Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-gb8f7 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-26T01:48:49.990Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false
This chapter is part of a book that is no longer available to purchase from Cambridge Core

26 - Music

from PART V.B - ARTS, SPORTS, & MOTOR SKILLS

Andreas C. Lehmann
Affiliation:
Hochschule fuer Musik Wuerzburg
Hans Gruber
Affiliation:
Institute for Education, University of Regensburg
K. Anders Ericsson
Affiliation:
Florida State University
Neil Charness
Affiliation:
Florida State University
Paul J. Feltovich
Affiliation:
University of West Florida
Robert R. Hoffman
Affiliation:
University of West Florida
Get access

Summary

Research Approaches to Individual Differences in Music

Individual differences in musical achievement have at all times awed musicians and audiences alike. In former times, royalty and nobility invited outstanding musicians to perform in their salons. Today, the general public crowds the concert halls when certain celebrities perform while other concerts are scarcely attended. Sometimes, special attractions such as child prodigies or musical savants capture the attention of the mass media.

Scientific attempts to understand individual differences have existed for a long time. Barrington (1770) investigated Mozart's early performance achievements and described it in some detail. Such single-case studies are highly informative. However, they usually do not suffice for modern scientific standards. They merely document high achievements under controlled conditions and attribute them to exceptional levels of talent. Doubts can be expressed about the accuracy and reliability of the information presented in biographies of famous musicians. More recent biographies mention skill acquisition explicitly from the perspective of musical talent research. This research explains exceptional performance as based in innate musical capacities.

Billroth's (1895) “Who is musical?” can be seen as a starting point for research on musical abilities in the 19th century. Later in the 1920s and 30s Seashore developed his “Measures of musical talents”, which assessed subjects' perceptual discrimination abilities (Seashore, 1938/1967). With few exceptions, such as Wellek's attempt in 1939 to identify racial differences in musical abilities (Wellek, 1970), most music aptitude tests have tried to predict the potential for music performance (Boyle, 1992, for a review).

