Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-2brh9 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-26T22:21:58.988Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Part VI - Evolution and Development

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 March 2020

Lance Workman
Affiliation:
University of South Wales
Will Reader
Affiliation:
Sheffield Hallam University
Jerome H. Barkow
Affiliation:
Dalhousie University, Nova Scotia
Get access

Summary

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2020

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

References

Sulloway, F. J. (1992). Freud, Biologist of the Mind: Beyond the Psychoanalytic Legend. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Sulloway, F. J. (1996). Born to Rebel: Birth Order, Family Dynamics, and Creative Lives. New York: Pantheon Books.Google Scholar

References

Abravanel, E., & Sigafoos, A. D. (1984). Exploring the presence of imitation during early infancy. Child Development, 55, 381392.Google Scholar
Anzures, G., Wheeler, A., Quinn, P. C., et al. (2012). Brief daily exposures to Asian females reverses perceptual narrowing for Asian faces in Caucasian infants. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 112(4), 484495.Google Scholar
Bardi, L., Regolin, L., & Simion, F. (2014). The first time ever I saw your feet: Inversion effect in newborns’ sensitivity to biological motion. Developmental Psychology, 50(4), 986.Google Scholar
Beier, J. S., & Spelke, E. S. (2012). Infants’ developing understanding of social gaze. Child Development, 83(2), 486496.Google Scholar
Belsky, J., & Most, R. K. (1981). From exploration to play: A cross-sectional study of infant free play behavior. Developmental Psychology, 17(5), 630.Google Scholar
Belsky, J., Steinberg, L., & Draper, P. (1991). Childhood experience, interpersonal development, and reproductive strategy: An evolutionary theory of socialization. Child Development, 62(4), 647670.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Belsky, J., Steinberg, L. D., Houts, R. M., et al. (2007). Family rearing antecedents of pubertal timing. Child Development, 78(4), 13021321.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bjorklund, D. F. (1987). How age changes in knowledge base contribute to the development of children’s memory: An interpretive review. Developmental Review, 7(2), 93130.Google Scholar
Bjorklund, D. F. (1997). The role immaturity in human development. Psychological Bulletin, 122(2), 153169.Google Scholar
Bjorklund, D. F. (2003). Evolutionary psychology from a developmental systems perspective: comment on Lickliter and Honeycutt (2003). Psychological Bulletin, 129, 836841.Google Scholar
Bjorklund, D. F. (2007). Why Youth Is Not Wasted on the Young: Immaturity in Human Development. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Bjorklund, D. F. (2015). Developing adaptations. Developmental Review, 38, 1335.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bjorklund, D. F. (2016). Prepared is not preformed: Comment on Witherington and Lickliter. Human Development, 59, 235241.Google Scholar
Bjorklund, D. F., & Ellis, B. J. (2014). Children, childhood, and development in evolutionary perspective. Developmental Review, 34(3), 225264.Google Scholar
Bjorklund, D. F., Gaultney, J. F., & Green, B. L. (1993). “I watch therefore I can do”: The development of meta-imitation over the preschool years and the advantage of optimism in one’s imitative skills. In Pasnak, R. & Howe, M. L., eds., Emerging Themes in Cognitive Development, Vol. 1. New York: Springer-Verlag, pp. 79102.Google Scholar
Bjorklund, D. F., & Green, B. L. (1992). The adaptive nature of cognitive immaturity. American Psychologist, 47, 4654.Google Scholar
Bjorklund, D. F., & Pellegrini, A. D. (2002). The Origins of Human Nature: Evolutionary Developmental Psychology. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Bjorklund, D. F., Ellis, B. J., & Rosenberg, J. S. (2007). Evolved probabilistic cognitive mechanisms: An evolutionary approach to gene × environment × development interactions. In Kail, R. V., ed., Advances in Child Development and Behavior, Vol. 35. Oxford: Elsevier, pp. 139.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bjorklund, D. F., Hernández Blasi, C., & Ellis, B. J. (2016). Evolutionary developmental psychology. In Buss, D. M., ed., Evolutionary Psychology Handbook, 2nd ed., Vol. 2. New York: Wiley, pp. 904925.Google Scholar
Boyce, W. T., & Ellis, B. J. (2005). Biological sensitivity to context: I. An evolutionary-developmental theory of the origins and functions of stress reactivity. Development and Psychopathology, 17, 271301.Google Scholar
Brooks, R., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2002). The importance of eyes: How infants interpret adult looking behavior. Developmental Psychology, 38, 958966.Google Scholar
Buss, D. M., Haselton, M. G., Shackelford, T. K., Bleske, A. L., & Wakefield, J. C. (1998). Adaptations, exaptations, and spandrels. American Psychologist, 53, 533548.Google Scholar
Byrne, R. W. (2005). Social cognition: Imitation, imitation, imitation. Current Biology, 15, R489R500.Google Scholar
Cook, M., & Mineka, S. (1989). Observational conditioning of fear to fear-relevant versus fear-irrelevant stimuli in rhesus monkeys. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 98, 448459.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Costello, E. J., Sung, M., Worthman, C., & Angold, A. (2007). Pubertal maturation and the development of alcohol use and abuse. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 88(Suppl. 1), S50S59.Google Scholar
Csibra, G., & Gergely, G. (2011). Natural pedagogy as evolutionary adaptation. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, 366, 11491157.Google Scholar
Cutting, N., Apperly, I. A., Chappell, J., & Beck, S. R. (2014). The puzzling difficulty of tool innovation: Why can’t children piece their knowledge together? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 125, 110117.Google Scholar
DeLoache, J. S., & LoBue, V. (2009). The narrow fellow in the grass: Human infants associate snakes and fear. Developmental Science, 12, 201207.Google Scholar
Di Giorgio, E., Leo, I., Pascalis, O., & Simion, F. (2012). Is the face-perception system human-specific at birth? Developmental Psychology, 48, 10831090.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dobzhansky, T. (1973). Genetic Diversity and Human Equality. New York: Basic Books.Google ScholarPubMed
Dunlosky, J., & Rawson, K. A. (2012). Overconfidence produces underachievement: Inaccurate self-evaluations undermine students’ learning and retention. Learning and Instruction, 22, 271280.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellis, B. J. (2004). Timing of pubertal maturation in girls: An integrated life history approach. Psychological Bulletin, 130, 920958.Google Scholar
Ellis, B. J., & Graber, J. (2000). Psychosocial antecedents of variation in girls’ pubertal timing: Maternal depression, stepfather presence, and marital and family stress. Child Development, 71, 485501.Google Scholar
Ellis, B. J., Figueredo, A. J., Brumbach, B. H., & Schlomer, G. L. (2009). Fundamental dimensions of environmental risk: The impact of harsh versus unpredictable environments on the evolution and development of life history strategies. Human Nature, 20, 204268.Google Scholar
Ellis, B. J., McFadyen-Ketchum, S., Dodge, K. A., Pettit, G. S., & Bates, J. E. (1999). Quality of early family relationships and individual differences in the timing of pubertal maturation in girls: A longitudinal test of an evolutionary model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 387401.Google Scholar
Flynn, E., & Whiten, A. (2012). Experimental “microcultures” in young children: Identifying biographic, cognitive, and social predictors of information transmission. Child Development, 83(3), 911925.Google Scholar
