1988). Continuity in intellectual growth from 12 months to 9 years. Intelligence, 12, 183197.Google Scholar
Hunt, E. B. (1983). On the nature of intelligence. Science, 219, 141146.Google Scholar
Hutman, T., & Dapretto, M. (2009). The emergence of empathy during infancy. Cognition, Brain, Behavior, 13, 367390.Google Scholar
Jaeger, S. (1985). The origin of the diary method in developmental psychology. In Eckhardt, G., Bringmann, W. G., & Sprung, L. (Eds.), Contributions to a history of developmental psychology (pp. 6374). Berlin: Mouton.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
James, W. (1890). Principles of psychology. New York: Holt.Google Scholar
Jensen, A. R. (1980). Chronometric analysis of intelligenceJournal of Social and Biological Structures3(2), 103122.Google Scholar
Johnson, W., Emde, R. N., Pennbrook, B., Stenberg, C., & Davis, M. (1982). Maternal perception of infant emotion from birth through 18 months. Infant Behavior and Development, 5, 313322.Google Scholar
Kagan, J. (1976). Emergent themes in human development. American Scientist, 64(2), 186196.Google Scholar
Kagan, J. (2009). Three seductive ideas. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Kandel, E. C. (2007). In search of memory: The emergence of a new science of mind. New York: W. W. Norton.Google Scholar
Kavšek, M. (2004). Predicting later IQ from infant visual habituation and dishabituation: A meta-analysis. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 25, 369393.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kavšek, M., & Bornstein, M. H. (2010). Visual habituation and dishabituation in preterm infants: A review and meta-analysis. Research in Developmental Disabilities31(5), 951975.Google Scholar
Keen, R. E., & Berthier, N. E. (2004). Continuities and discontinuities in infants’ representation of objects and events. Advances in Child Development and Behavior, 32, 243279.Google Scholar
Kim, G., & Kwak, K. (2011). Uncertainty matters: Impact of stimulus ambiguity on infant social referencing. Infant and Child Development, 20, 449462.Google Scholar
Kirkham, N. Z., Slemmer, J. A., & Johnson, S. P. (2002). Visual statistical learning in infancy: Evidence for a domain general learning mechanism. Cognition, 83, B35B42.Google Scholar
Kirkham, N. Z., Slemmer, J. A., Richardson, D. C., & Johnson, S. P. (2007). Location, location, location: Development of spatiotemporal sequence learning in infancy. Child Development, 78, 15591571.Google Scholar
Kisilevsky, B. S., Hains, S. M. J., Lee, K., Xie, X., Huang, H., Ye, H.-H., et al. (2003). Effects of experience on fetal voice recognition. Psychological Science, 14, 220224.Google Scholar
Klein, P. S. (1988). Stability and change in interaction of Israeli mothers and infants. Infant Behavior and Development, 11, 5570.Google Scholar
Kopp, C. B., & McCall, R. B. (1980). Stability and instability in mental test performance among normal, at-risk, and handicapped infants and children. In Baltes, P. B. & Brim, O. G. Jr. (Eds.), Life-span development and behavior (vol. 4, pp. 3361). New YorkAcademic.Google Scholar
Kuhl, P. K. (2009). Linking infant speech perception to language acquisition: Phonetic learning predicts language growth. In McCardle, P., Colombo, J., & Freund, L. (Eds.), Infant pathways to language: Methods, models, and research directions. New York: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Kuhl, P. K., Conboy, B. T., Coffey-Corina, S., Padden, D., Rivera-Gaxiola, M., & Nelson, T. (2008). Phonetic learning as a pathway to language: New data and native language magnet theory expanded (NLM-e)Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences363(1493), 9791000.Google Scholar
Kuhl, P. K., Conboy, B. T., Padden, D., Nelson, T., & Pruitt, J. (2005). Early speech perception and later language development: Implications for the “critical period.” Language Learning and Development1(34), 237264.Google Scholar
Kuhl, P. K., & Rivera-Gaxiola, M. (2008). Neural substrates of language acquisitionAnnual Review of Neuroscience31, 511534.Google Scholar
Lamott, A. (2013). Some assembly required: A journal of my son’s first son. New York: Riverhead Books.Google Scholar
Laucht, M., Esser, G., & Schmidt, M. H. (1994). Contrasting infant predictors of later cognitive functioningJournal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry35(4), 649662.Google Scholar
