Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-l7hp2 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-22T15:58:31.333Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false
This chapter is part of a book that is no longer available to purchase from Cambridge Core

19 - Expertise in Medicine and Surgery

from PART V.A - PROFESSIONAL DOMAINS

Geoff Norman
Affiliation:
Clinical Epidemiology and Biostatistics, Faculty of Health Sciences, McMaster University
Kevin Eva
Affiliation:
Clinical Epidemiology and Biostatistics, Faculty of Health Sciences, McMaster University
Lee Brooks
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, McMaster University
Stan Hamstra
Affiliation:
Department of Surgery, University of Toronto
K. Anders Ericsson
Affiliation:
Florida State University
Neil Charness
Affiliation:
Florida State University
Paul J. Feltovich
Affiliation:
University of West Florida
Robert R. Hoffman
Affiliation:
University of West Florida
Get access

Summary

Introduction

Expertise in medicine requires mastery of a diversity of knowledge and skills – motor, cognitive, and interpersonal – which makes it unlike many other fields of expertise, such as chess, bridge, computer programming, or gymnastics. Although some specialties such as pathology or surgery may emphasize one kind of skill or another, most clinicians must be skilled in all domains and must also master an enormous knowledge base drawn from areas as diverse as molecular biology, ethics, and psychology.

Perhaps paradoxically, despite the considerable effort required to achieve mastery, there is no formal equivalent of elite performance, which has been a topic of many other chapters in this book. Though there is stiff competition to enter medical school and only about 15% of Canadian applicants get a position, once in, better than 95% will graduate, get placement in a specialty (residency) program, and enter practice. Once certified competent, competition in practice is absent. Medicine has its legendary clinicians, but these are as rare as Olympic gold medalists and have not been systematically studied.

That is not to say that there are no measures of relative expertise. In some domains, particularly surgery, treatment success can be measured with indicators such as death, complications, or blood loss, and has been linked to physician characteristics like specialty certification (Ericsson, 2004; Norcini et al., 2002) and undergraduate training (Tamblyn et al., 2002).