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2006

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Ackerman, P. L. (1986). Individual differences in information processing: An investigation of intellectual abilities and task performance during practice. Intelligence, 10, 101–139.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ackerman, P. L. (1990). A correlational analysis of skill specificity: Learning, abilities, and individual differences. JEP: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 16, 883–901.Google Scholar
Barrington, D. (1770). Account of a very remarkable young musician. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, 60, 4–64.Google Scholar
Barry, N., & Hallam, S. (2001). Practice. In Parncutt, R. & McPherson, G. (Eds.), Science and psychology of music performance (pp. 151–166). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Berliner, P. (1994). Thinking in jazz. Chicago: Chicago University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Besson, M. (1997). Electrophysiological studies of music processing. In Deliege, I. & Sloboda, J. (Eds.), Perception and cognition of music (pp. 217–250). London: Taylor & Francis.Google Scholar
Billroth, T. (1895). Wer ist musikalisch? Nachgelassene Schrift. [Who is musical?] (Ed. by Hanslick, E.). Berlin: Paetel.Google Scholar
Bloom, B. S. (1985). Generalizations about talent development. In Bloom, B. S. (Ed.), Developing talent in young people (pp. 507–549). New York: Ballantine.Google Scholar
Boshuizen, H. P. A., & Schmidt, H. G. (1992). On the role of biomedical knowledge in clinical reasoning by experts, intermediates and novices. Cognitive Science, 16, 153–184.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boyle, J. D. (1992). Evaluation of music ability. In Colwell, R. (Ed.), Handbook of research in music teaching and learning (pp. 247–265). New York: Schirmer.Google Scholar
Brandfonbrener, A., & Lederman, R. (2002). Performing arts medicine. In Colwell, R. & Richardson, C. (Eds.), The new handbook of research on music teaching and learning (pp. 1009–1022). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Chaffin, R., & Imreh, G. (2001). A comparison of practice and self-report as sources of information about the goals of expert practice. Psychology of Music, 29, 39–69.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chaffin, R., Imreh, G., & Crawford, M. (2002). Practicing perfection: Memory and piano performance. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google ScholarPubMed
Charness, N., Clifton, J., & MacDonald, L. (1988). Case study of a musical mono-savant. In Obler, L. & Fein, D. (Eds.), The exceptional brain: Neuropsychology of talent and special abilities (pp. 277–293). New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
Colley, A., Banton, L., & Down, J. (1992). An expert-novice comparison in musical composition. Psychology of Music, 20, 124–137.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Csikszentmihalyi, M., Rathunde, K., & Whalen, S. (1993). Talented teenagers: The roots of success or failure. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Davidson, J. W., Howe, M. J. A., Moore, D. G., & Sloboda, J. A. (1996). The role of parental influences in the development of musical ability. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 14, 399–412.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Easton, C. (1989). Jacqueline du Pre: A biography. New York: Summit.Google Scholar
Elbert, T., Candia, V., Altenmüller, E. O., Rau, H., Sterr, A., Rockstroh, B., Pantev, C., & Taub, E. (1998). Alteration of digital representations in somatosensory cortex in focal hand dystonia. NeuroReport, 9, 3571–3575.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Elbert, T., Pantev, C., Wienbruch, C., Rockstroh, B., & Taub, E. (1995). Increased cortical representation of the fingers of the left hand in string players. Science, 270, 305–307.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ericsson, K. A. (2003). The search for general abilities and basic capacities. In Sternberg, R. J. & Grigorenko, E. L. (Eds.), The psychology of abilities, competencies, and expertise (pp. 93–125). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ericsson, K. A., & Crutcher, R. J. (1990). The nature of exceptional performance. In Baltes, P. B., Featherman, D. L., & Lerner, R. M. (Eds.), Life-span development and behavior (Vol. 10, pp. 187–217). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Ericsson, K. A., & Kintsch, W. (1995). Long-term working memory. Psychological Review, 102, 211–245.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ericsson, K. A., Krampe, R. T., & Tesch-Römer, C. (1993). The role of deliberate practice in the acquisition of expert performance. Psychological Review, 100, 363–406.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ericsson, K. A., & Lehmann, A. C. (1996). Expert and exceptional performance: Evidence of maximal adaptations to task constraints. Annual Review of Psychology, 47, 273–305.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Farnsworth, P. R. (1969). The social psychology of music. Ames: Iowa University Press.Google Scholar
Fitts, P. M., & Posner, M. I. (1967). Human performance. Belmont: Brooks/Cole.Google Scholar
Fiz, J. A., Aguilar, J., Carreras, A., Teixido, A., Haro, M., Rodenstein, D., & Morera, J. (1993). Maximum respiratory pressures in trumpet players. Chest, 104, 1203–1204.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Galton, F. (1979). Hereditary genius: An inquiry into its laws and consequences. London: Friedman. (Original published 1869.) Google Scholar