Legerstee, M., & Markova, G. (2008). Variations in 10-month-old infant imitation of people and things. Infant Behavior and Development, 31, 8191.Google Scholar
Legg, S., & Hutter, M. (2007). A collection of definitions of intelligence. Frontiers in Artificial Intelligence and Applications, 157, 1724.Google Scholar
Lerner, R. M., Hershberg, R. M., Hilliard, L. J., & Johnson, S. K. (2015). Concepts and theories of human development. In Bornstein, M. H. & Lamb, M. E. (Eds.), Developmental science: An advanced textbook (7th ed., pp. 341). New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Lewis, M. (1997). Altering fate: Why the past does not predict the future. New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
Lewontin, R. (2005). The triple helix. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University PressGoogle Scholar
Lorenz, K. (1935/1970). Studies in animal and human behavior (Trans. R. Martin). London: Methuen.Google Scholar
Lunden, M., & Silven, M. (2011). Balanced communication in mid-infancy promotes early vocabulary development: Effects of play with mother and father in mono-and bilingual familiesInternational Journal of Bilingualism15(4), 535559.Google Scholar
Maccoby, E. E., & Martin, J. A. (1983). Socialization in the context of the family: Parent–child interaction. In Hetherington, E. M. (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology, vol. 4, Socialization, personality, and social development (3rd ed., pp. 1101). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Macken, M. A., & Barton, D. (1980). The acquisition of the voicing contrast in English: A study of voice onset time in word-initial stop consonantsJournal of Child Language7(1), 4174.Google Scholar
Manian, N., & Bornstein, M. H. (2009). Dynamics of emotion in infants of clinically depressed and nondepressed mothers. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 50, 14101418.Google Scholar
Marchman, V. A., & Fernald, A. (2008). Speed of word recognition and vocabulary knowledge in infancy predict cognitive and language outcomes in later childhoodDevelopmental Science11(3), F9F16.Google Scholar
Mash, C., Arterberry, M. E., & Bornstein, M. H. (2007). Mechanisms of visual object recognition in infancy: Five-month-olds generalize beyond the interpolation of familiar views. Infancy, 12, 3143.Google Scholar
McCall, R. B. (1994). What process mediates prediction of childhood IQ from infant habituation and recognition memory? Speculations on the roles of inhibition and rate of information processing. Intelligence, 18, 107125.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCall, R. B., & Carriger, M. S. (1993). A meta-analysis of infant habituation and recognition memory performance as predictors of later IQ. Child Development, 64, 5779.Google Scholar
McCall, R. B., Hogarty, P. S., & Hurlburt, N. (1972). Transitions in infant sensorimotor development and the prediction of childhood IQ. American Psychologist, 27(8), 728748.Google Scholar
McCrink, K., & Wynn, K. (2007). Ratio abstraction by 6-month-old infants. Psychological Science, 18, 740745.Google Scholar
McLaughlin, B., White, D., McDevitt, T., & Raskin, R. (1983). Mothers’ and fathers’ speech to their young children: Similar or different? Journal of Child Language, 10, 245252.Google Scholar
Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (2002). Imitation, memory, and the representation of persons. Infant Behavior and Development, 25, 3961.Google Scholar
Mendelson, M. J. (1993). Becoming a brother: A child learns about life, family, and self. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Molfese, D. L. (2000). Predicting dyslexia at 8 years of age using neonatal brain responsesBrain and Language72(3), 238245.Google Scholar
Murray, L., de Rosnay, M., Pearson, J., Bergeron, C., Schofield, E., Royal-Lawson, M., et al. (2008). Intergenerational transmission of social anxiety: The role of social referencing processes in infancy. Child Development, 79, 10491064.Google Scholar