Furlow, F. B. (1997). Human neonatal cry quality as an honest signal of fitness. Evolution and Human Behavior, 18, 175193.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gardiner, A. K. (2014). Beyond irrelevant actions: Understanding the role of intentionality in children’s imitation of relevant actions. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 119, 5472.Google Scholar
Gardiner, A., Bjorklund, D. F., Greif, M. L., & Gray, S. K. (2012). Choosing and using tools: Prior experience and task difficulty influence preschoolers’ tool-use strategies. Cognitive Development, 27, 240254.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gardiner, A., Greif, M., & Bjorklund, D. F. (2011). Guided by intention: Preschoolers’ imitation reflects inferences of causation. Journal of Cognition and Development, 12, 355373.Google Scholar
Gava, L., Valenza, E., Turati, C., & de Schonen, S. (2008). Effect of partial occlusion on newborns’ face preference and recognition. Developmental Science, 11, 563574.Google Scholar
Geary, D. C. (1995). Reflections of evolution and culture in children’s cognition: Implications for mathematical development and instruction. American Psychologist, 50, 2437.Google Scholar
Geary, D. C. (2002). Sexual selection and human life history. In Kail, R. V., ed., Advances in Child Development and Behavior, Vol. 30. San Diego, CA: Academic Press, pp. 41101.Google Scholar
Geary, D. C. (2005). The Origin of Mind: Evolution of Brain, Cognition, and General Intelligence. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Geary, D. C. (2010). Male, Female: The Evolution of Human Sex Differences, 2nd ed. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Geary, D. C., & Bjorklund, D. F. (2000). Evolutionary developmental psychology. Child Development, 71, 5765.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gergely, G., & Csibra, G. (2005). The social construction of the cultural mind: Imitative learning as a mechanism of human pedagogy. Interaction Studies, 6, 463481.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
German, T. P., & Johnson, S. C. (2002). Function and the origins of the design stance. Journal of Cognition and Development, 3, 279300.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Graber, J. A., Brooks-Gunn, J., & Warren, M. P. (1995). The antecedents of menarcheal age: Heredity, family environment, and stressful life events. Child Development, 66, 346359.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gredlein, J. M., & Bjorklund, D. F. (2005). Sex differences in young children’s use of tools in a problem-solving task: The role of object-oriented play. Human Nature, 16, 211232.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heimann, M. (1989). Neonatal imitation gaze aversion and mother–infant interaction. Infant Behavior & Development, 12, 495505.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hernández Blasi, C., & Bjorklund, D. F. (2003). Evolutionary developmental psychology: A new tool for better understanding human ontogeny. Human Development, 46, 259281.Google Scholar
Hill, K. (1993). Life history theory and evolutionary anthropology. Evolutionary Anthropology, 2, 7888.Google Scholar
Hill, K., & Kaplan, H. (1999). Life history traits in humans: Theory and empirical studies. Annual Review of Anthropology, 28, 397430.Google Scholar
Hoehl, S., & Pauen, S. (2017). Do infants associate spiders and snakes with fearful facial expressions? Evolution and Human Behavior, 38, 404413.Google Scholar
Hornik, R., Risenhoover, N., & Gunnar, M. (1987). The effects of maternal positive, neutral, and negative affective communications on infant responses to new toys. Child Development, 58, 937944.Google Scholar
Jacobson, S. W. (1979). Matching behavior in the young infant. Child Development, 50, 425430.Google Scholar
Kaplan, H., & Lancaster, J. B. (2003). An evolutionary and ecological analysis of human fertility, mating patterns and parental investment. In Wachter, K. W. & Bulatao, R. A., eds., Offspring: Fertility Behavior in Biodemographic Perspective. Washington, DC: National Research Council, National Academies Press, pp. 170223.Google Scholar
Kaplan, H., Hill, K., Lancaster, J., & Hurtado, A. M. (2000). A theory of human life history evolution: Diet intelligence, and longevity. Evolutionary Anthropology, 9, 156185.Google Scholar
Kasturiratne, A., Wickremasinghe, A. R., de Silva, N., et al. (2008). The global burden of snakebite: A literature analysis and modelling based on regional estimates of envenoming and deaths. PLoS Medicine, 5, 15911604.Google Scholar
Keeley, L. H. (1996). War before Civilization: The Myth of the Peaceful Savage. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Kelly, D. J., Liu, S., Lee, K., et al. (2009). Development of the other-race effect in infancy: Evidence toward universality? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 104, 105114.Google Scholar
Kelly, D. J., Quinn, P. C., Slater, A. M., et al. (2007). The other-race effect develops during infancy. Psychological Science, 18, 10841089.Google Scholar
Kenward, B. (2012). Over-imitating preschoolers believe unnecessary actions are normative and enforce their performance by a third party. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 112, 195207.Google Scholar
Konner, M. (2010). The Evolution of Childhood: Relationships, Emotions, Mind. Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press.Google Scholar
Lee, K., Quinn, P. C., & Pascalis, O. (2017). Face race processing and racial bias in early development: A perceptual-social linkage. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 26, 256262.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Legare, C. H. (2017). Cumulative cultural learning: Development and diversity. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 114(30), 78777883.Google Scholar
Legerstee, M. (1991). The role of person and object in eliciting early imitation. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 51, 423433.Google Scholar
Liszkowski, U., Carpenter, M., Striano, T., & Tomasello, M. (2006). 12- and 18-month-olds point to provide information for others. Journal of Cognition and Development, 7, 173187.Google Scholar
Liszkowski, U., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2007). Pointing out new news, old news, and absent referents at 12 months of age. Developmental Science, 10, F1F7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
LoBue, V. (2010). And along came a spider: An attentional bias for the detection of spiders in young children and adults. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 107, 5966.Google Scholar
LoBue, V. (2013). What are we so afraid of? How early attention shapes our most common fears. Child Development Perspectives, 7, 3842.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
LoBue, V., & DeLoache, J. S. (2010). Superior detection of threat-relevant stimuli in infancy. Developmental Science, 13, 221228.Google Scholar
LoBue, V., & Rakison, D. H. (2013). What we fear most: Developmental advantage for threat-relevant stimuli. Developmental Review, 33, 285303.Google Scholar
Lockhart, K. L., Chang, B., & Tyler, S. (2002). Young children’s belief about the stability of traits: Protective optimism. Child Development, 73, 14081430.Google Scholar
Lockman, J. J., & Kahrs, B. A. (2017). New insights into the development of human tool use. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 26, 330334.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lyons, D. E., Young, A. G., & Keil, F. C. (2007). The hidden structure of overimitation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104, 1975119756.Google Scholar
McGuigan, N., Makinson, J., & Whiten, A. (2011). From over-imitation to super-copying: Adults imitate causally irrelevant aspects of tool use with higher fidelity than young children. British Journal of Psychology, 102, 118.Google Scholar
