Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-rcrh6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-22T14:59:43.688Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

18 - Acceptability Studies in (Non-English) Germanic Languages

from Part III - Experimental Studies of Specific Populations and Language Families

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  16 December 2021

Grant Goodall
Affiliation:
University of California, San Diego
Get access

Summary

This chapter surveys experimental studies of Germanic languages other than English, including the Scandinavian languages, Dutch, and German. Experimental investigations of the syntax of these languages are mainly of two types. The first type concerns syntactic phenomena that are found both in English and the other Germanic languages. Several phenomena of this type are reviewed in this chapter, including island constraints, the that-trace effect, and superiority. With regard to these phenomena, the major question has been whether syntactic constraints apply in the same way across the Germanic languages, as would be expected given the close relatedness of the Germanic languages. The second type of experimental syntactic research reviewed in this chapter is concerned with syntactic phenomena that are only found in the non-English Germanic languages. These include word-order alternations related to the verb-second property of the non-English Germanic languages and the less rigid word order of the Germanic SOV languages.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2021

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Andersson, S.-G. & Kvam, S. (1984). Satzverschränkung im heutigen Deutsch. Tübingen: Narr.Google Scholar
Bader, M., Ellsiepen, E., Koukoulioti, V., & Portele, Y. (2017). Filling the prefield: Findings and challenges. In Freitag, C., Bott, O., & Schlotterbeck, F., eds., Two Perspectives on V2: The Invited Talks of the DGfS 2016 Workshop “V2 in Grammar and Processing: Its Causes and Its Consequences.” Konstanz: University of Konstanz, pp. 2749.Google Scholar
Bader, M. & Häussler, J. (2010). Word order in German: A corpus study. Lingua, 120(3), 717762.Google Scholar
Bader, M. & Schmid, T. (2009). Verb clusters in Colloquial German. Journal of Comparative Germanic Linguistics, 12(3), 175228.Google Scholar
Barbiers, S., Bennis, H., & Dros-Hendriks, L. (2018). Merging verb cluster variation. Linguistic Variation, 18(1), 144196.Google Scholar
Bard, E. G., Robertson, D., & Sorace, A. (1996). Magnitude estimation of linguistic acceptability. Language, 72(1), 3268.Google Scholar
Bayer, J., Häussler, J., & Bader, M. (2016). A new diagnostic for cyclic Wh-movement: Discourse particles in German questions. Linguistic Inquiry, 47(4), 591629.Google Scholar
Bayer, J. & Salzmann, M. (2013). That-trace effects and resumption: How improper movement can be repaired. In Brandt, P. & Fuß, E., eds., Repairs: The Added Value of Being Wrong. Berlin: De Gruyter, pp. 275333.Google Scholar
Bennis, H. (1986). Gaps and Dummies. Dordrecht: Foris.Google Scholar
Birner, B. J. & Ward, G. (1998). Information Status and Noncanonical Word Order in English. Amsterdam and Philadelphia: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Boeckx, C. (2007). Islands. Language and Linguistics Compass, 2(1), 151167.Google Scholar
Bornkessel, I. & Schlesewsky, M. (2009). Processing Syntax and Morphology: A Neurocognitive Perspective. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Bouma, G. J. (2008). Starting a sentence in Dutch: A corpus study of subject- and object- fronting. Doctoral dissertation, University of Groningen.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1973). Conditions on transformations. In Anderson, S. R. & Kiparsky, P., eds., A Festschrift for Morris Halle. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston, pp. 232286.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. & Lasnik, H. (1977). Filters and control. Linguistic Inquiry, 8, 425504.Google Scholar
