Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-t7czq Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-26T01:43:34.531Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false
This chapter is part of a book that is no longer available to purchase from Cambridge Core

40 - Aging and Expertise

from PART VI - GENERALIZABLE MECHANISMS MEDIATING EXPERTISE AND GENERAL ISSUES

Ralf Th. Krampe
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Leuven
Neil Charness
Affiliation:
Psychology Department, Florida State University
K. Anders Ericsson
Affiliation:
Florida State University
Neil Charness
Affiliation:
Florida State University
Paul J. Feltovich
Affiliation:
University of West Florida
Robert R. Hoffman
Affiliation:
University of West Florida
Get access

Summary

Introduction

Outstanding accomplishments by older individuals, such as the wisdom of elderly statesmen, the virtuoso performances of older musicians, or the swan-song oeuvres of famous composers have been the subject of admiration throughout human history. Commonsense or folk psychology rarely considers such achievements as incompatible with older age. On the contrary, in the public's opinion advanced age has been identified with maturity or heightened levels of experience that complement the exceptional talents or gifts that had presumably enabled outstanding individuals to surpass ordinary people in the first place. Allegedly, these dispositions are the driving force leading to high achievements, and the presumed stability of related capacities is believed to guarantee that outstanding individuals' superior skills remain at their disposition throughout adulthood. In traditionalist cultures (as in Germany or Japan) such appreciations of early achievement and seniority overshadow actual accomplishments and remain an integral part of society and job promotion until this day.

The scientific study of interindividual differences and the experimental investigation of human performance in normal adults portray a less optimistic picture of adult development, at least in the normal population. Ubiquitous findings of negative age-graded changes in psychometric ability factors and reduced speed or accuracy in most cognitive-motor tasks have motivated theories of broad decline, like the notion of general, age-related slowing (Salthouse, 1985a). In the light of these findings, the accomplishments by older experts and the high performance levels in many older professionals present a puzzle.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2006