Meaney, M. J. (2007). Environmental programming of phenotypic diversity in female reproductive strategies. Advances in Genetics, 59, 173215.Google Scholar
Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1977). Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates. Science, 198, 7578.Google Scholar
Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1992). Early imitation within a functional framework: The importance of person identity, movement, and development. Infant Behavior and Development, 15(4), 479505.Google Scholar
Menard, J. L., & Hakvoort, R. M. (2007). Variations of maternal care alter offspring levels of behavioural defensiveness in adulthood: Evidence for a threshold model. Behavioural Brain Research, 176, 302313.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mondloch, C. J., Lewis, T. M., Budreau, D. R., et al. (1999). Face perception during early infancy. Psychological Science, 10, 419422.Google Scholar
Mood, D. W. (1979). Sentence comprehension in preschool children: Testing an adaptive egocentrism hypothesis. Child Development, 50: 247250.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nagy, E., & Molnar, P. (2004). Homo imitans or Homo provocans? Human imprinting model of neonatal imitation. Infant Behavior and Development, 27(1), 5463.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nettle, D. (2010). Dying young and living fast: Variation in life history across English neighborhoods. Behavioral Ecology, 21, 387395.Google Scholar
Nettle, D., & Cockerill, M. (2010). Development of social variation in reproductive schedules: A study of an English urban area. PLoS ONE, 5, e12690.Google Scholar
Nielsen, M. (2006). Copying actions and copying outcomes: Social learning through the second year. Developmental Psychology, 42, 555565.Google Scholar
Nielsen, M., & Tomaselli, K. (2010). Over-imitation in the Kalahari Desert and the origins of human cultural cognition. Psychological Science, 5, 729736.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Öhman, A., Flykt, A., & Esteves, F. (2001). Emotion drives attention: Detecting the snake in the grass. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 466478.Google Scholar
Oostenbroek, J., Suddendorf, T., Nielsen, M., et al. (2016). Comprehensive longitudinal study challenges the existence of neonatal imitation in humans. Current Biology, 26(10), 13341338.Google Scholar
Oppenheim, R. W. (1981). Ontogenetic adaptations and retrogressive processes in the development of the nervous system and behavior. In Connolly, K. J. & Prechtl, H. F. R., eds., Maturation and Development: Biological and Psychological Perspectives. Philadelphia, PA: International Medical Publications, pp. 73108.Google Scholar
Parent, C. I., & Meaney, M. J. (2008). The influence of natural variations in maternal care on play fighting in the rat. Developmental Psychobiology, 50, 767776.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pascalis, O., de Haan, M., & Nelson, C. A. (2002). Is face processing species-specific during the first year of life? Science, 296, 13211323.Google Scholar
Pascalis, O., Scott, L. S., Kelly, D. J., et al. (2005). Plasticity of face processing in infancy. Proceeding of the National Academy of Sciences, 102, 52975300.Google Scholar
Pellegrini, A. D. (2013). Play. In Zelazo, P., ed., Oxford Handbook of Developmental Psychology. New York: Oxford University Press, pp. 276299.Google Scholar
Pellegrini, A. D., & Smith, P. K. (1998). Physical activity play: The nature and function of neglected aspect of play. Child Development, 69, 577598.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Placek, C. D., & Quinlan, R. J. (2012). Adolescent fertility and risky environments: A population-level perspective across the lifespan. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 279, 40034008.Google Scholar
Quinlan, R. J. (2007). Human parental effort and environmental risk. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 274, 121125.Google Scholar
Rakison, D. H. (2005). Infant perception and cognition: An evolutionary perspective on early learning. In Ellis, B. J. & Bjorklund, D. F., eds., Origins of the Social Mind: Evolutionary Psychology and Child Development. New York: Guilford, pp. 317353.Google Scholar
Ross, J., Anderson, J. R., & Campbell, R. N. (2011). I remember me: Mnemonic self-reference effects in preschool children. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 76(3), 300.Google Scholar
Saxbe, D. E., & Repetti, R. L. (2009). Brief report: Fathers’ and mothers’ marital relationship predicts daughters’ pubertal development two years later. Journal of Adolescence, 32, 415423.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shin, H. E., Bjorklund, D. F., & Beck, E. F. (2007). The adaptive nature of children’s overestimation in a strategic memory task. Cognitive Development, 22, 197212.Google Scholar
Simpson, J. A., & Belsky, J. (2008). Attachment theory within a modern evolutionary framework: Theory, research, and clinical applications. In Cassidy, J. & Shaver, P. R., eds., Handbook of Attachment: Theory, Research, and Clinical Applications, 2nd ed. New York: Guilford Press, pp. 131157.Google Scholar
Simpson, J. A., Griskevicius, V., Kuo, S., Sung, S., & Collins, W. A. (2012). Evolution, stress, and sensitive periods: The influence of unpredictability in early versus late childhood on sex and risky behavior. Developmental Psychology, 48, 674686.Google Scholar
Stearns, S. (1992). The Evolution of Life Histories. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Thompson, R. A. (2006). The development of the person: Social understanding, relationships, conscience, self. In Damon, W. & Lerner, R. M., gen. eds.; Eisenberg, N., vol. ed., Handbook of Child Psychology: Vol. 3, Social, Emotional, and Personality Development, 6th ed. New York: Wiley, pp. 2498.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. (2000). Culture and cognitive development. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 9, 3740.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. (2009). Why We Cooperate. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M., & Carpenter, M. (2007). Shared intentionality. Developmental Science, 10, 121125.Google Scholar
Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (1992). The psychological foundations of culture. In Cosmides, L., Tooby, J., & Barkow, J. H., eds., The Adapted Mind. New York: Oxford University Press, pp. 19136.Google Scholar
Trickett, P. K., Noll, J. G., & Putnam, F. W. (2011). The impact of sexual abuse on female development: Lessons from a multigenerational, longitudinal research study. Development and Psychopathology, 23, 453476.Google Scholar
Vaish, A., & Striano, T. (2004). Is visual reference necessary? Contributions of facial versus vocal cues in 12-month-olds’ social referencing behavior. Developmental Science, 7, 261269.Google Scholar
Volk, A. A., & Atkinson, J. A. (2013). Infant and child death in the human environment of evolutionary adaptation. Evolution and Human Behavior, 34, 182192.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wakako, S., Wada, K., Yamamoto, T., Mohri, I., & Taniike, M. (2014). Development of preference for conspecific faces in human infants. Developmental Psychology, 50, 979985.Google Scholar
Whiten, A., McGuigan, N., Marshall-Pescini, S., & Hopper, L. M. (2009). Emulation, imitation, over-imitation and the scope of culture for child and chimpanzee. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, 364, 24172428.Google Scholar
Williams, G. C. (1966). Adaptation and Natural Selection. Princeton, NJ: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Williamson, R. A., & Markman, E. M. (2006). Precision of imitation as a function of preschoolers’ understanding of the goal of the demonstration. Developmental Psychology, 42, 723731.Google Scholar
Wilson, M., & Daly, M. (1997). Life expectancy, economic inequality, homicide, and reproductive timing in Chicago neighborhoods. British Medical Journal, 314, 12711274.Google Scholar

