Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-p9bg8 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-23T00:09:52.954Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

16 - The Bias of Crowds: Rethinking Implicit Bias in Social Context

from Section IV - Improving Measurement and Theorizing About Implicit Bias

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  aN Invalid Date NaN

Jon A. Krosnick
Affiliation:
Stanford University, California
Tobias H. Stark
Affiliation:
Utrecht University, The Netherlands
Amanda L. Scott
Affiliation:
The Strategy Team, Columbus, Ohio
Get access

Summary

Implicit bias has always been understood as an individual attitude that is rooted in one’s social environment. However, in practice, the field has focused more heavily on the individual attitude, to the neglect of the social environment. In this chapter, we describe an alternative view of implicit bias – the Bias of Crowds model – that reinterprets implicit bias as a feature of social contexts more than persons. In doing so, we argue that, akin to the “wisdom of crowds” effect, implicit bias may emerge as the aggregate effect of individual fluctuations in concept accessibility that are transitory and context-dependent. We also explain how this novel interpretation of implicit bias resolves long-standing concerns regarding the temporal instability and weak predictive validity of implicit attitudes measures. Finally, we review direct empirical tests of the model and its predictions and consider future avenues for research, as well as theoretical and practical implications.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2025

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Anderson, C. (2016). White Rage: The Unspoken Truth of Our Racial Divide. New York, NY: Bloomsbury Publishing.Google Scholar
Arendt, F., & Northrup, T. (2015). Effects of long-term exposure to news stereotypes on implicit and explicit attitudes. International Journal of Communication, 9, 23702390.Google Scholar
Banaji, M. R. (2001). Implicit attitudes can be measured. In Roediger, H. L. III, Nairne, J. S., Neath, I. E., & Surprenant, A. M. (Eds.), The Nature of Remembering: Essays in Honor of Robert G. Crowder. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association, pp. 117150.Google Scholar
Baron, A. S. (2015). Constraints on the development of implicit intergroup attitudes. Child Development Perspectives, 9, 5054. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12105CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baron, A. S., & Banaji, M. R. (2006). The development of implicit attitudes: Evidence of race evaluations from ages 6 and 10 and adulthood. Psychological Science, 17, 5358. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2005.01664.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berard, T. J. (2008). The neglected social psychology of institutional racism. Sociology Compass, 2, 734764. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1751-9020.2007.00089.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bertrand, M., & Mullainathan, S. (2004). Are Emily and Greg more employable than Lakisha and Jamal? A field experiment on labor market discrimination. American Economic Review, 94, 9911013. https://doi.org/10.1257/0002828042002561CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blair, I. V., & Brondolo, E. (2017). Moving beyond the individual: Community-level prejudice and health. Social Science & Medicine, 183, 169172. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2017.04.041CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bobo, L. D., & Charles, C. Z. (2009). Race in the American mind: From the Moynihan report to the Obama candidacy. The Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science, 621, 243259. https://doi.org/10.1177/0002716208324759CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cameron, C. D., Brown-Iannuzzi, J. L., & Payne, B. K. (2012). Sequential priming measures of implicit social cognition: A meta-analysis of associations with behavior and explicit attitudes. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 16, 330350. https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868312440047CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carlsson, R., & Agerström, J. (2016). A closer look at the discrimination outcomes in the IAT literature. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 57, 278287. https://doi.org/10.1111/sjop.12288CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clemen, R. T. (1989). Combining forecasts: A review and annotated bibliography. International Journal of Forecasting, 5, 559583. https://doi.org/10.1016/0169-2070(89)90012-5CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cunningham, W. A., Preacher, K. J., & Banaji, M. R. (2001). Implicit attitude measures: Consistency, stability, and convergent validity. Psychological Science, 12, 163170. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00328CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dasgupta, N. (2013). Implicit attitudes and beliefs adapt to situations: A decade of research on the malleability of implicit prejudice, stereotypes, and the self-concept. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 47, 233279. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-407236-7.00005-XCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dasgupta, N., & Asgari, S. (2004). Seeing is believing: Exposure to counterstereotypic women leaders and its effect on the malleability of automatic gender stereotyping. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 40, 642658. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2004.02.003CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dasgupta, N., & Rivera, L. M. (2008). When social context matters: The influence of long-term contact and short-term exposure to admired outgroup members on implicit attitudes and behavioral intentions. Social Cognition, 26, 5466. https://doi.org/10.1521/soco.2008.26.1.112CrossRefGoogle Scholar