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Ackerman, P. L. (1988). Determinants of individual differences during skill acquisition: Cognitive abilities and information processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 117, 288–318.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Allaire, J. C., & Marsiske, M. (1999). Everyday cognition: Age and intellectual ability correlates. Psychology and Aging, 14, 627–644.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Allaire, J. C., & Marsiske, M. (2002). Well- and ill-defined measures of everyday cognition: Relationship to elders' intellectual ability and functional status. Psychology and Aging, 17, 101–115.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anderson, J. R. (1982). Acquisition of cognitive skill. Psychological Review, 89, 369–406.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baird, L. L. (1985). Do grades and tests predict adult accomplishment? Research in Higher Education, 23, 3–85.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baltes, P. B., & Baltes, M. M. (1990). Psychological perspectives on successful aging: The model of selective optimization with compensation. In Baltes, P. B. & Baltes, M. M. (Eds.), Successful aging: Perspectives from the behavioral sciences (pp. 1–34). Cambridge, NY: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Book, W. F. (1924). Voluntary motor ability of the world's champion typists. Journal of Applied Psychology, 8, 283–308.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bosman, E. A. (1993). Age-related differences in the motoric aspects of transcription typing skill. Psychology and Aging, 8, 87–102.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Callahan, J. S., Kiker, D. S., & Cross, T. (2003). Does method matter? A meta-analysis of the effects of training method on older learner training performance. Journal of Management, 29, 663–680.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cattell, R. B. (1971). Abilities: Their structure, growth, and action. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin.Google Scholar
Cerella, J. (1985). Information processing rates in the elderly. Psychological Bulletin, 98, 67–83.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cerella, J. (1990). Aging and information processing rates in the elderly. In Birren, J. E. & Schaie, K. W. (Eds.), Handbook of the psychology of aging (3rd ed., pp. 201–221). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Cerella, J., & Fozard, J. L. (1984). Lexical access and age. Developmental Psychology, 20, 235–243.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Charness, N. (1981a). Aging and skilled problem solving. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 110, 21–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Charness, N. (1981b). Search in chess: Age and skill differences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human perception and Performance, 7, 467–476.Google Scholar
Charness, N. (1983). Age, skill, and bridge bidding: A chronometric analysis. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 22, 406–416.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Charness, N. (1989). Expertise in chess and bridge. In Klahr, D. & Kotovsky, K. (Eds.), Complex information processing: The impact of Herbert A. Simon (pp. 183–208). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Charness, N., & Bosman, E. A. (1990). Expertise and aging: Life in the lab. In Hess, T. M. (Ed.), Aging and cognition: Knowledge organization and utilization (pp. 343–385). North-Holland: Elsevier Science Publishers B.V. Google Scholar
Charness, N., Krampe, R. T., & Mayr, U. (1996). The role of practice and coaching in entrepreneurial skill domains: An international comparison of life-span chess skill acquisition. In Ericsson, K. A. (Ed.), The road to excellence: The acquisition of expert performance in the arts and sciences, sports, and games (pp. 51–80). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Charness, N., Park, D. C., & Sabel, B. A. (2001). Communication, technology and aging: Opportunities and challenges for the future. New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Charness, N., Tuffiash, M., Krampe, R., Reingold, E. M., & Vasyukova, E. (2005). The role of deliberate practice in chess expertise. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 19, 151–165.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chase, W. G., & Ericsson, K. A. (1982). Skill and working memory. In Bower, G. H. (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 16, pp. 1–58). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Clarkson-Smith, L., & Hartley, A. A. (1990). The game of bridge as an exercise in working memory and reasoning. Journal of Gerontology, 45, P233–P238.Google ScholarPubMed
Colonia-Willner, R. (1998). Practical intelligence at work: Relationship between aging and cognitive efficiency among managers in a bank environment. Psychology and Aging, 13, 45–57.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coon, H., & Carey, G. (1989). Genetic and environmental determinants of musical ability in twins. Behavioral Genetics, 19, 183–193.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Detterman, D. K., & Daniel, M. H. (1989). Correlations of mental tests with each other and with cognitive variables are highest for low IQ groups. Intelligence, 13, 349–359.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dixon, R. A., & Baltes, P. B. (1986). Toward life-span research on the functions and pragmatics of intelligence. In Sternberg, R. J. & Wagner, R. K. (Eds.), Practical intelligence: Nature and origins of competence in the everyday world (pp. 203–235). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Elo, A. E. (1965). Age changes in master chess performances. Journal of Gerontology, 20, 289–299.Google Scholar
Elo, A. E. (1986). The rating of chessplayers, past and present (2nd ed.). New York: Arco.Google Scholar