References

Alvard, M., & Nolin, D. (2002). Rousseau’s whale hunt? Coordination among big-game hunters. Current Anthropology, 43, 533559.Google Scholar
Bacharach, M. (2006). Beyond Individual Choices: Teams and Frames in Game Theory. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bakeman, R., & Adamson, L. B. (1984). Coordinating attention to people and objects in mother–infant and peer–infant interaction. Child Development, 55, 12781289.Google Scholar
Benozio, A., & Diesendruck, G. (2017). Parochial compliance: Young children’s biased consideration of authorities’ preferences regarding intergroup interactions. Child Development, 88, 15271535.Google Scholar
Bicchieri, C. (2006). The Grammar of Society: The Nature and Dynamics of Social Norms. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Boehm, C. (1999). Hierarchy in the Forest: The Evolution of Egalitarian Behavior. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Boyd, R., Gintis, H., & Bowles, S. (2010). Coordinated punishment of defectors sustains cooperation and can proliferate when rare. Science, 328, 617620.Google Scholar
Bratman, M. (1992). Shared cooperative activity. Psychological Review, 101, 327341.Google Scholar
Bräuer, J., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2005). All great ape species follow gaze to distant locations and around barriers. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 119, 145154.Google Scholar
Bullinger, A. F., Melis, A. P., & Tomasello, M. (2011). Chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes, prefer individual over collaborative strategies towards goals. Animal Behaviour, 82, 11351141.Google Scholar
Butler, L. P., & Walton, G. M. (2013). The opportunity to collaborate increases preschoolers’ motivation for challenging tasks. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 116, 953961.Google Scholar
Calcott, B. (2008). The other cooperation problem: Generating benefit. Biology & Philosophy, 23, 179203.Google Scholar
Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2007). The Gestural Communication of Apes and Monkeys. Manhaw, NJ: LEA.Google Scholar
Call, J., Hare, B., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2004). “Unwilling” versus “unable”: Chimpanzees’ understanding of human intentional action. Developmental Science, 7, 488498.Google Scholar
Camerer, C. F. (2011). Behavioral Game Theory: Experiments in Strategic Interaction. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Cameron, J. A., Alvarez, J. M., Ruble, D. N., & Fuligni, A. J. (2001). Children’s lay theories about ingroups and outgroups: Reconceptualizing research on prejudice. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 5, 118128.Google Scholar
Carpenter, M., & Call, J. (2013). How joint is the joint attention of chimpanzees and human infants? In Terrace, H. S. & Metcalfe, J., eds., Agency and Joint Attention. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 4961.Google Scholar
Carpenter, M., & Liebal, K. (2011). Joint attention, communication, and knowing together in infancy. In Seemann, A., ed., Joint Attention: New Developments. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 159181.Google Scholar
Carpenter, M., Nagell, K., & Tomasello, M. (1998). Social cognition, joint attention, and communicative competence from 9 to 15 months of age. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 63, 1143.Google Scholar
Carpenter, M., Tomasello, M., & Striano, T. (2005). Role reversal imitation and language in typically developing infants and children with autism. Infancy, 8, 253278.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, H. H. (1996). Using Language. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Debove, S., André, J. B., & Baumard, N. (2015). Partner choice creates fairness in humans. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 282, 20150392.Google Scholar
Debove, S., Baumard, N., & André, J. B. (2015). Evolution of equal division among unequal partners. Evolution, 69, 561569.Google Scholar
Doebeli, M., & Hauer, C. (2005). Models of cooperation based on the prisoner’s dilemma and the snowdrift game. Ecology Letters, 8, 748766.Google Scholar
Duguid, S., Wyman, E., Bullinger, A. F., Herfurth-Majstorovic, K., & Tomasello, M. (2014). Coordination strategies of chimpanzees and human children in a Stag Hunt game. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 281, 20141973.Google Scholar
Dunham, Y., Baron, A. S., & Carey, S. (2011). Consequences of “minimal” group affiliations in children. Child Development, 82, 793811.Google Scholar
Fehr, E., & Fischbacher, U. (2003). The nature of human altruism. Nature, 425, 785791.Google Scholar
Fehr, E., & Fischbacher, U. (2004). Third-party punishment and social norms. Evolution and Human Behavior, 25, 6387.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fehr, E., Bernhard, H., & Rockenbach, B. (2008). Egalitarianism in young children. Nature 454, 10791084.Google Scholar
Fletcher, G. E., Warneken, F., & Tomasello, M. (2012). Differences in cognitive processes underlying the collaborative activities of children and chimpanzees. Cognitive Development, 27, 136153.Google Scholar
Gilbert, M. (1989). On Social Facts. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Gilbert, M. (1996). Living Together: Rationality, Sociality, and Obligation. Lanham, MD: Rowman & Littlefield.Google Scholar
Gilbert, M. (2008). Social convention revisited. Topoi, 27, 516.Google Scholar
Gintis, H., Bowles, S., Boyd, R., & Fehr, E., eds. (2005). Moral Sentiments and Material Interests. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Göckeritz, S., Schmidt, M. F. H., & Tomasello, M. (2014). Young children’s creation and transmission of social norms. Cognitive Development, 30, 8195.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldvicht-Bacon, E., & Diesendruck, G. (2016). Children’s capacity to use cultural focal points in coordination problems. Cognition, 149, 95103.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gräfenhain, M., Behne, T., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2009). Young children’s understanding of joint commitments. Developmental Psychology, 45, 14301443.Google Scholar
Grueneisen, S., & Tomasello, M. (2017). Children coordinate in a social dilemma by taking turns and along dominance asymmetries. Developmental Psychology, 53, 265273.Google Scholar
Grueneisen, S., & Tomasello, M. (2019). Children use rules to coordinate in a social dilemma. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 179, 362374.Google Scholar
Grueneisen, S., Wyman, E., & Tomasello, M. (2015a). Children use salience to solve coordination problems. Developmental Science, 18, 495501.Google Scholar
Grueneisen, S., Wyman, E., & Tomasello, M. (2015b). Conforming to coordinate: Children use majority information for peer coordination. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 33, 136147.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grueneisen, S., Wyman, E., & Tomasello, M. (2015c). “I know you don’t know I know …” Children use second-order false-belief reasoning for peer coordination. Child Development, 86, 287293.Google Scholar
Hamann, K., Warneken, F., Greenberg, J. R., & Tomasello, M. (2011). Collaboration encourages equal sharing in children but not in chimpanzees. Nature, 476, 328331.Google Scholar
Hamann, K., Warneken, F., & Tomasello, M. (2012). Children’s developing commitments to joint goals. Child Development, 83, 137145.Google Scholar
Hammerstein, P., ed. (2003). Genetic and Cultural Evolution of Cooperation. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Hardecker, S., Schmidt, M. F. H., & Tomasello, M. (2017). Children’s developing understanding of the conventionality of rules. Journal of Cognition and Development, 18, 163188.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hare, B. (2017). Survival of the friendliest: Homo sapiens evolved via selection for prosociality. Annual Review of Psychology, 68, 155186.Google Scholar
Hare, B., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2001). Do chimpanzees know what conspecifics know and do not know? Animal Behaviour, 61, 139151.Google Scholar
Haun, D. B. M., Rekers, Y., & Tomasello, M. (2012). Majority-biased transmission in chimpanzees and human children, but not orangutans. Current Biology, 22, 727731.Google Scholar