Christensen, K. R., Kizach, J., & Nyvad, A. M. (2013). Escape from the island: Grammaticality and (reduced) acceptability of wh-island violations in Danish. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 42, 5170.Google Scholar
Christensen, K. R. & Nyvad, A. M. (2014). On the nature of escapable relative islands. Nordic Journal of Linguistics, 48(1), 3765.Google Scholar
Cowart, W. (1997). Experimental Syntax: Applying Objective Methods to Sentence Judgments. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
Croft, W. (2003). Typology and Universals. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Culicover, P. W. (1993). Evidence against ECP accounts of the that-t effect. Linguistic Inquiry, 24(3), 557561.Google Scholar
Ellsiepen, E. & Bader, M. (2018). Constraints on argument linearization in German. Glossa: a Journal of General Linguistics, 3(1), 136. DOI:10.5334/gjgl.258Google Scholar
Engdahl, E. (1982). Restrictions on unbounded dependencies in Swedish. In Engdahl, E. & Ejerhed, E., eds., Readings on Unbounded Dependencies in Scandinavian Languages. Stockholm: Almqvist & Wiksell, pp. 151174.Google Scholar
Engdahl, E. (1997). Relative clause extractions in context. Working Papers in Scandinavian Syntax, 60, 5179.Google Scholar
Erteschik-Shir, N. (1973). On the nature of island constraints. Doctoral dissertation, Massachusetts Institute of Technology.Google Scholar
Erteschik-Shir, N. & Lappin, S. (1979). Dominance and the functional explanation of island phenomena. Theoretical Linguistics, 6(1–3), 4186.Google Scholar
Fanselow, G. (1987). Konfigurationalität: Untersuchungen zur Universalgrammatik am Beispiel des Deutschen. Tübingen: Narr.Google Scholar
Fanselow, G. (2006). On pure syntax (uncontaminated by information structure). In Brandt, P. & E. Fuss, , eds., Form, Structure and Grammar. Berlin: Akademie Verlag, pp. 137158.Google Scholar
Fanselow, G., Lenertova, D., & Weskott, T. (2008). Studies on the acceptability of object movement to Spec, CP. In Steube, A., ed., The Discourse Potential of Underspecified Structures, vol. 8. Berlin: De Gruyter, pp. 413438.Google Scholar
Fanselow, G., Schlesewsky, M., Vogel, R., & Weskott, T. (2011). Animacy effects on crossing wh-movement in German. Linguistics, 49(4), 657683.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fanselow, G. & Weskott, T. (2010). A short note on long movement in German. Linguistische Berichte, 222, 129140.Google Scholar
Featherston, S. (2005a). Magnitude estimation and what it can do for your syntax: Some wh-constraints in German. Lingua, 115(11), 15251550.Google Scholar
Featherston, S. (2005b). That-trace in German. Lingua, 115(9), 12771302.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frey, W. (2004). A medial topic position for German. Linguistische Berichte, 198, 153190.Google Scholar
Gibson, E. (2000). The dependency locality theory: A distance-based theory of linguistic complexity. In Marantz, A., Miyashita, Y., & O’Neil, W., eds., Image, Language, Brain. Papers from the First Mind Articulation Project Symposium, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 95126.Google Scholar
Goodall, G. (2015). The D-linking effect on extraction from islands and non-islands. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 111.Google Scholar
Grewendorf, G. (1988). Aspekte der deutschen Syntax: Eine Rektions-Bindungs-Analyse. Tübingen: Narr.Google Scholar
Haider, H. (1993). Deutsche Syntax – generativ. Vorstudien zu einer projektiven Theorie der Grammatik. Tübingen: Narr.Google Scholar
Haider, H. (2010). The Syntax of German. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Häussler, J., Grant, M., Fanselow, G. & Frazier, L. (2015). Superiority in English and German: Cross-language grammatical differences? Syntax, 18(3), 235265.Google Scholar
Heycock, C., Sorace, A., & Hansen, Z. S. (2010). V-to-I and V2 in subordinate clauses: An investigation of Faroese in relation to Icelandic and Danish. Journal of Comparative Germanic Linguistics, 13(1), 6197.Google Scholar