Gaser, C., & Schlaug, G. (2003). Gray matter differences between musicians and nonmusicians. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 999, 514–517.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gembris, H. (1998). Musikalische Begabung und Entwicklung. [Musical ability and development] Augsburg: Wissner.Google Scholar
Goolsby, T. W. (1994). Profiles of processing: Eye movements during sightreading. Music Perception, 12, 97–123.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Green, L. (2002). How popular musicians learn. London: Ashgate.Google Scholar
Gruber, H., Degner, S., & Lehmann, A. C. (2004). Why do some commit themselves in deliberate practice for many years – and so many do not? Understanding the development of professionalism in music. In Radovan, M. & Dordević, N. (Eds.), Current issues in adult learning and motivation (pp. 222–235). Ljubljana: Slovenian Institute for Adult Education.Google Scholar
Gruber, H., Weber, A., & Ziegler, A. (1996). Einsatzmöglichkeiten retrospektiver Befragungen bei der Untersuchung des Expertiseerwerbs. [The use of retrospective inquiry in the study of expertise acquisition] In Gruber, H. & Ziegler, A. (Eds.), Expertiseforschung. Theoretische und methodische Grundlagen (pp. 169–190). Opladen: Westdeutscher Verlag.Google Scholar
Gruhn, W., & Rauscher, F. (2002). The neurobiology of music cognition and learning. In Colwell, R. & Richardson, C. (Eds.), The new handbook of research on music teaching and learning (pp. 445–460). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Gruson, L. M. (1988). Rehearsal skill and musical competence: Does practice make perfect? In Sloboda, J. A. (Ed.), Generative processes in music (pp. 91–112). Oxford: Clarendon.Google Scholar
Hallam, S. (1995). Professional musicians' approaches to the learning and interpretation of music. Psychology of Music, 23, 111–128.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hargreaves, D., Cork, C., & Setton, T. (1991). Cognitive strategies in jazz improvisation: An exploratory study. Canadian Journal of Research in Music Education, 33, 47–54.Google Scholar
Hayes, J. R. (1989). The complete problem solver. (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Houtsma, A. J., Durlach, N. I., & Horowitz, D. M. (1987). Comparative learning of pitch and loudness identification. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 81, 129–132.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
J⊘rgensen, H. (1997). Time for practising? In J⊘rgensen, H. & Lehmann, A. C. (Eds.), Does practice make perfect? (pp. 123–140). Oslo: Norges Musikhogskole.Google Scholar
Kauffman, W. H., & Carlsen, J. C. (1989). Memory for intact music works: The importance for musical expertise and retention interval. Psychomusicology, 8, 3–19.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keele, S., Pokorny, R., Corcos, D., & Ivry, R. (1985). Do perception and motor production share a common timing mechanism? Acta Psychologica, 60, 173–193.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kopiez, R. (1998). “Singers are late beginners”: Sängerbiographien aus Sicht der Expertiseforschung. Eine Schwachstellenanalyse. [Singers' biographies from the perspective of research on expertise. An analysis of weaknesses] In Gembris, H., Kraemer, R., & Maas, G. (Eds.), Singen als Gegenstand der Grundlagenforschung (pp. 37–56). Augsburg: Wissner.Google Scholar
Kopiez, R., Langner, J., & Steinhagen, P. (1999). Afrikanische Trommler (Ghana) bewerten und spielen europäische Rhythmen African dicummers assess and play European rhythms. Musicae Scientiae, 3, 139–160.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krampe, R. T., & Ericsson, K. A. (1996). Maintaining excellence: Deliberate practice and elite performance in young and older pianists. JEP: General, 125, 331–359.Google Scholar
Lehmann, A. C. (1997a). Acquired mental representations in music performance: Anecdotal and preliminary empirical evidence. In J⊘rgensen, H. & Lehmann, A. C. (Eds.), Does practice make perfect? (pp. 141–164). Oslo: Norges Musikhogskole.Google Scholar
Lehmann, A. C. (1997b). Acquisition of expertise in music: Efficiency of deliberate practice as a moderating variable in accounting for sub-expert performance. In Deliège, I. & Sloboda, J. A. (Eds.), Perception and cognition of music (pp. 161–190). London: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Lehmann, A. C. (2002). Effort and enjoyment in deliberate practice: A research note. In Hanken, I. M., Nielsen, S. G., & Nerland, M. (Eds.), Research in and for music education. Festschrift for Harald J⊘rgensen (pp. 153–166). Oslo: Norwegian Academy of Music.Google Scholar
Lehmann, A. C., & Ericsson, K. A. (1996). Music performance without preparation: Structure and acquisition of expert sight-reading. Psychomusicology, 15, 1–29.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lehmann, A. C., & Ericsson, K. A. (1997). Research on expert performance and deliberate practice: Implications for the education of amateur musicians and music students. Psychomusicology, 16, 40–58.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lehmann, A. C., & Ericsson, K. A. (1998). The historical development of domains of expertise: Performance standards and innovations in music. In Steptoe, A. (Ed.), Genius and the mind: Studies of creativity and temperament in the historical record (pp. 64–97). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Lehmann, A. C., & Papousek, S. (2003). Self-reported performance goals predict actual practice behavior among adult piano beginners. In Kopiez, R., Lehmann, A. C., Wolther, I., & Wolf, C. (Eds.), Proceedings of the 5th Triennial Conference of the European Society for the Cognitive Sciences of Music (pp. 389–392). Hannover: University of Music and Drama.Google Scholar