Haun, D. B. M., Rekers, Y., & Tomasello, M. (2014). Children conform to the behavior of peers; other great apes stick with what they know. Psychological Science, 25, 21602167.Google Scholar
Hirata, S., & Fuwa, K. (2007). Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) learn to act with other individuals in a cooperative task. Primates, 48, 1321.Google Scholar
Horner, V., & Whiten, A. (2005). Causal knowledge and imitation/emulation switching in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and children (Homo sapiens). Animal Cognition, 8, 164181.Google Scholar
Kaminski, J., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2008). Chimpanzees know what others know but not what they believe. Cognition, 109, 224234.Google Scholar
Karg, K., Schmelz, M., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2015). Chimpanzees strategically manipulate what others can see. Animal Cognition, 18, 10691076.Google Scholar
Kinzler, K. D., Dupoux, E., & Spelke, E. S. (2007). The native language of social cognition. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104, 1257712580.Google Scholar
Leavens, D. A., Hopkins, W. D., & Thomas, R. K. (2004). Referential communication by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 118, 4857.Google Scholar
Lewis, D. (1969). Convention: A Philosophical Study. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Liszkowski, U., Carpenter, M., Henning, A., Striano, T., & Tomasello, M. (2004). Twelve-month-olds point to share attention and interest. Developmental Science, 7, 297307.Google Scholar
Lyons, D. E., Young, A. G., & Keil, F. C. (2007). The hidden structure of overimitation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104, 1975119756.Google Scholar
Mehta, J., Starmer, C., & Sugden, R. (1994). The nature of salience: An experimental investigation. American Economic Review, 84, 658673.Google Scholar
Melis, A. P., Hare, B., & Tomasello, M. (2006). Engineering cooperation in chimpanzees: Tolerance constraints on cooperation. Animal Behaviour, 72, 275286.Google Scholar
Melis, A. P., Grocke, P., Kalbitz, J., & Tomasello, M. (2016). One for you, one for me: Humans’ unique turn-taking skills. Psychological Science, 27, 987996.Google Scholar
Milinski, M., Semmann, D., & Krambeck, H. J. (2002). Reputation helps solve the “Tragedy of the Commons”. Nature, 415, 424426.Google Scholar
Milward, S., Kita, S., & Apperly, I. A. (2014). The development of co-representation effects in a joint task: Do children represent a co-actor? Cognition, 132, 269279.Google Scholar
Moll, H., Richter, N., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2008). Fourteen-month-olds know what “we” have shared in a special way. Infancy, 13, 90101.Google Scholar
Ostrom, E. (2000). Collective action and the evolution of social norms. Journal of Economic Perspectives, 14, 137158.Google Scholar
Peacocke, C. (2005). Joint attention: Its nature, reflexivity, and relation to common knowledge. In Eilan, N., Hoerl, C., McCormack, T., & Roessler, J., eds., Joint Attention: Communication and Other Minds. Oxford: Clarendon Press, pp. 298324.Google Scholar
Raihani, N. J., & Bshary, R. (2011). The evolution of punishment in n-player games: A volunteer’s dilemma. Evolution, 65, 27252728.Google Scholar
Rakoczy, H., Warneken, F., & Tomasello, M. (2008). The sources of normativity: Young children’s awareness of the normative structure of games. Developmental Psychology, 44, 875881.Google Scholar
Rapoport, A., & Chammah, A. M. (1966). The game of chicken. American Behavioral Scientist, 10, 1028.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rekers, Y., Haun, D. B. M., & Tomasello, M. (2011). Children, but not chimpanzees, prefer to collaborate. Current Biology, 21, 17561758.Google Scholar
Schelling, T. (1960). The Strategy of Conflict. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Schmelz, M., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2011). Chimpanzees know that others make inferences. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108, 30773079.Google Scholar
Schmidt, M. F. H., Rakoczy, H., & Tomasello, M. (2011). Young children attribute normativity to novel actions without pedagogy or normative language. Developmental Science, 14, 530539.Google Scholar
Schmidt, M. F. H., Rakoczy, H., & Tomasello, M. (2012). Young children enforce social norms selectively depending on the violator’s group affiliation. Cognition, 124, 325333.Google Scholar
Schmidt, M. F. H., Butler, L. P., Heinz, J., & Tomasello, M. (2016). Young children see a single action and infer a social norm: Promiscuous normativity in 3-year-olds. Psychological Science, 27, 13601370.Google Scholar
Schneider, A. -C., Melis, A. P., & Tomasello, M. (2012). How chimpanzees solve collective action problems. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 279, 49464954.Google Scholar
Searle, J. R. (1990). Collective intentions and actions. In Cohen, P. R., Morgan, J., & Pollack, M. E., eds., Intentions in Communication. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 227241.Google Scholar
Searle, J. R. (1995). The Construction of Social Reality. London: Penguin.Google Scholar
Thomas, K. A., DeScioli, P., Haque, O. S., & Pinker, S. (2014). The psychology of coordination and common knowledge. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 107, 657676.Google Scholar
Tollefsen, D. (2005). Let’s pretend! Children and joint action. Philosophy of the Social Sciences, 35, 7597.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. (1995). Joint attention as social cognition. In Moore, C. & Dunham, P. J., eds., Joint Attention: Its Origins and Role in Development. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, pp. 103130.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. (2016). A Natural History of Human Morality. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M., & Carpenter, M. (2005). The emergence of social cognition in three young chimpanzees. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 70, vii132.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., Call, J., Behne, T., & Moll, H. (2005). Understanding and sharing intentions: The origins of cultural cognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28, 675735.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M., Melis, A., Tennie, C., Wyman, E., & Herrmann, E. (2012). Two key steps in the evolution of human: The interdependence hypothesis. Current Anthropology, 53, 673692.Google Scholar
Tomonaga, M., Tanaka, M., Matsuzawa, T., et al. (2004). Development of social cognition in infant chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): Face recognition, smiling, gaze, and the lack of triadic interactions. Japanese Psychological Research, 46, 227235.Google Scholar
Trevarthen, C. (1979). Communication and cooperation in early infancy. A description of primary intersubjectivity. In Bullowa, M., ed., Before Speech: The Beginning of Human Communication. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, pp. 321347.Google Scholar
Turner, J. (1997). The Institutional Order: Economy, Kinship, Religion, Polity, Law, and Education in Evolutionary and Comparative Perspective. New York: Longman.Google Scholar
Warneken, F., Chen, F., & Tomasello, M. (2006). Cooperative activities in young children and chimpanzees. Child Development, 77, 640663.Google Scholar
Warneken, W., Lohse, K., Melis, A. P., & Tomasello, M. (2011). Young children share the spoils after collaboration. Psychological Science, 22, 267273.Google Scholar
Warneken, F., Gräfenhain, M., & Tomasello, M. (2012). Collaborative partner or social tool? New evidence for young children’s understanding of joint intentions in collaborative activities. Developmental Science, 15, 5461.Google Scholar
Wyman, E. (2014). Language and collective fiction: From children’s pretense to social institutions. In Lewis, J., Knight, C., & Dor, D., eds., The Social Origins of Language. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 171183.Google Scholar
Wyman, E., Rakoczy, H., & Tomasello, M. (2009). Normativity and context in young children’s pretend play. Cognitive Development, 24, 146155.Google Scholar
Wyman, E., Rakoczy, H., & Tomasello, M. (2013). Non-verbal communication enables coordination with others in a children’s “Stag Hunt” game. European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 10, 597610.Google Scholar