Ericsson, K. A. (1990). Peak performance and age: An examination of peak performance in sports. In Baltes, P. B. & Baltes, M. M. (Eds.), Successful aging: Perspectives from the behavioral sciences (pp. 164–196). New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ericsson, K. A., & Charness, N. (1994). Expert performance: Its structure and acquisition. American Psychologist, 49, 725–747.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ericsson, K. A., & Kintsch, W. (1995). Long-term working memory. Psychological Review, 102, 211–245.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ericsson, K. A., Krampe, R. T., & Tesch-Römer, C. (1993). The role of deliberate practice in the acquisition of expert performance. Psychological Review, 100, 363–406.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ericsson, K. A., & Lehmann, A. C. (1996). Expert and exceptional performance: Evidence on maximal adaptations on task constraints. Annual Review of Psychology, 47, 273–305.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fitts, P. (1964). Perceptual-motor skill learning. In Melton, A. W. (Ed.), Categories of human learning (pp. 243–285). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Fleishman, E. A. (1972). On the relation between abilities, learning, and human performance. American Psychologist, 27, 1017–1032.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Galton, F. Sir. (1979). Hereditary genius: An inquiry into its laws and consequences. London: Julian Friedman Publishers. (Originally published in 1869).Google Scholar
Hambrick, D. Z., & Engle, R. W. (2002). Effects of domain knowledge, working memory capacity and age on cognitive performance: An investigation of the knowledge-is-power hypothesis. Cognitive Psychology, 44, 339–387.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hambrick, D. Z., Salthouse, T. A., & Meinz, E. J. (1999). Predictors of crossword puzzle proficiency and moderators of age-cognition relations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 128, 131–164.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hasher, L., Stoltzfus, E. R., Zacks, R. T., & Rympa, B. (1991). Age and inhibition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 17, 163–169.Google Scholar
Hertzog, C. (1989). Influences of cognitive slowing on age differences in intelligence. Developmental Psychology, 25, 636–651.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hoffman, R. R., Shadbolt, N. R., Burton, A. M., & Klein, G. (1995). Eliciting knowledge from experts: A methodological analysis. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 62, 129–158.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Horn, J. L. (1982). The theory of fluid and crystallized intelligence in relation to concepts of cognitive psychology and aging in adulthood. In Craik, F. I. M. & Trehub, S. (Eds.), Aging and cognitive processes (pp. 237–278). New York: Plenum.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Horn, J. L., & Masunaga, H. (2000). New directions for research into aging and intelligence: The development of expertise. In Perfect, T. J. & Maylor, E. A. (Eds.), Models of cognitive aging. Debates in psychology. (pp. 125–159). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Howe, M. J. A., Davidson, J. W., & Sloboda, J. A. (1999). Innate talents: Reality or myth? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 21, 399–442.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hulin, C. L., Henry, R. A., & Noon, S. L. (1990). Adding a dimension: Time as a factor in the generalizability of predictive relationships. Psychological Bulletin, 107, 328–340.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kaufman, A. S. (2001). WAIS-III IQS, Horn's theory, and generational changes from young adulthood to old age. Intelligence, 29, 131–167.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keele, S. W., & Hawkins, H. L. (1982). Explorations of individual differences relevant to high level skill. Journal of Motor Behavior, 14, 3–23.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keele, S. W., Pokorny, R. A., Corcos, D. M., & Ivry, R. (1985). Do perception and motor production share common timing mechanisms: A correlational analysis. Acta Psychologica, 60, 173–191.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kliegl, R., Smith, J., & Baltes, P. B. (1989). Testing-the-limits and the study of adult age differences in cognitive plasticity of a mnemonic skill. Developmental Psychology, 25, 247–256.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Knopf, M., Preussler, W., & Stefanek, J. (1995). “18, 20, 2.” – Does expertise in skat compensate for age-related deficits in working memory? Swiss Journal of Psychology – Schweizerische Zeitschrift Fuer Psychologie – Revue Suisse de Psychologie, 54, 225–236.Google Scholar
Kramer, A. F., Hahn, S., McAuley, E., Cohen, N. J., Banich, M. T., Harrison, C., et al. (1999). Exercise, aging, and cognition: Healthy body, healthy mind? In Rogers, W. A. & Fisk, A. D. (Eds.), Human factors interventions for the health care of older adults (pp. 91–120). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Kramer, A. F., & Willis, S. L. (2002). Enhancing the cognitive vitality of older adults. Current Directions in Psychological Sciences, 11, 173–177.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krampe, R. T. (1994). Maintaining excellence: Cognitive-motor performance in pianists differing in age and skill level. Berlin, Germany: Edition Sigma.Google Scholar
Krampe, R. T. (2002). Aging, expertise and fine motor movement. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 26, 769–776.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Krampe, R. T., & Baltes, P. B. (2003). Intelligence as adaptive resource development and resource allocation: A new look through the lenses of SOC and expertise. In Sternberg, R. J. & Grigorenko, E. L. (Eds.), The psychology of abilities, competencies, and expertise (pp. 31–69). New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krampe, R. T., Engbert, R., & Kliegl, R. (2001). Age-specific problems in rhythmic timing. Psychology and Aging, 16, 12–30.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Krampe, R. T., Engbert, R., & Kliegl, R. (2002). The effects of expertise and age on rhythm production: Adaptations to timing and sequencing constraints. Brain and Cognition, 48, 179–194.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Krampe, R. T., & Ericsson, K. A. (1996). Maintaining excellence: Deliberate practice and elite performance in young and older pianists. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 125, 331–359.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krampe, R. T., Kliegl, R., Mayr, U., Engbert, R., & Vorberg, D. (2000). The fast and the slow of skilled bimanual rhythm production: Parallel versus integrated timing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 26, 206–233.Google ScholarPubMed
Krampe, R. T., Mayr, U., & Kliegl, R. (2005). Timing, sequencing, and executive control in repetitive movement production. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 31, 379–397.Google ScholarPubMed