Murray, L., & Trevarthen, C. (1985). Emotional regulation of interactions between two-month-olds and their mothers. In Field, T. M. & Fox, N. A. (Eds.), Social perception in infants (pp. 177197). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Needham, A. (2009). Learning in infants’ object perception, object-directed action, and tool use. In Woodward, A. & Needham, A. (Eds.), Learning and the infant mind (pp. 208226). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Nettelbeck, T. (1987). Inspection time and intelligence. In Vernon, P. A. (Ed.), Speed of information processing and intelligence (pp. 295346). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Newman, R., Ratner, N. B., Jusczyk, A. M., Jusczyk, P. W., & Dow, K. A. (2006). Infants’ early ability to segment the conversational speech signal predicts later language development: A retrospective analysisDevelopmental Psychology42(4), 643655.Google Scholar
Nicely, P., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., & Bornstein, M. H. (1999). Mothers’ attuned responses to infant affect expressivity promote earlier achievement of language milestones. Infant Behavior and Development, 22, 557568.Google Scholar
Oakes, L. M., Horst, J. S., Kovack-Lesh, K. L., & Perone, S. (2009). How infants learn categories. In Woodward, A. & Needham, A. (Eds.), Learning and the infant mind (pp. 144171). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Overton, W. F. (2015). Process and relational developmental systems. In Overton, W. F. & Molenaar, P. C. (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology and developmental science, vol. 1, Theory and method (7th ed., pp. 962). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.Google Scholar
Pancsofar, N., Vernon-Feagans, L., & Family Life Project Investigators. (2010). Fathers’ early contributions to children’s language development in families from low-income rural communitiesEarly Childhood Research Quarterly25(4), 450463.Google Scholar
Parke, R. D., & Cookston, J. (2019). Fathers and families. In Bornstein, M. H. (Ed.), Handbook of parenting (3rd ed., vol.3, pp. 64136). New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Pearson, R. M., Lightman, S. L., & Evans, J. (2011). Attentional processing of infant emotion during late pregnancy and mother-infant relations after birth. Archives of Women’s Mental Health, 14(1), 2331.Google Scholar
Phelps, E., Furstenberg, F. F. Jr., & Colby, A. (Eds.) (2002). Looking at lives: American longitudinal studies of the twentieth century. Troy, NY: Russell Sage Foundation.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1952). The origins of intelligence in children (Trans. M. Cook). New York: W. W. Norton.Google Scholar
Plato (1970). The laws (Trans. T. J. Saunders). Harmondsworth, Middlesex: Penguin. (Original work written ca. 355 BC)Google Scholar
Plomin, R., & DeFries, J. C. (1985). Origins of individual differences in infancy: The Colorado Adoption Project. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Porter, R. H., & Levy, F. (1995). Olfactory mediation of mother–infant interactions in a selected mammalian species. In Wong, R. (Ed.), Biological perspectives on motivated activities (pp. 77110). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Porter, R. H., & Winberg, J. (1999). Unique salience of maternal breast odors for newborn infants. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 23, 439449.Google Scholar
Prentice, D. A., & Miller, D. T. (1992). When small effects are impressivePsychological Bulletin112(1), 160164.Google Scholar
Preyer, W. (1882). Die seele des kindes. Leipzig: Grieben. Published in English in 1888–1889 as The mind of the child, Parts 1 and 2. (trans. H. W. Brown). New York: Appleton. (Reprinted by Arno Press, 1973.)Google Scholar
Prochner, L., & Doyon, P. (1997). Researchers and their subjects in the history of child study: William Blatz and the Dionne quintuplets. Canadian Psychology, 38, 103110.Google Scholar
Rankin, C., Abrams, T., Barry, R., Bhatnagar, S., Cerruti, D., Fang, C.-W., & Thompson, R. (2009). Habituation: An evaluation and revision of Thompson and Spencer (1966). Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 92, 135138.Google Scholar
Reilly, S., Bavin, E. L., Bretherton, L., Conway, L., Eadie, P., Cini, E., et al. (2009). The Early Language in Victoria Study (ELVS): A prospective, longitudinal study of communication skills and expressive vocabulary development at 8, 12 and 24 monthsInternational Journal of Speech-Language Pathology11(5), 344357.Google Scholar
Richards, J. E. (1997). Effects of attention on infants’ preference for briefly exposed visual stimuli in the paired-comparison recognition-memory paradigm. Developmental Psychology, 33, 2231.Google Scholar