References

Abramowitz, L. K., & Bartolomei, M. S. (2012). Genomic imprinting: Recognition and marking of imprinted loci. Current Opinion in Genetics and Development, 22, 72–8.Google Scholar
Bassel-Duby, R., & Olson, E. N. (2006). Signaling pathways in skeletal muscle remodeling. Annual Reviews Biochemistry, 75, 1937.Google Scholar
Bernal, A. J., Dolinoy, D. C., Huang, D., et al. (2013). Adaptive radiation-induced epigenetic alterations mitigated by antioxidants. FASEB Journal, 27(2), 665671.Google Scholar
Brown, W. M. (2001). Genomic imprinting and the cognitive architecture mediating human culture. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 1, 251–58.Google Scholar
Brown, W. M. (2011). The parental antagonism theory of language evolution: Preliminary evidence for the proposal. Human Biology, 83(2), 213245.Google Scholar
Brown, W. M. (2015). Exercise-associated DNA methylation change in skeletal muscle and the importance of imprinted genes: A bioinformatics meta-analysis. British Journal of Sports Medicine, 49(24), 15671578.Google Scholar
Brown, W. M. (2018). Conflict within: The epigenetics of troubled sleep. Clinical Psychiatry, 4, 24.Google Scholar
Brown, W. M., & Consedine, N. S. (2004). Just how happy is the happy puppet? An emotion signalling and kinship theory perspective on the behavioral phenotype of children with Angelman syndrome. Medical Hypotheses, 63, 377385.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, W. M., & Olding, R. J. (2017). Epigenetic-based hormesis and age-dependent altruism: Additions to the behavioural constellation of deprivation. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 40, e320.Google Scholar
Brown, W. M., Consedine, N. S., & Magai, C. (2005). Altruism relates to health in an ethnically diverse sample of older adults. Journals of Gerontology, 60B(3), 143152.Google Scholar
Bruni, O., Ferri, R., Dagostino, G., et al. (2004). Sleep disturbances in Angelman syndrome: A questionnaire study. Brain and Development, 26(4), 233240.Google Scholar
Buiting, K. (2010). Prader–Willi syndrome and Angelman syndrome. American Journal of Medical Genetics Part C: Seminars in Medical Genetics, 154C(3), 365376.Google Scholar
Buss, D. M. (1995). Evolutionary psychology: A new paradigm for psychological science. Psychological Inquiry, 6(1), 130.Google Scholar
Chalk, T. E. W., & Brown, W. M. (2014). Exercise epigenetics and the fetal origins of disease. Epigenomics, 6(5), 469472.Google Scholar
Cruickshanks, H. A., & Tufarelli, C. (2009). Isolation of cancer-specific chimeric transcripts induced by hypomethylation of the LINE-1 antisense promoter. Genomics, 94(6), 397406.Google Scholar
Dauber, A., Cunha-Silva, M., Macedo, D. B., et al. (2017). Paternally inherited DLK1 deletion associated with familial central precocious puberty. Journal of Clinical Endocrinology & Metabolism, 102(5), 15571567.Google Scholar
Deaton, A. M., & Bird, A. (2011). CpG islands and the regulation of transcription. Genes & Development, 25(10), 10101022.Google Scholar
Dent, C. L., & Isles, A. R. (2014). Brain-expressed imprinted genes and adult behaviour: The example of Nesp and Grb10. Mammalian Genome, 25(1–2), 8793.Google Scholar
Eggermann, T. (2009). Silver–Russell and Beckwith–Wiedemann syndromes: Opposite (epi)mutations in 11p15 result in opposite clinical pictures. Hormone Research in Paediatrics, 71(2), 3035.Google Scholar
Faria, G. S., Varela, S. A., & Gardner, A. (2017). Sexual selection modulates genetic conflicts and patterns of genomic imprinting. Evolution, 71(3), 526–40.Google Scholar
Gillman, A. S., Gardiner, C. K., Koljack, C. E., & Bryan, A. D. (2018). Body mass index, diet, and exercise: Testing possible linkages to breast cancer risk via DNA methylation. Breast Cancer Research and Treatment, 168(1), 241248.Google Scholar
Gluckman, P. D., Hanson, M. A., Buklijas, T., Low, F. M., & Beedle, A. S. (2009). Epigenetic mechanisms that underpin metabolic and cardiovascular diseases. Nature Reviews Endocrinology, 5(7), 401408.Google Scholar
Haig, D. (1993). Genetic conflicts in human pregnancy. Quarterly Review of Biology, 68(4), 495532.Google Scholar
Haig, D. (1999). Asymmetric relations. Evolution and Human Behavior, 20(2), 8398.Google Scholar
Haig, D. (2000). Genomic imprinting, sex-biased dispersal, and social behavior. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 907(1), 149163.Google Scholar
Haig, D. (2002). Genomic Imprinting and Kinship. Piscataway, NJ: Rutgers University Press.Google Scholar
Haig, D. (2003). On intrapersonal reciprocity. Evolution and Human Behavior, 24, 418425.Google Scholar
Haig, D. (2010). Transfers and transitions: Parent–offspring conflict, genomic imprinting, and the evolution of human life history. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107(Suppl. 1), 17311735.Google Scholar
Haig, D., & Wharton, R. (2003). Prader–Willi syndrome and the evolution of human childhood. American Journal of Human Biology, 15(3), 320329.Google Scholar