Lim, V., & Altenmüller, E. O. (2003). Musicians' cramp: Instrumental and gender differences. Medical Problems of Performing Artists, 18, 21–27.Google Scholar
Mach, E. (1981). Great pianists speak for themselves. London: Robson.Google Scholar
Manturzewska, M. (1995). A biographical study of the life-span development of professional musicians. In Manturzewska, M., Miklaszewski, K., & Bialkowski, A. (Eds.), Psychology of music today (pp. 311–337). Warsaw: Fryderyk Chopin Music Academy.Google Scholar
Matthay, T. (1926). On memorizing and playing from memory and on the laws of practice generally. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
McPherson, G. E., & Davidson, J. W. (2002). Musical practice: Mother and child interactions during the first year of learning an instrument. Music Education Research, 4, 141–156.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McPherson, G. E., & Zimmerman, B. J. (2002). Self-regulation of musical learning: A social cognitive perspective. In Colwell, R. & Richardson, C. (Eds.), The new handbook of research on music teaching and learning (pp. 327–347). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Miller, L. K. (1999). The savant syndrome: Intellectual impairment and exceptional skill. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 31–46.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Münte, T. F., Altenmüller, E. O., & Jäncke, L. (2002). The musician's brain as a model of neuroplasticity. Nature Reviews: Neuroscience, 3, 473–478.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Münzer, S., Berti, S., & Pechmann, T. (2002). Encoding timbre, speech, and tones: Musicians vs. Non-musicians. Psychologische Beiträge, 44, 187–202.Google Scholar
Palmer, C. (1997). Music performance. Annual Review of Psychology, 48, 115–138.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pantev, C., Roberts, L. E., Schulz, M., Engelien, A., & Ross, B. (2001). Timbre-specific enhancements of auditory cortical representations in musicians. Neuroreport 12, 169–174.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ragert, P., Schmidt, A., Altenmüller, E. O., & Dinse, R. (2004). Superior tactile performance and learning in professional pianists: Evidence for metaplasticity in musicians. European Journal of Neuroscience, 19, 473–478.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rauscher, F. H., & Hinton, S. C. (2003). Type of music training selectively influences perceptual processing. In Kopiez, R., Lehmann, A., Wolther, I., & Wolf, C. (Eds.), Proceedings of the 5th Triennial Conference of the European Society for the Cognitive Sciences of Music (pp. 89–92). Hannover: University of Music and Drama.Google Scholar
Renwick, J., & McPherson, G. E. (2002). Interest and choice: Student-selected repertoire and its effect on practising behavior. British Journal of Music Education, 19, 173–188.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Seashore, (1938/1967). The psychology of music. New York: Dover.Google Scholar
Shuter-Dyson, R. (1999). Musical ability. In Deutsch, D. (Ed.), The psychology of music (2nd ed., pp. 627–652). San Diego: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Sloboda, J. A., Davidson, J. W., Howe, M. J. A., & Moore, D. G. (1996). The role of practice in the development of performing musicians. British Journal of Psychology, 87, 287–309.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sloboda, J. A., Hermelin, B., & O'Connor, N. (1985). An exceptional musical memory. Music Perception, 3, 155–170.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sosniak, L. A. (1985). Learning to be a concert pianist. In Bloom, B. S. (Ed.), Developing talent in young people (pp. 19–67). New York: Ballantine.Google Scholar
Sudnow, D. (1993). Ways of the hand: The organisation of improvised conduct. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Sundberg, J. (1987). The science of the singing voice. De Kalb, IL: Northern Illinois University Press.Google Scholar
Vitouch, O. (2005). Erwerb musikalischer Expertise. [Acquisition of musical expertise] In Stoffer, T. H. & Oerter, R. (Eds.), Allgemeine Musikpsychologie (pp. 657–715). Göttingen: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Wagner, C. (1988). The pianist's hand: Anthropometry and biomechanics. Ergonomics, 31, 97–131.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weisberg, R. W. (1999). Creativity and knowledge. In Sternberg, R. J. (Ed.), Handbook of creativity (pp. 226–250). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Wellek, A. (1970). Typologie der Musikbegabung im deutschen Volke [Typology of musical abilities in the German people]. München: Beck. (Original published 1939.)Google Scholar
Williamon, A. (Ed.). (2004). Musical excellence: Strategies and techniques to enhance performance. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Williamon, A., & Valentine, E. (2000). Quantity and quality of musical practice as predictors of performance quality. British Journal of Psychology, 91, 353–376.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Winner, E. (1996). The rage to master: The decisive role of talent in the visual arts. In Ericsson, K. A. (Ed.), The road to excellence (pp. 271–302). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Woody, R. H. (1999). The relationship between advanced musicians' explicit planning and their expressive performance of dynamic variations in an aural modeling task. Journal of Research in Music Education, 47, 331–342.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Woody, R. H. (2003). Explaining expressive performance: Component cognitive skills in an aural modeling task. Journal of Research in Music Education, 51(1), 51–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×