Hofmeister, P. & Sag, I. A. (2010). Cognitive constraints and island effects. Language, 86(2), 366415.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hörberg, T. (2018). Functional motivations behind direct object fronting in written Swedish: A corpus-distributional account. Glossa: A Journal of General Linguistics, 3(1), 81. DOI:10.5334/gjgl.502Google Scholar
Hörberg, T., Koptjevskaja-Tamm, M., & Kallioinen, P. (2013). The neurophysiological correlate to grammatical function reanalysis in Swedish. Language and Cognitive Processes, 28(3), 388416.Google Scholar
Keller, F. (2000a). Evaluating competition-based models of word order. In Gleitman, L. R. & Joshi, A. K., eds., Proceedings of the 22nd Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum, pp. 747752.Google Scholar
Keller, F. (2000b). Gradience in grammar: Experimental and computational aspects of degrees of grammaticality. Doctoral dissertation, University of Edinburgh.Google Scholar
Keller, F. (2006). Linear optimality theory as a model of gradience in grammar. In Fanselow, G., Féry, C., Vogel, R., & Schlesewsky, M., eds., Gradience in Grammar: Generative Perspectives. New York: Oxford University Press, pp. 270287.Google Scholar
Keller, F. & Sorace, A. (2003). Gradient auxiliary selection and impersonal passivization in German: An experimental investigation. Journal of Linguistics, 39, 57108.Google Scholar
Kempen, G. & Harbusch, K. (2004). Generating natural word orders in a semi-free word order language: Treebank-based linearization preferences for German. In Gelbukh, A., ed., Fifth International Conference on Intelligent Text Processing and Computational Linguistics (CICLing2004), Seoul, South Korea (Lecture Notes in Computer Science, 2945). Berlin: Springer, pp. 350354.Google Scholar
Kizach, J. & Balling, L. W. (2013). Givenness, complexity, and the Danish dative alternation. Memory & Cognition, 41(8), 11591171.Google Scholar
Kiziak, T. (2010). Extraction Asymmetries: Experimental Evidence from German. Amsterdam and Philadelphia: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kluender, R. (1998). On the distinction between strong and weak islands: A processing perspective. In Culicover, P. W. & McNally, L., eds., The Limits of Syntax (Syntax and Semantics, 29). San Diego, CA: Academic Press, pp. 241279.Google Scholar
Kluender, R. & Kutas, M. (1993). Subjacency as a processing phenomenon. Language and Cognitive Processes, 8, 573633.Google Scholar
Konieczny, L. (2000). Locality and parsing complexity. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 29, 627645.Google Scholar
Kristensen, L. B., Engberg-Pedersen, E., & Poulsen, M. (2014). Context improves comprehension of fronted objects. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 43(2), 125140.Google Scholar
Kristensen, L. B., Engberg-Pedersen, E., & Wallentin, M. (2014). Context predicts word order processing in Broca’s region. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 26(12), 27622777.Google Scholar
Kush, D., Lohndal, T., & Sprouse, J. (2018). Investigating variation in island effects. Natural Language & Linguistic Theory, 36, 743779.Google Scholar
Lenerz, J. (1977). Zur Abfolge nominaler Satzglieder im Deutschen. Tübingen: Narr.Google Scholar
Lohndal, T. (2007). That-t in Scandinavian and elsewhere: Variation in the position of C. Working Papers in Scandinavian Syntax, 79, 4773.Google Scholar
Maling, J. & Zaenen, A. (1978). The nonuniversality of a surface filter. Linguistic Inquiry, 9(3), 475497.Google Scholar
Meng, M., Bader, M., & Bayer, J. (1999). Die Verarbeitung von Subjekt-Objekt-Ambiguitäten im Kontext. In Wachsmuth, I. & Jung, B., eds., KogWiss99. Proceedings der 4. Fachtagung der Gesellschaft fur Kognitionswissenschaft. St. Augustin: Infix Verlag, pp. 244249.Google Scholar
Nyvad, A. M., Christensen, K. R., & Vikner, S. (2017). CP-recursion in Danish: A cP/Cpanalysis. The Linguistic Review, 34(3), 449477.Google Scholar