De Houwer, J. (2014). A propositional model of implicit evaluation. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 8, 342353. https://doi.org/10.1111/spc3.12111CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dovidio, J. F., Kawakami, K., & Gaertner, S. L. (2002). Implicit and explicit prejudice and interracial interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82, 6268. https://doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.82.1.62CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dunham, Y., Baron, A., & Banaji, M. (2006). From American city to Japanese village: A cross-cultural investigation of implicit race attitudes. Child Development, 77, 12681281. Retrieved from: www.jstor.org/stable/3878431CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dunham, Y., Baron, A. S., & Banaji, M. R. (2008). The development of implicit intergroup cognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12, 248253. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2008.04.006CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dunham, Y., Chen, E. E., & Banaji, M. R. (2013). Two signatures of implicit intergroup attitudes: Developmental invariance and early enculturation. Psychological Science, 24, 860868. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797612463081CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fazio, R. H., Jackson, J. R., Dunton, B. C., et al. (1995). Variability in automatic activation as an unobtrusive measure of racial attitudes: A bona fide pipline? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 10131027. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.69.6.1013CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fazio, R. H., Sanbonmatsu, D. M., Powell, M. C., et al. (1986). On the automatic activation of attitudes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 229238. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.50.2.229CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fazio, R. H., & Williams, C. J. (1986). Attitude accessibility as a moderator of the attitude-perception and attitude-behavior relations: An investigation of the 1984 presidential election. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 505514. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.51.3.505CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Feagin, J. R. (2006). Systemic Racism: A Theory of Oppression. New York, NY: Routledge/Taylor & Francis Group.Google Scholar
Feagin, J. R., & Feagin, C. B. (1986). Discrimination American style: Institutional Racism and Sexism. Malabar, FL: R.E. Krieger Publishing Company.Google Scholar
Forscher, P. S., Lai, C. K., Axt, J. R., et al. (2019). A meta-analysis of change in implicit bias. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 117, 522559. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspa0000160CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fredrickson, G. M. (2015). Racism: A Short History. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Galton, F. (1907). Vox populi. Nature, 75(1949), 7. https://doi.org/10.1038/075450a0CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gawronski, B., & Bodenhausen, G. V. (2006). Associative and propositional processes in evaluation: An integrative review of implicit and explicit attitude change. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 692731. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.132.5.692CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gawronski, B., & De Houwer, J. (2014). Implicit measures in social and personality psychology. In Reis, H. T., & Judd, C. M. (Eds.), Handbook of Research Methods in Social and Personality Psychology (2nd ed., pp. 283310). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gawronski, B., Morrison, M., Phills, C. E., et al. (2017). Temporal stability of implicit and explicit measures: A longitudinal analysis. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 43, 300312. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167216684131CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gawronski, B., & Payne, B. K. (Eds.). (2010). Handbook of Implicit Social Cognition: Measurement, Theory, and Applications. New York, NY: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Giulietti, C., Tonin, M., & Vlassopoulos, M. (2019). Racial discrimination in local public services: A field experiment in the United States. Journal of European Economic Association, 17(1). https://doi.org/10.1093/jeea/jvx045CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Green, A. R., Carney, D. R., Pallin, D. J., et al. (2007). Implicit bias among physicians and its prediction of thrombolysis decision for black and white patients. Journal of General Internal Medicine, 22, 12311238. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11606-007-0258-5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greenwald, A. G., & Banaji, M. R. (1995). Implicit social cognition: Attitudes, self-esteem, and stereotypes. Psychological Review, 102, 427. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.102.1.4CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greenwald, A. G., Banaji, M. R., & Nosek, B. A. (2015). Statistically small effects of the Implicit Association Test can have societally large effects. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 108, 553561. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspa0000016CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E., & Schwartz, J. L. K. (1998). Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: The Implicit Association Test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 14641480. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.74.6.1464CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greenwald, A. G., Poehlman, T. A., Uhlmann, E. L., et al. (2009). Understanding and using the Implicit Association Test: III. Meta-analysis of predictive validity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97, 1741. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0015575CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greenwald, A. G., Smith, C. T., Sriram, N., et al. (2009). Implicit race attitudes predicted vote in the 2008 U.S. presidential election. Analyses of Social Issues and Public Policy, 9, 241253. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1530-2415.2009.01195.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hall, W. J., Chapman, M. V., Lee, K. M., et al. (2015). Implicit racial/ethnic bias among health care professionals and its influence on health care outcomes: A systematic review. American Journal of Public Health, 105, e60e76. https://doi.org/10.2105/AJPH.2015.302903CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hehman, E., Flake, J. K., & Calanchini, J. (2018). Disproportionate use of lethal force in policing is associated with regional racial biases of residents. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 9, 393401. https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550617711229CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heiphetz, L., Spelke, E. S., & Banaji, M. R. (2013). Patterns of implicit and explicit attitudes in children and adults: Tests in the domain of religion. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 142, 864879. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0029714CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hensch, M. (2016, September 26). Clinton: We must fight ‘implicit bias.’ The Hill. Retrieved from: http://thehill.com/blogs/ballot-box/presidential-races/297939-clinton-i-think-implicit-bias-is-a-problem-for-everyoneGoogle Scholar
Higgins, E. T. (1996). Knowledge activation: Accessibility, applicability, and salience. In Higgins, E. T. & Kruglanski, A. W. (Eds.), Social Psychology: Handbook of Basic Principles. New York, NY: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Higgins, E. T., King, G. A., & Mavin, G. H. (1982). Individual construct accessibility and subjective impressions and recall. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 43, 3547. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.43.1.35CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Higgins, E. T., Rholes, W. S., & Jones, C. R. (1977). Category accessibility and impression formation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 13, 141154. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-1031(77)80007-3CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hofmann, W., Gawronski, B., Gschwendner, T., et al. (2005). A meta-analysis on the correlation between the Implicit Association Test and explicit self-report measures. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31, 13691385. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167205275613CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kugelmass, H. (2016). “Sorry, I’m not accepting new patients”: An audit study of access to mental health care. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 57, 168183. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022146516647098CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lai, C. K., Skinner, A. L., Cooley, E., et al. (2016). Reducing implicit racial preferences: II. Intervention effectiveness across time. Journal of Expeirmental Psychology: General, 145, 10011016. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000179Google ScholarPubMed
Legewie, J., & Schaeffer, M. (2016). Contested boundaries: Explaining where ethnoracial diversity provokes neighborhood conflict. American Journal of Sociology, 122, 125161. https://doi.org/10.1086/686942CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leitner, J. B., Hehman, E., Ayduk, O., et al. (2016). Racial bias is associated with ingroup death rate for Blacks and Whites: Insights from Project Implicit. Social Science & Medicine, 170, 220227. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2016.10.007CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leitner, J. B., Hehman, E., & Snowden, L. R. (2018). States higher in racial bias spend less on disabled Medicaid enrollees. Social Science & Medicine, 208, 150157. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2018.01.013CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lorge, I., Fox, D., Davitz, J., et al. (1958). A survey of studies contrasting the quality of group performance and individual performance, 1920–1957. Psychological Bulletin, 55, 337372. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0042344CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