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2006

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Anastakis, D. J., Regehr, G., Reznick, R. K., Cusimano, M. D., Murnaghan, J., Brown, M., & Hutchison, C. (1999). Assessment of technical skills transfer from the bench training model to the human model. American Journal of Surgery, 177, 167–170.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ark, T. K., Brooks, L. R., & Eva, K. W. (2006). Giving learners the best of both worlds: Do clinical teachers need to guard against teaching pattern recognition to novices? Academic Medicine; In press.
Avrahami, J., Kareev, Y., Bogot, Y., Caspi, R. et al. (1997). Teaching by examples: Implications for the process of category acquisition. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 50A, 586–606.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anderson, J. R. (1980). Cognitive psychology and its implications (p. 292).San Francisco: W. H. Freeman.Google Scholar
Barrows, H. S., Norman, G. R., Neufeld, V. R., & Feightner, J. W. (1982). The clinical reasoning process of randomly selected physicians in general medical practice. Clinical and Investigative Medicine, 5, 49–56.Google ScholarPubMed
Bordage, G., & Zacks, R. (1984). The structure and knowledge in the memories of medical students and general practitioners: Categories and prototypes. Medical Education, 18, 406–416.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bordage, G., & Lemieux, M. (1991). Semantic structures and diagnostic thinking of experts and novices. Academic Medicine, 66, S70–S72.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bordage, G., Connell, K. J., Chang, R. W., Gecht, M. R., & Sinacore, J. M. (1997). Assessing the semantic content of clinical case presentations: Studies of reliability and concurrent validity. Academic Medicine, 72, S37–S39.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boshuizen, H. P. A., & Schmidt, H. G. (1992). Biomedical knowledge and clinical expertise. Cognitive Science, 16, 153–184.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brooks, L. R. (1978). Decentralized control of categorization: The role of prior processing episodes. In Neisser, U. (Ed.), Concepts and conceptual development (pp. 141–174). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Brooks, L. R., Norman, G. R., & Allen, S. W. (1991). The role of specific similarity in a medical diagnostic task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 120, 278–287.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burns, B. D. (2004). The effects of speed on skilled chess performance. Psychological Science, 15, 442–447.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Caulford, P. G., Lamb, S. B., Kaigas, T. B., Hanna, E., Norman, G. R., & Davis, D. A. (1994). Physician incompetence: Specific problems and predictors. Academic Medicine, 69, S16–S18.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Choudhry, N. K., Fletcher, R. H., & Soumerai, S. B. (2005). Systematic review: The relationship between clinical experience and quality of health care. Annals of Internal Medicine, 142, 260–273.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Coderre, S., Mandin, H., Harasym, P. H., & Fick, G. H. (2003). Diagnostic reasoning strategies and diagnostic success. Medical Education, 37, 695–703.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Coughlin, L. D., & Patel, V. L. (1987). Processing of critical information by physicians and medical students. Journal of Medical Education, 62, 818–828.Google ScholarPubMed
Coulson, R. L., Feltovich, P. J., & Spiro, R. J. (1997). Cognitive flexibility in medicine: An application to the recognition and understanding of hypertension. Advances in Health Sciences Education, 2, 141–161.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cuschieri, A. (1995). Visual displays and visual perception in minimal access surgery. Seminars in Laparoscopic Surgery, 2, 209–214.Google ScholarPubMed
Custers, E. J., Boshuizen, H. P., & Schmidt, H. G. (1996). The influence of medical expertise, case typicality and illness script component on case processing and disease probability estimates. Memory and Cognition, 24, 384–399.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Groot, A. D. (1978). Thought and choice in chess. The Hague: Mouton.Google Scholar
Elstein, A. S., Shulman, L. S., & Sprafka, S. A. (1978). Medical problem solving: An analysis of clinical reasoning. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ericsson, K. A. (2000). How experts attain and maintain superior performance: Implications for the enhancement of skilled performance in older individuals. Journal of Aging and Physical Activity, 8, 346–352.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ericsson, K. A. (2004). Deliberate practice and the acquisition and maintenance of expert performance in medicine and related domains. Academic Medicine, 79, S70–S81.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eva, K. W. (2002). The aging physician: Changes in cognitive processing and their influence on medical practice. Academic Medicine, 77, S1–S6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eva, K. W., Brooks, L. R., & Norman, G. R. (2001). Does ‘shortness of breath’ = ‘dyspnea’: The biasing effect of feature instantiation in medical diagnosis. Academic Medicine, 76, S11–S13.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eva, K. W., Norman, G. R., & Brooks, L. R. (2002). Forward reasoning as a hallmark of expertise in medicine: logical, psychological, phenomenological inconsistencies. Advances in Psychology Research, 8, 25–40.Google Scholar
Eva, K. W., Norman, G. R., Neville, A. J., Wood, T. J., & Brooks, L. R. (2002). Expert / novice differences in memory: A reformulation. Teaching and Learning in Medicine, 14, 257–263.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Feltovich, P. J., Johnson, P. E., Moller, J. H., & Swanson, D. B. (1984). The role and development of medical knowledge in diagnostic expertise. In Clancey, W. J. & Shortliffe, E. H. (Eds.), Readings in medical artificial intelligence: The first decade. Reading, MA: Addison Wesley.Google Scholar