Kubeck, J. E., Delp, N. D., Haslett, T. K., & McDaniel, M. A. (1996). Does job-related training performance decline with age? Psychology and Aging, 11, 92–107.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Labouvie, G. V., Frohring, W. R., & Baltes, P. B. (1973). Changing relationship between recall performance and abilities as a function of stage of learning and timing of recall. Journal of Educational Psychology, 64, 191–198.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lehmann, A. C., & Ericsson, K. A. (1993). Sight-reading ability of expert pianists in the context of piano accompanying. Psychomusicology, 12, 182–195.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lehmann, A. C., & Ericsson, K. A. (1996). Performance without preparation: Structure and acquisition of expert sight-reading and accompanying performance. Psychomusicology, 15(1–2), 1–29.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lima, S. D., Hale, S., & Myerson, J. (1991). How general is general slowing? Evidence from the lexical domain. Psychology and Aging, 6, 416–425.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lindenberger, U., Kliegl, R., & Baltes, P. B. (1992). Professional expertise does not eliminate negative age differences in imagery-based memory performance during adulthood. Psychology and Aging, 7, 585–593.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lindenberger, U., & Pötter, U. (1998). The complex nature of unique and shared effects in hierarchical linear regression: Implications for developmental psychology. Psychological Methods, 3, 218–230.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MacKay, D. G. (1982). The problems of flexibility, fluency, and speed-accuracy trade-off in skilled behavior. Psychological Review, 89(5), 483–506.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Masunaga, H., & Horn, J. (2001). Expertise in relation to aging changes in components of intelligence. Psychology & Aging, 16, 293–311.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maylor, E. A. (1994). Ageing and the retrieval of specialized and general knowledge: Performance of masterminds. British Journal of Psychology, 85, 105–114.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McClelland, G. H., & Judd, C. M. (1993). Statistical difficulties of detecting interactions and moderator effects. Psychological Bulletin, 114, 376–390.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McEvoy, G. M., & Cascio, W. F. (1989). Cumulative evidence of the relationship between employee age and job performance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 74, 11–17.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meinz, E. J. (2000). Experience-based attenuation of age-related differences in music cognition tasks. Psychology and Aging, 15, 297–312.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meinz, E. J., & Salthouse, T. A. (1998). The effects of age and experience on memory for visually presented music. Journals of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 53B, P60–P69.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mireles, D. E., & Charness, N. (2002). Computational explorations of the influence of structured knowledge on age-related cognitive decline. Psychology & Aging, 17, 245–259.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morrow, D., Leirer, V., Altiteri, P., & Fitzsimmons, C. (1994). When expertise reduces age differences in performance. Psychology and Aging, 9, 134–148.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morrow, D. G., Menard, W. E., Stine-Morrow, E. A. L., Teller, T., & Bryant, D. (2001). The influence of expertise and task factors on age differences in pilot communication. Psychology and Aging, 16, 31–46.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morrow, D. G., Ridolfo, H. E., Menard, W. E., Sanborn, A., Stine-Morrow, E. A. L., Magnor, C., et al. (2003). Environmental support promotes expertise-based mitigation of age differences on pilot communication tasks. Psychology and Aging, 18, 268–284.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Myerson, J., Hale, S., Wagstaff, D., Poon, L. W., & Smith, G. A. (1990). The information-loss model: A mathematical theory of age-related cognitive slowing. Psychological Review, 97, 475–487.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Over, R., & Thomas, P. (1995). Age and skilled psychomotor performance: A comparison of younger and older golfers. International Journal of Aging & Human Development, 41(1), 1–12.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Plomin, R. (1990). The role of inheritance of behavior. Science, 248, 183–188.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Plomin, R., & Rende, R. (1991). Human behavioral genetics. Annual Review of Psychology, 42, 161–190.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rabbitt, P. M. A. (1993). Crystal quest. A search for the basis of maintenance of practised skills into old age. In Baddeley, A. & Weiskrantz, L. (Eds.), Attention: Selection, awareness, and control. Oxford, UK: Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
Rapp, M., Krampe, R. T., & Baltes, P. B. (2005). Adaptive task prioritization in aging: Selective ressource allocation to postural control is preserved in Alzheimer disease. American Journal of Geriaric Psychiatry 14 (1), 52–61.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rosenbaum, D. A., Kenny, S., & Derr, M. (1983). Hierarchical control of rapid movement sequences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 9, 86–102.Google ScholarPubMed
Salthouse, T. A. (1984). Effects of age and skill in typing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 113, 345–371.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Salthouse, T. A. (1985a). Speed of behavior and its implications for cognition. In Birren, J. E. & Schaie, K. W. (Eds.), Handbook of the psychology of aging (pp. 400–426). New York: Van Nostrand Reinhold.Google Scholar
Salthouse, T. A. (1985b). A theory of cognitive aging. Amsterdam: North Holland Press.Google Scholar