MacDonald, H. (2017, October 9). The false ‘science’ of implicit bias. The Wall Street Journal. Retrieved from: www.wsj.com/articles/the-false-science-of-implicit-bias-1507590908Google Scholar
Marini, M., Sriram, N., Schnabel, K., et al. (2013). Overweight people have low levels of implicit weight bias, but overweight nations have high levels of implicit weight bias. PLoS ONE, 8(12), e83543. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0083543CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McConahay, J. B., Hardee, B. B., & Batts, V. (1981). Has racism declined in America? It depends on who is asking and what is asked. The Journal of Conflict Resolution, 25, 563579. https://doi.org/10.1177/002200278102500401CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mejia, J., & Parker, C. (2018). When transparency fails: Bias and financial incentives in ridesharing platforms. Kelley School of Business Research Paper No. 1859. https://doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.3209274CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mellers, B., Ungar, L., Baron, J., et al. (2014). Psychological strategies for winning a geopolitical forecasting tournament. Psychological Science, 25, 11061115. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797614524255CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meyer, D. E., & Schvaneveldt, R. W. (1971). Facilitation in recognizing pairs of words: Evidence of a dependence between retrieval operations. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 90, 227234. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0031564CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Milkman, K. L., Akinola, M., & Chugh, D. (2015). What happens before? A field experiment exploring how pay and representation differentially shape bias on the pathway into organizations. Journal of Applied Psychology, 100, 16781712. https://doi.org/10.1037/apl0000022CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, C. T., Varni, S. E., Solomon, S. E., et al. (2016). Macro-level implicit HIV prejudice and the health of community residents with HIV. Health Psychology, 35, 807815. https://doi.org/10.1037/hea0000314CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moss-Racusin, C., Dovidio, J. F., Brescoll, V. L., et al. (2012). Science faculty’s subtle gender biases favor male students. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109, 1647416479. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1211286109CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nordell, J. (2017, May 7). Is this how discrimination ends? The Atlantic. Retrieved from: www.theatlantic.com/science/archive/2017/05/unconscious-bias-training/525405/Google Scholar
Norton, M. I., Vandello, J. A., & Darley, J. M. (2004). Casuistry and social category bias. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87, 817831. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.87.6.817CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nosek, B. A., & Banaji, M. R. (2001). The go/no-go association task. Social Cognition, 19, 625666. https://doi.org/10.1521/soco.19.6.625.20886CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nosek, B. A., Smyth, F. L., Hansen, J. J., et al. (2007). Pervasiveness and correlates of implicit attitudes and stereotypes. European Review of Social Psychology, 18, 3688. https://doi.org/10.1080/10463280701489053CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nosek, B. A., Smyth, F. L., Sriram, N., et al. (2009). National differences in gender-science stereotypes predict national sex differences in science and math achievement. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106, 1059310597. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0809921106CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Okonofua, J. A., & Eberhardt, J. L. (2015). Two strikes: Race and the disciplining of young students. Psychological Science, 26, 617624. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797615570365CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Olson, M. A., & Fazio, R. H. (2006). Reducing automatically activated racial prejudice through implicit evaluative conditioning. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32, 421433. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167205284004CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Orchard, J., & Price, J. (2017). County-level racial prejudice and the black-white gap in infant health outcomes. Social Science & Medicine, 181, 191198. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2017.03.036CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oswald, F. L., Mitchell, G., Blanton, H., et al. (2013). Predicting ethnic and racial discrimination: A meta-analysis of IAT criterion studies. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 105, 171192. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0032734CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oswald, F. L., Mitchell, G., Blanton, H., et al. (2015). Using the IAT to predict ethnic and racial discrimination: Small effect sizes of unknown societal significance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 108, 562571. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspa0000023CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Page, S. (2007). The Difference: How the Power of Diversity Creates Better Groups, Firms, Schools, and Societies. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Park, M. (2018, April 17). What the Starbucks incident tells us about implicit bias. CNN. Retrieved from: www.cnn.com/2018/04/17/health/implicit-bias-philadelphia-starbucks/index.htmlGoogle Scholar