Feltovich, P. J., & Barrows, H. S. (1984). Issues of generality in medical problem solving. In Schmidt, H. G. & DeVolder, M. L. (Eds.), Tutorials in problem-based learning. Assen, the Netherlands: Van Gorcum.Google Scholar
Feltovich, P. J., Coulson, R. L., & Spiro, R. J. (1997). Issues of expert flexibilty in contexts characterized by complexity and change. In Feltovich, P. J., Ford, K. D., & Hoffman, R. R. (Eds.), Expertise in context: Human and machine. Menlo Park, CA: AAAI / MIT Press.Google Scholar
Francis, N. K., Hanna, G. B., Cresswell, A. B., Carter, F. J., & Cuschieri, A. (2001). The performance of master surgeons on standard aptitude testing. American Journal of Surgery, 182, 30–33.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gobet, F., & Simon, H. A. (2000). Five seconds or sixty? Presentation time in expert memory. Cognitive Science, 24, 651–682.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grace, D. M. (1989). Aptitude testing in surgery. Canadian Journal of Surgery, 32, 396–397.Google ScholarPubMed
Grober, E. D., Hamstra, S. J., Wanzel, K. R., Reznick, R. K., Matsumoto, E. D., Sidhu, R. S., & Jarvi, K. A. (2004). The educational impact of bench model fidelity on the acquisition of technical skill: The use of clinically relevant outcome measures. Annals of Surgery, 240, 374–381.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gruppen, L. D., & Frohna, A. Z. (2002). Clinical reasoning. In Norman, G. R., Newble, D. I., & Vleuten, C. P. M (Eds.), International Handbook of Research in Medical Education (pp. 205–230). Dordrecht: Kluwer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Halm, E. A., Lee, C., & Chassin, M. R. (2002). Is volume related to outcome in health care? A systematic review and methodological critique of the literature. Annals of Internal Medicine, 137, 511–520.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hashem, A., Chi, M. T. H., & Friedman, C. P. (2003). Medical errors as a result of specialization. Journal of Biomedical Informatics, 36, 61–69.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hassebrock, F., Bullemer, P., & Johnson, P. E. (1988). When less is more: Selective memory of problem-solving experts. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Educational Research Association, New Orleans.
Hatala, R. M., Norman, G. R., & Brooks, L. R. (1999). Influence of a single example upon subsequent electrocardiogram interpretation. Teaching and Learning in Medicine, 11, 110–117.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hatala, R. M., Norman, G. R., & Brooks, L. R. (2003). Practice makes perfect: The critical role of deliberate practice in the acquisition of ECG interpretation skills. Advances in Health Sciences Education, 8, 17–26.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Higgins, M. P., & Tully, M. P. (2005). Hospital doctors and their schemas about appropriate prescribing. Medical Education, 39, 184–193.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hobus, P. P., Schmidt, H. G., Boshuizen, H. P., & Patel, V. L. (1987). Contextual factors in the activation of first diagnostic hypotheses: Expert-novice differences. Medical Education, 21, 471–476.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keck, J. W., Arnold, L., Willoughby, L., & Calkins, V. (1979). Efficacy of cognitive/noncognitive measures in predicting resident-physician performance. Journal of Medical Education, 54, 759–65.Google ScholarPubMed
Kim, N. S., & Ahn, W. (2002). The influence of naïve causal theories an lay concepts of mental illness. American Journal of Psychology, 115, 33–65.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kintsch, W. (1974). The representation of meaning in memory. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Kulatanga-Moruzi, C., Brooks, L. R., & Norman, G. R. (2001). Coordination of analytical and similarity based processing strategies and expertise in dermatological diagnosis. Teaching and Learning in Medicine, 13, 110–116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kulatanga-Moruzi, C., Brooks, L. R., & Norman, G. R. (2004). The diagnostic disadvantage of having all the facts: Using comprehensive feature lists to bias medical diagnosis. Journal of Experimental Pychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 30, 563–572.Google Scholar
Matsumoto, E. D., Hamstra, S. J., Radomski, S. B., & Cusimano, M. D. (2002). The effect of bench model fidelity on endourologic skills: A randomized controlled study. Journal of Urology, 167, 1243–1247.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Medin, D. L., Altom, M. W., & Murphy, T. D. (1984). Given versus induced category representations: Use of prototype and exemplar information in classification. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 10, 333–352.Google ScholarPubMed
Moorthy, K., Munz, Y., Jiwanji, M., Bann, S., Chang, A., & Darzi, A. (2004). Validity and reliability of a virtual reality upper gastrointestinal simulator and cross validation using structured assessment of individual performance with video playback. Surgical Endoscopy, 18, 328–333.Google ScholarPubMed
Muzzin, L. J., Norman, G. R., Feightner, J. W., & Tugwell, P. (1983). Expertise in recall of clinical protocols in two specialty areas. Proceedings of the 22nd Conference on Research in Medical Education (pp. 122–127). Washington, DC: Association of American Medical Colleges.Google Scholar
Naik, V. N., Matsumoto, E. D., Houston, P. L., Hamstra, S. J., Yeung, R. Y. M., Mallon, J. S., & Martire, T. M. (2001). Fiberoptic orotracheal intubation on anesthetized patients: Do manipulation skills learned on a simple model transfer into the operating room? Anesthesiology, 95, 343–348.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nendaz, M. R., & Bordage, G. (2002). Promoting diagnostic problem representation. Medical Education, 36, 760–766.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Neufeld, V. R., Norman, G. R., Barrows, H. S., & Feightner, J. W. (1981). Clinical problem solving by medical students: A longitudinal and cross-sectional analysis. Medical Education, 15, 315–322.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Norcini, J. J., Lipner, R. S., & Kimball, H. R. (2002). Certifying examination performance and patient outcomes following acute myocardial infarction. Medical Education, 36, 853–859.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Norman, G. R., Brooks, L. R., & Allen, S. W. (1989). Recall by experts and novices as a record of processing attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 15, 1166–1174.Google ScholarPubMed