Rivera‐Gaxiola, M., Silva‐Pereyra, J., & Kuhl, P. K. (2005). Brain potentials to native and non‐native speech contrasts in 7‐and 11‐month‐old American infants. Developmental science8(2), 162172.Google Scholar
Rose, S. A., Feldman, J. F., & Jankowski, J. J. (2012). Implications of infant cognition for executive functions at age 11Psychological Science23(11), 13451355.Google Scholar
Rosenthal, R., & Rubin, D. B. (1982). A simple, general purpose display of magnitude of experimental effectJournal of Educational Psychology74(2), 166169.Google Scholar
Rosenthal, R., & Rubin, D. B. (1983). A note on percent variance explained as a measure of the importance of effectsJournal of Applied Social Psychology9(5), 395396.Google Scholar
Rousseau, J. J. (1762). Emile. New York: Barron’s Educational Series.Google Scholar
Rovee-Collier, C. (1997). Dissociations in infant memory: Rethinking the development of implicit and explicit memoryPsychological Review104(3), 467498.Google Scholar
Rovee-Collier, C., & Cuevas, K. (2009). Multiple memory systems are unnecessary to account for infant memory development: An ecological model. Developmental Psychology, 45, 160174.Google Scholar
Sattler, J. M. (1992). Assessment of children’s intelligence. In Walker, C. E. & Roberts, M. C. (Eds.), Handbook of clinical child psychology (2nd ed., pp. 85100). Oxford: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Scarr, S., Weinberg, R. A., & Waldman, I. D. (1993). IQ correlations in transracial adoptive families. Intelligence, 17, 541555.Google Scholar
Shaddy, D. J., & Colombo, J. (2004). Developmental changes in infant attention to dynamic and static stimuli. Infancy, 5, 355365.Google Scholar
Siegel, L. S. (1989). A reconceptualisation of prediction from infant test scores. In Bornstein, M. H. & Krashnegor, N. A. (Eds.), Stability and continuity in mental development: Behavioral and biological perspectives (pp. 89103). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Sigman, M., Cohen, S. E., & Beckwith, L. (1997). Why does infant attention predict adolescent intelligence? Infant Behavior and Development20(2), 133140.Google Scholar
Sigman, M., Cohen, S. E., Beckwith, L., Asarnow, R., & Parmelee, A. H. (1991). Continuity in cognitive abilities from infancy to 12 years of ageCognitive Development6(1), 4757.Google Scholar
Sigman, M., Cohen, S. E., Beckwith, L., & Topinka, C. (1987). Task persistence in 2-year-old preterm infants in relation to subsequent attentiveness and intelligence. Infant Behavior and Development, 10, 295305.Google Scholar
Singh, L., Liederman, J., Mierzejewski, R., & Barnes, J. (2011). Rapid reacquisition of native phoneme contrasts after disuse: You do not always lose what you do not useDevelopmental science14(5), 949959.Google Scholar
Smith, L., Fagan, J. F., & Ulvund, S. E. (2002). The relation of recognition memory in infancy and parental socioeconomic status to later intellectual competence. Intelligence, 30(3), 247259.Google Scholar
Sorce, J. F., & Emde, R. N. (1981). Mother’s presence is not enough: Effect of emotional availability on infant exploration. Developmental Psychology, 17, 737745.Google Scholar
Sroufe, L. A., Egeland, B., Carlson, E., & Collins, W. A. (2005). The development of the person: The Minnesota study of risk and adaptation from birth to adulthood. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Stankov, L. (1983). Attention and intelligence. Journal of Educational Psychology, 75, 471490.Google Scholar
Steiner, J. E. (1979). Human facial expressions in response to taste and smell stimulation. In Reese, H. & Lipsitt, L. (Eds.), Advances in child development and behavior (vol. 13, pp. 257295). New York: Academic.Google Scholar
Stern, D. (1990). Diary of a child. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Sternberg, R. J. (Ed.) (2020). Cambridge handbook of intelligence (2nd ed.). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Sternberg, R. J., & Detterman, D. K. (Eds.) (1986). What is intelligence? Contemporary viewpoints on its nature and definition. Santa Barbara, CA: Praeger Pub Text.Google Scholar
Sternberg, R. J., Grigorenko, E. L., & Bundy, D. A. (2001). The predictive value of IQ. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 47, 141.Google Scholar
Stoecker, J. J., Colombo, J., Frick, J. E., & Ryther, J. S. (1998). Long- and short-looking infants’ recognition of symmetrical and asymmetrical visual forms. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 71, 6378.Google Scholar