Pesetsky, D. (1987). Wh-in-situ: Movement and unselective binding. In Reuland, E. J. & ter Meulen, A. G. B., eds., The Representation of (In)definiteness, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 98129.Google Scholar
Pesetsky, D. (2017). Complementizer-trace effects. In Everaert, M. & van Riemsdijk, H., eds., The Wiley Blackwell Companion to Syntax, 2nd ed. New York: John Wiley & Sons, pp. 134.Google Scholar
Phillips, C. (2013). Some arguments and nonarguments for reductionist accounts of syntactic phenomena. Language and Cognitive Processes, 28(1–2), 156187.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Poulsen, M. (2008). Acceptability and processing of long-distance dependencies in Danish. Nordic Journal of Linguistics, 31(1), 73107.Google Scholar
Prince, E. F. (1981). Toward a taxonomy of given-new information. In Cole, P., ed., Radical Pragmatic. New York: Academic Press, pp. 223255.Google Scholar
Reis, M. (1995). Extractions from verb-second clauses in German? In Lutz, U. & Pafel, J., eds., On Extraction and Extraposition in German. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 4588.Google Scholar
Rizzi, L. (1997). The fine structure of the left periphery. In Haegeman, L., ed., Elements of Grammar. Dordrecht: Kluwer, pp. 281337.Google Scholar
Ross, J. R. (1967). Constraints on variables in syntax. Doctoral dissertation, Massachusetts Institute of Technology.Google Scholar
Salzmann, M., Häussler, J., Bader, M., & Bayer, J. (2013). That-trace effects without traces: An experimental investigation. In S. Kleine & S. Sloggett, eds., NELS 42: Proceedings of the 42nd Meeting of the North East Linguistic Society, vol. 2. CreateSpace Independent Publishing Platform, pp. 149162.Google Scholar
Skopeteas, S. & Fanselow, G. (2009). Effects of givenness and constraints on free word order. In Zimmermann, M. & Féry, C., eds., Information Structure: Theoretical, Typological, and Experimental Perspectives. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 307331.Google Scholar
Sorace, A. (1992). Conditions on syntactic knowledge: Auxiliary selection in native and non-native grammars of Italian. Doctoral dissertation, University of Edinburgh.Google Scholar
Speyer, A. (2008). German Vorfeld-filling as constraint interaction. In Benz, A. & Kühnlein, P., eds., Constraints in Discourse. Amsterdam and Philadelphia: John Benjamins, pp. 267290.Google Scholar
Sprouse, J., Caponigro, I., Greco, C., & Cecchetto, C. (2016). Experimental syntax and the variation of island effects in English and Italian. Natural Language & Linguistic Theory, 34(1), 307344.Google Scholar
Sprouse, J. & Hornstein, N., eds. (2013). Experimental Syntax and Island Effects. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Sprouse, J., Schütze, C., & Almeida, D. (2013). A comparison of informal and formal acceptability judgments using a random sample from Linguistic Inquiry 2001–2010. Lingua, 134, 219248.Google Scholar
Sprouse, J., Wagers, M., & Phillips, C. (2012). A test of the relation between working-memory capacity and syntactic island effects. Language, 88(1), 82123.Google Scholar
Störzer, M. & Stolterfoht, B. (2018). Is German discourse-configurational? Experimental evidence for a topic position. Glossa: A Journal of General Linguistics, 3(1), 124.Google Scholar
Tutunjian, D., Heinat, F., Klingvall, E., & Wiklund, A.-L. (2017). Processing relative clause extractions in Swedish. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 2118.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Uszkoreit, H., Brants, T., Duchier, D., Krenn, B., Konieczny, L., Oepen, S., & Skut, W. (1998). Studien zur performanzorientierten Linguistik Aspekte der Relativsatzextraposition im Deutschen. Kognitionswissenschaft, 7, 129133.Google Scholar
Weskott, T., Hörnig, R., Fanselow, G., & Kliegl, R. (2011). Contextual licensing of marked OVS word order in German. Linguistische Berichte, 225, 318.Google Scholar
Wiltschko, M. (1997). D-Linking, scrambling und superiority in German. GAGL: Groninger Arbeiten zur germanistischen Linguistik, 41, 107142.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×