Norman, G. R., Trott, A. L., Brooks, L. R., & Smith, E. K. M. (1994). Cognitive differences in clinical reasoning related to postgraduate training. Teaching and Learning in Medicine, 6, 114–120.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Papp, K. K., Polk, H. C. J., & Richardson, J. D. (1997). The relationship between criteria used to select residents and performance during residency. American Journal of Surgery, 173, 326–329.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Patel, V. L., & Groen, G. J. (1986), Knowledge based solution strategies in medical reasoning. Cognitive Science, 10, 91–115.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Patel, V. L., Evans, D. A., & Groen, G. J. (1989). Biomedical knowledge and clinical reasoning. In Evans, D. A. & Patel, V. L. (Eds.), Cognitive science in medicine: Biomedical modelling (pp. 127–166). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Regehr, G., Cline, J., Norman, G. R., & Brooks, L. R. (1994). Effect of processing strategy on diagnostic skill in dermatology. Academic Medicine, , S34–S36.Google Scholar
Rikers, R. M., Loyens, S. M., & Schmidt, H. G. (2004). The role of encapsulated knowledge in clinical case representations of medical students and family doctors. Medical Education, 38, 1035–1043.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Risucci, D., Geiss, A., Gellman, L., Pinard, B., & Rosser, J. (2001). Surgeon-specific factors in the acquisition of laparoscopic surgical skills. American Journal of Surgery, 181, 289–293.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rosch, E. H., & Mervis, C. B. (1976). Family resemblances: Studies in the internal structure of categories. Cognitive Psychology, 7, 573–605.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schueneman, A. L., Pickleman, J., & Freeark, R. J. (1985). Age, gender, lateral dominance, and prediction of operative skill among general surgery residents. Surgery, 98, 506–515.Google ScholarPubMed
Schmidt, H. G., & Boshuizen, H. P. A. (1993). On the origin of intermediate effects in clinical case recall. Memory and Cognition, 21, 338–351.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schmidt, H. G., Norman, G. R., & Boshuizen, H. P. (1990). A cognitive perspective on medical expertise: Theory and implications. Academic Medicine, 65, 611–621.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schuwirth, L., Gorter, S., Heijde, D., Rethans, J. J., Brauer, J., Houben, H., Linden, S., Vleuten, C., & Scherpbier, A. (2005). The role of a computerized case-based testing procedure in practice performance assessment. Advances in Health Sciences Education, 10, 145–155.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scott, D. J., Bergen, P. C., Rege, R. V., Laycock, Z. R., Tesfay, S. T., & Valentine, R. J., Euhus, D. M., Jeyarajah, D. R., Thompson, W. M., & Jones, D. B. (2000). Laparoscopic training on bench models: Better and more cost effective than operating room experience? Journal of the American College of Surgeons, 191, 272–283.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Simon, H. A., & Chase, W. G. (1973). Skill at chess. American Scientist, 61, 394–403.Google Scholar
Squire, D., Giachino, A. A., Profitt, A. W., & Heaney, C. (1989). Objective comparison of manual dexterity in physicians and surgeons. Canadian Journal of Surgery, 32, 467–470.Google ScholarPubMed
Steele, R. J., Walder, C., & Herbert, M. (1992). Psychomotor testing and the ability to perform an anastomosis in junior surgical trainees. British Journal of Surgery, 79, 1065–1067.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sternberg, R. J., & Frensch, P. A. (1992). On being an expert: A cost-benefit analysis. In Hoffman, R. (Ed.), The psychology of expertise: Cognitive research and empirical artificial intelligence. New York: Springer-Verlag.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tamblyn, R. M., Abrahamowicz, M., Dauphinee, W. D., Hanley, J. A., Norcini, J., Girard, N., Grand'Maison, P., & Brialovsky, C. (2002). Association between licensure examination scores and practice in primary care. Journal of the American Medical Association, 288, 3019–3026.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Turnbull, J., Carbotte, R., Hanna, E., Norman, G. R. et al. (2000). Cognitive difficulty in physicians. Academic Medicine, 75, 177–181.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van Rossum, H. J. M., & Bender, W. W. (1990). What can be learned from a boy with acute appendicitis? Persistent effects of a case presentation on the diagnostic judgment of family doctors. Presented at the Fourth Ottawa Conference, Ottawa, Ontario.
Verkoeijn, P. P., Rikers, R. M., Schmidt, H. G., Wiel, M. W., & Kooman, J. P. (2004). Case representation by medical experts, intermediates and novices for laboratory data presented with or without a clinical context. Medical Education, 38, 617–627.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wanzel, K. R., Hamstra, S. J., Anastakis, D. J., Matsumoto, E. D., & Cusimano, M. D. (2002). Effect of visual-spatial ability on learning of spatially-complex surgical skills. Lancet, 359, 230–231.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wanzel, K. R., Hamstra, S. J., Caminiti, M. F., Anastakis, D. J., Grober, E. D., & Reznick, R. K. (2003). Visual-spatial ability correlates with efficiency of hand motion and successful surgical performance. Surgery, 134, 750–757.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Woods, N. N., Brooks, L. R., & Norman, G. R. (2005). The value of basic science. Medical Education, 39, 107–112.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×