Hamilton, W. D. (1987). Discriminating nepotism: Expectable, common, overlooked. In Fletcher, D. J. C & Michener, C. D, eds., Kin Recognition in Animals. New York: Wiley, pp. 417438. Reprinted in Hamilton, W. D. (2001). Narrow Roads of Gene Land: Volume 2, Evolution of Sex. Oxford: W.H. Freeman, pp. 345366.Google Scholar
Horvath, S. (2013). DNA methylation age of human tissues and cell types. Genome Biology, 14(10), R115.Google Scholar
Horvath, S. (2015). Erratum to: DNA methylation age of human tissues and cell types. Genome Biology, 16, 96.Google Scholar
Hupkes, M., Jonsson, M. K., Scheenen, W. J., et al. (2011). Epigenetics: DNA demethylation promotes skeletal myotube maturation. FASEB Journal, 25(11), 38613872.Google Scholar
Kong, A., Steinthorsdottir, V., Masson, G., et al. (2009). Parental origin of sequence variants associated with complex diseases. Nature, 462(7275), 868874.Google Scholar
Mackay, D. J., & Temple, I. K. (2010). Transient neonatal diabetes mellitus type 1. American Journal of Medical Genetics Part C: Seminars in Medical Genetics, 154C(3), 335342.Google Scholar
Maeda, T., Oyama, J., Higuchi, Y., et al. (2011). The physical ability of Japanese female elderly with cerebrovascular disease correlates with the telomere length and subtelomeric methylation status in their peripheral blood leukocytes. Gerontology, 57(2), 137143.Google Scholar
McNamara, P. (2014). Comment on David Haig’s troubled sleep: Implications for functions of infant sleep. Evolution, Medicine, and Public Health, 2014(1), 5456.Google Scholar
Mehr, S. A., Kotler, J., Howard, R. M., Haig, D., & Krasnow, M. M. (2017). Genomic imprinting is implicated in the psychology of music. Psychological Science, 28(10), 14551467.Google Scholar
Micheletti, A. J., Ruxton, G. D., & Gardner, A. (2017). Intrafamily and intragenomic conflicts in human warfare. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 284(1849), 201z2699.Google Scholar
Monk, D., Arnaud, P., Frost, J., et al. (2009). Reciprocal imprinting of human GRB10 in placental trophoblast and brain: Evolutionary conservation of reversed allelic expression. Human Molecular Genetics, 18(16), 30663074.Google Scholar
Morales-Lara, D., De-La-Peña, C., & Murillo-Rodríguez, E. (2018). Dad’s snoring may have left molecular scars in your DNA: The emerging role of epigenetics in sleep disorders. Molecular Neurobiology, 55(4), 27132724.Google Scholar
Morison, I. M. (2001). The imprinted gene and parent-of-origin effect database. Nucleic Acids Research, 29(1), 275276.Google Scholar
Murphy, S. K. (2006). Callipyge mutation affects gene expression in cis: A potential role for chromatin structure. Genome Research, 16(3), 340346.Google Scholar
Park, S. S., Skaar, D. A., Jirtle, R. L., & Hoyo, C. (2017). Epigenetics, obesity and early-life cadmium or lead exposure. Epigenomics, 9(1), 5775.Google Scholar
Pepper, G. V., & Nettle, D. (2017). The behavioural constellation of deprivation: Causes and consequences. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 40, e346.Google Scholar
Portela, A., & Esteller, M. (2010). Epigenetic modifications and human disease. Nature Biotechnology, 28(10), 10571068.Google Scholar
SanMiguel, J. M., & Bartolomei, M. S. (2018). DNA methylation dynamics of genomic imprinting in mouse development. Biology of Reproduction, 99, 252262.Google Scholar
Santos, L., Elliott-Sale, K. J., & Sale, C. (2017). Exercise and bone health across the lifespan. Biogerontology, 18(6), 931946.Google Scholar
Small, K. S., Hedman, Å. K., Grundberg, E., et al. (2011). Identification of an imprinted master trans regulator at the KLF14 locus related to multiple metabolic phenotypes. Nature Genetics, 43(6), 561564.Google Scholar
Sone, S. (1994). Muscle histochemistry in the Prader–Willi syndrome. Brain & Development, 16, 183188.Google Scholar
Swartz, J. R., Hariri, A. R., & Williamson, D. E. (2017). An epigenetic mechanism links socioeconomic status to changes in depression-related brain function in high-risk adolescents. Molecular Psychiatry, 22, 209214.Google Scholar
Taylor, S. M., & Jones, P. A. (1979). Multiple new phenotypes induced in 10T1/2 and 3T3 cells treated with 5-azacytidine. Cell, 17, 771779.Google Scholar
Tucci, V. (2016). Genomic imprinting: A new epigenetic perspective of sleep regulation. PLoS Genetics, 12(5), e1006004.Google Scholar
Ubeda, F., & Gardner, A. (2011). A model for genomic imprinting in the social brain: Adults. Evolution, 65, 462475.Google Scholar
Waterland, R. A., & Jirtle, R. L. (2003). Transposable elements: Targets for early nutritional effects on epigenetic gene regulation. Molecular and Cellular Biology, 23(15), 52935300.Google Scholar
Wilson, E. M., & Rotwein, P. (2006). Control of MyoD function during Initiation of muscle differentiation by an autocrine signaling pathway activated by insulin-like growth factor-II. Journal of Biological Chemistry, 281(40), 2996229971.Google Scholar
Zeng, H., Irwin, M. L., Lu, L., et al. (2012). Physical activity and breast cancer survival: An epigenetic link through reduced methylation of a tumor suppressor gene L3MBTL1. Breast Cancer Research and Treatment, 133(1), 127135.Google Scholar