Salthouse, T. A. (1991a). Age and experience effects on the interpretation of orthographic drawings of three-dimensional objects. Psychology and Aging, 6, 426–433.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Salthouse, T. A. (1991b). Expertise as the circumvention of human processing limitations. In Ericsson, K. A. & Smith, J. (Eds.), Toward a general theory of expertise: Prospects and limits (pp. 286–300). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Salthouse, T. A. (1991c). Mediation of adult age differences in cognition by reductions in working memory and speed of processing. Psychological Science, 2, 179–183.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Salthouse, T. A. (1996). The processing-speed theory of adult age differences in cognition. Psychological Review, 103, 403–428.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Salthouse, T. A., Babcock, R. L., Skovronek, E., Mitchell, D. R. D., & Palmon, R. (1990). Age and experience effects in spatial visualization. Developmental Psychology, 26, 128–136.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Salthouse, T. A., & Maurer, T. J. (1996). Aging, job performance, and career development. In Birren, J. E. & Schaie, K. W. (Eds.), Handbook of the psychology of aging (4th ed., pp. 353–364). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Schaie, K. W. (1989). Perceptual speed in adulthood: Cross-sectional and longitudinal studies. Psychology and Aging, 4, 443–453.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schmidt, F. L., & Hunter, J. E. (1998). The validity and utility of selection methods in personnel psychology: Practival and theoretical implications of 85 years of research findings. Psychological Bulletin, 124, 262–274.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schooler, C., & Mulatu, M. S. (2001). The reciprocal effects of leisure time activities and intellectual functioning in older people: A longitudinal analysis. Psychology and Aging, 16, 466–482.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schooler, C., Mulatu, M. S., & Oates, G. (1999). The continuing effects of substantively complex work on the intellectual functioning of older workers. Psychology and Aging, 14, 483–506.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schulz, R., & Curnow, C. (1988). Peak performance and age among superathletes: Track and field, swimming, baseball, tennis and golf. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 43, 113–120.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schulz, R., Musa, D., Staszewski, J., & Siegler, R. S. (1994). The relationship between age and major league baseball performance: Implications for development. Psychology and Aging, 9, 274–286.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Simonton, D. K. (1996). Creative expertise: A life-span developmental perspective. In Ericsson, K. A. (Ed.), The road to excellence (pp. 227–253). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Singer, T., Lindenberger, U., & Baltes, P. B. (2003). Plasticity of memory for new learning in very old age: A story of major loss? Psychology and Aging, 18, 306–317.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sloboda, J. A. (1991). Musical expertise. In Ericsson, K. A. & Smith, J. (Eds.), Toward a general theory of expertise. Prospects and limits (pp. 153–171). Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Sparrow, P. R., & Davies, D. R. (1988). Effects of age, tenure, training, and job complexity on technical performance. Psychology and Aging, 3, 307–314.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Starkes, J., Deakin, J., Allard, F., Hodges, N. J., & Hayes, A. (1996). Deliberate practice: What is it anyway? In Ericsson, K. A. (Ed.), The road to excellence: The acquisition of expert performance in the arts and sciences, sports, and games (pp. 81–106). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Sternberg, R. J. (1999). Intelligence as developing expertise. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 24, 359–375.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sternberg, R. J., & Wagner, R. K. (1986). Practical intelligence: Nature and origins of competence in the everyday world. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Swaen, G. M. H., Kant, I., Amelsvoort, L. G. P. M., & Beurskens, A. J. H. M. (2002). Job mobility, its determinants, and its effects: Longitudinal data from the maastricht cohort study. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 7, 121–129.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Taylor, J. L., O'Hara, R., Mumenthaler, M. S., Rosen, A. C., & Yesavage, J. A. (2005). Cognitive ability, expertise, and age differences in following air-traffic control instructions. Psychology and Aging, 20, 117–133.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Telford, C. W., & Spangler, H. (1935). Training effects in motor skills. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18, 141–147.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wagner, R. K., & Sternberg, R. J. (1991). Tacit knowledge: Its uses in identifying, assessing, and developing managerial talent. In Jones, J., Steffy, B., & Bray, D. (Eds.), Applying psychology in business: The manager's handbook (pp. 333–344). New York: Human Sciences Press.Google Scholar
Waldman, D. A., & Avolio, B. J. (1986). A meta-analysis of age-differences in job-performance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 71, 33–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Walsh, D. A., & Hershey, D. A. (1993). Mental models and the maintenance of complex problem-solving skills in old age. In Cerella, J., Rybash, J., Hoyer, W., & Commons, M. (Eds.), Adult information processing: Limits on loss (pp. 553–584). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Westerman, S. J., Davies, D. R., Glendon, A. I., & Stammers, R. B. (1998). Ageing and word processing competence: Compensation or compilation. British Journal of Psychology, 89, 579–597.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Willis, S. L., & Marsiske, M. (1991). Lifespan perspective on practical intelligence. In Tupper, T. E. & Cicerone, K. D. (Eds.), The neuropsychology of everyday life: Issues in development and rehabilitation (pp. 183–197). Boston, MA: Kluwer Academic Publishers.Google Scholar
Winner, E. (1982). Invented worlds: The psychology of the arts. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Woollacott, M., & Shumway-Cook, A. (2002). Attention and the control of posture and gait: A review of an emerging area of research. Gait and Posture, 16, 1–14.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yang, L., Krampe, R. T., & Baltes, P. B. (2006). Basic forms of cognitive plasticity extended into the oldest-old: Retest learning, age, and cognitive functioning. Psychology and Aging, in press.

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×