Park, J., Felix, K., & Lee, G. (2007). Implicit attitudes toward Arab-Muslims and the moderating effects of social information. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 29, 3545. https://doi.org/10.1080/01973530701330942CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Payne, B. K. (2001). Prejudice and perception: The role of automatic and controlled processes in misperceiving a weapon. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 181192. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.81.2.181CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Payne, B. K., Cheng, C. M., Govorun, O., et al. (2005). An inkblot for attitudes: Affect misattribution as implicit measurement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 277293. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.89.3.277CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Payne, B. K., Krosnick, J. A., Pasek, J., et al. (2010). Implicit and explicit prejudice in the 2008 American presidential election. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 46, 367374. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2009.11.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Payne, B. K., & Lundberg, K. B. (2014). The affect misattribution procedure: Ten years of evidence on reliability, validity, and mechanisms. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 8, 672686. https://doi.org/10.1111/spc3.12148CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Payne, K., Niemi, L., & Doris, J. M. (2018, March 27). How to think about “implicit bias.” Scientific American. Retrieved from: www.scientificamerican.com/article/how-to-think-about-implicit-bias/Google Scholar
Payne, B. K., Vuletich, H. A., & Brown-Iannuzzi, J. L. (2018). Historical roots of implicit bias in slavery. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 116, 1169311698. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1818816116CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Payne, B. K., Vuletich, H. A., & Lundberg, K. B. (2017a). The bias of crowds: How implicit bias bridges personal and systemic prejudice. Psychological Inquiry, 28, 233248. https://doi.org/10.1080/1047840X.2017.1335568CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Payne, B. K., Vuletich, H. A., & Lundberg, K. B. (2017b). Flipping the script on implicit bias research with the Bias of Crowds. Psychological Inquiry, 28, 306311. https://doi.org/10.1080/1047840X.2017.1380460CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Petty, R. E., Briñol, P., & DeMarree, K. G. (2007). The meta-cognitive model (MCM) of attitudes: Implications for attitude measurement, change, and strength. Social Cognition, 25, 609642. https://doi.org/10.1521/soco.2007.25.5.657CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pryor, J. B., Reeder, G. D., Wesselmann, E. D., et al. (2013). The influence of social norms upon behavioral expressions of implicit and explicit weight-related stigma in an interactive game. Yale Journal of Biology and Medicine, 86, 189201.Google Scholar
Putnam, R. (2007) ‘E Pluribus Unum: Diversity and Community in the Twenty-First Century: The Johan Skytte Prize Lecture’. Scandinavian Political Studies, 30, 134–67. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9477.2007.00176.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rae, J. R., Newheiser, A., & Olson, K. R. (2015). Exposure to racial out-groups and implicit race bias in the United States. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 6, 535543. https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550614567357CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rholes, W. S., & Pryor, J. B. (1982). Cognitive accessibility and causal attributions. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 8, 719729. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167282084019CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Richeson, J. A., & Sommers, S. R. (2016). Toward a social psychology of race and race relations for the twenty-first century. Annual Review of Psychology, 67, 439463. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-010213-115115CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roediger, H. L. III, & McDermott, K. B. (1995). Creating false memories: Remembering words not presented in lists. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21, 803814. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.21.4.803Google Scholar
Rooth, D. O. (2010). Automatic associations and discrimination in hiring: Real world evidence. Labour Economics, 17, 523534. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.labeco.2009.04.005CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rose, J. (2018, May 29). ‘I’m not aware of that’: Starbucks employees receive racial bias training. National Public Radio. Retrieved from: www.npr.org/2018/05/29/615263473/thousands-of-starbucks-stores-close-for-racial-bias-trainingGoogle Scholar