Taine, H. A. (1877). Taine on the acquisition of language by children. Mind, 2, 252259.Google Scholar
Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., & Bornstein, M. H. (1989). Habituation and maternal encouragement of attention in infancy as predictors of toddler language, play, and representational competence. Child Development, 60, 738751.Google Scholar
Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., & Bornstein, M. H. (1993). Antecedents of exploratory competence at one year. Infant Behavior and Development, 16(4), 423439.Google Scholar
Thompson, R. A. (2006). The development of the person: Social understanding, relationships, conscience, self. In Kuhn, D. & Siegler, R. S. (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Cognition, perception, and language (6th ed., vol. 2, pp. 2498). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.Google Scholar
Thompson, R. F., & Spencer, W. A. (1966). Habituation: A model phenomenon for the study of neuronal substrates of behavior. Psychological Review, 73, 1643.Google Scholar
Tiedemann, D. (1787).Beobachtungen über die Entwicklung der Seelenfähigkeiten bei Kindern. Hessische Beiträge zur Gelehrsamkeit und Kunst, 2, 313315 and 3, 486488. (Observations on the development of the mental faculties of children, Pedagogical Seminary, 1927, 34, 205230.)Google Scholar
Tinbergen, N. (1951). The study of instinct. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Tsao, F. M., Liu, H. M., & Kuhl, P. K. (2004). Speech perception in infancy predicts language development in the second year of life: A longitudinal studyChild Development75(4), 10671084.Google Scholar
Vacha-Haase, T., & Thompson, B. (2004). How to estimate and interpret various effect sizesJournal of Counseling Psychology51(4), 473481.Google Scholar
Vernon, P. E. (1947). The variations of intelligence with occupation, age, and localityBritish Journal of Mathematical and Statistical Psychology1(1), 5263.Google Scholar
Vernon, P. E. (Ed.) (1987). Speed of information-processing and intelligence. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Wachs, T. D., & Chan, A. (1986). Specificity of environmental action, as seen in environmental correlates of infants’ communication performance. Child Development, 57, 14641474.Google Scholar
Wagner, L., & Lakusta, L. (2009). Using language to navigate the infant mind. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 4, 177184.Google Scholar
Wallace, D. B., Franklin, M. B., & Keegan, R. T. (1994). The observing eye: A century of baby diaries. Human Development, 37(1), 129.Google Scholar
Watson, J. B. (1924/1970). Behaviorism (Rev. ed.) New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Wechsler, D. (1958). The measurement and appraisal of adult intelligence (4th ed.). Baltimore, MD: Williams & Wilkins Co.Google Scholar
Winnicott, D. W. (1965). The maturational processes and the facilitating environment: Studies in the theory of emotional development. New York: International Universities Press.Google Scholar
Wohlwill, J. F. (1973). The study of behavioral development. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Wolfe, C. D., & Bell, M. A. (2007). The integration of cognition and emotion during infancy and early childhood: Regulatory processes associated with the development of working memoryBrain and Cognition65(1), 313.Google Scholar
Woodward, A. L. (2009). Infants’ grasp of others’ intentions. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 18, 5357.Google Scholar
Yamaguchi, M., Kuhlmeier, V. A., Wynn, K., & VanMarle, K. (2009). Continuity in social cognition from infancy to childhood. Developmental Science, 12(5), 746752.Google Scholar
Yeaton, W., & Sechrest, L. (1981). Meaningful measures of effect. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 49, 766767.Google Scholar
Yu, H., McCoach, D. B., Gottfried, A. W., & Gottfried, A. E. (2018). Stability of intelligence from infancy through adolescence: An autoregressive latent variable model. Intelligence, 69, 815.Google Scholar
Zigler, E., Abelson, W. D., & Seitz, V. (1973). Motivational factors in the performance of economically disadvantaged children on the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test. Child Development, 44, 294303.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×