Rudman, L.A., Phelan, J. E., & Heppen, J. B. (2007). Developmental sources of implicit attitudes. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33, 17001713. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167207307487CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Salter, P., & Adams, G. (2013). Toward a critical race psychology. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 7, 781793. https://doi.org/10.1111/spc3.12068CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sawyer, J., & Gampa, A. (2018). Implicit and explicit racial attitudes changed during Black Lives Matter. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 44, 10391059. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167218757454CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schmid, K., Ramiah, A. A., & Hewstone, M. (2014). Neighborhood ethnic diversity and trust: The role of intergroup contact and perceived threat. Psychological Science, 25, 665674. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797613508956CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schuman, H., Steeh, C., Bobo, L., et al. (1997). Racial Attitudes in America. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Siegel, R. (2018, May 29). Starbucks’s bias training finally happened. Here’s what it looked like. The Washington Post. Retrieved from: www.washingtonpost.com/news/business/wp/2018/05/29/starbucks-training-finally-happened-heres-what-it-looked-likeGoogle Scholar
Sinclair, S., Dunn, E., & Lowery, B. (2005). The relationship between parental racial attitudes and children’s implicit prejudice. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 41, 283289. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2004.06.003CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sinclair, S., Lowery, B. S., Hardin, C. D., et al. (2005). Social tuning of automatic racial attitudes: The role of affiliative motivation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 583592. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.89.4.583CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Singal, J. (2017, January 11). Psychology’s favorite tool for measuring racism isn’t up to the job. New York Magazine. Retrieved from: www.thecut.com/2017/01/psychologys-racism-measuring-tool-isnt-up-to-the-job.htmlGoogle Scholar
Srull, T. K., & Wyer, R. S. (1979). The role of category accessibility in the interpretation of information about persons: Some determinants and implications. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 16601672. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.37.10.1660CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Srull, T. K., & Wyer, R. S. (1980). Category accessibility and social perception: Some implications for the study of person memory and interpersonal judgment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 38, 841856. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.38.6.841CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stolle, D., Soroka, S., & Johnston, R. (2008). When does diversity erode trust? Neighborhood diversity, interpersonal trust and the mediating effect of social interactions. Political Studies, 56, 5775. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9248.2007.00717.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Surowiecki, J. (2004). The Wisdom of Crowds: Why the Many Are Smarter Than the Few and How Collective Wisdom Shapes Business, Economies, Societies, and Nations. New York, NY: Doubleday Books.Google Scholar
Tetlock, P. E. & Gardner, D. (2015). Superforecasting: The Art and Science of Prediction. New York, NY: Crown.Google Scholar
U.S. Department of Housing and Urban Development. (2013). Housing discrimination against racial and ethnic minorities 2012. Washington, DC. Retrieved from: www.huduser.gov/portal/Publications/pdf/HUD-514_HDS2012.pdfGoogle Scholar
Vuletich, H. A., & Payne, B. K. (2019). Stability and change in implicit bias. Psychological Science, 30, 854862. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797619844270CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Webb, T. L., Sheeran, P., & Pepper, J. (2012). Gaining control over responses to implicit attitude tests: Implementation intentions engender fast responses on attitude-incongruent trials. British Journal of Social Psychology, 51, 1332. https://doi.org/10.1348/014466610X532192CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Westgate, E., Riskind, R. G., & Nosek, B. A. (2015). Implicit preferences for straight people over lesbian women and gay men weakened from 2006 to 2013. Collabra, 1, 110. https://doi.org/10.1525/collabra.18CrossRefGoogle Scholar
White, D. (2016, October 5). Read a transcript of the vice-presidential debate. Time. Retrieved from: http://time.com/4517096/vice-presidential-debate-kaine-pence-transcript/Google Scholar
Wittenbrink, B., Judd, C. M., & Park, B. (1997). Evidence for racial prejudice at the implicit level and its relationships with questionnaire measures. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 262274. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.72.2.262CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yogeeswaran, K., & Dasgupta, N. (2010). Will the “real” American please stand up? The effect of implicit stereotypes about nationality on discriminatory behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 36, 13321345. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167210380928CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zerhouni, O., Rougier, M., & Muller, D. (2016). “Who (really) is Charlie?”: French cities with lower implicit prejudice toward Arabs demonstrated larger participation rates in Charlie Hebdo rallies. International Review of Social Psychology, 29, 6976, https://doi.org/10.5334/irsp.50CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×