References

Andreou, E. (2004). Bully/victim problems and their association with Machiavellianism and self-efficacy in Greek primary school children. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 74, 297309.Google Scholar
Archer, J. (1988). The Behavioural Biology of Aggression. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Archer, J. (2009). Does sexual selection explain human sex differences in aggression? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 32, 249311.Google Scholar
Arnocky, S., & Vaillancourt, T. (2012). A multi-informant longitudinal study on the relationship between aggression, peer victimization, and dating status in adolescence. Evolutionary Psychology, 10, 253270.Google Scholar
Ball, H. A., Arsenault, L., Taylor, A., et al. (2008). Genetic and environmental influences on victims, bullies and bully–victims in childhood. Journal of Child Psychiatry and Psychiatry 49, 104112.Google Scholar
Bertolotti, T., & Magnani, L. (2013). A philosophical and evolutionary approach to cyber-bullying: social networks and the disruption of sub-moralities. Ethics and Information Technology, 15, 285299.Google Scholar
Bjorklund, D. F., & Hawley, P. H. (2014). Aggression grows up: Looking through an evolutionary developmental lens to understand the causes and consequences of human aggression. In Shackelford, T. K. & Hansen, R. D., eds., The Evolution of Violence. New York: Springer, pp. 159186.Google Scholar
Blackwell, T. (2015). Provocative new study finds bullies have highest self esteem, social status, lowest rates of depression. https://nationalpost.com/health/provocative-new-study-finds-bullies-have-highest-self-esteem-social-status-lowest-rates-of-depression.Google Scholar
Boivin, M., Brendgen, M., Vitaro, F., et al. (2012). Strong genetic contribution to peer relationship difficulties at school entry: Findings from a longitudinal twin study. Child Development, 84, 10981114.Google Scholar
Book, A. S., Volk, A. A., & Hosker, A. (2012). Adolescent bullying and personality: An adaptive approach. Personality and Individual Differences, 52, 218223.Google Scholar
Bowes, L., Joinson, C., Wolke, D., & Lewis, G. (2015). Peer victimisation during adolescence and its impact on depression in early adulthood: Prospective cohort study in the United Kingdom. British Medical Journal, 350, h2469.Google Scholar
Campbell, D. T. (1975). On the conflicts between biological and social evolution and between psychology and moral tradition. American Psychologist, 30, 11031126.Google Scholar
Connolly, J., Pepler, D., Craig, W., & Taradash, A. (2000). Dating experiences of bullies in early adolescence. Child Maltreatment, 5, 299310.Google Scholar
Cook, C. R., Williams, K. R., Guerra, N. G., Kim, T. E., & Sadek, S. (2010). Predictors of bullying and victimization in childhood and adolescence: A meta-analytic investigation. School Psychology Quarterly, 25, 6583.Google Scholar
Copeland, W. E., Wolke, D., Angold, A., & Costello, J. (2013). Adult psychiatric outcomes of bullying and being bullied by peers in childhood and adolescence. Journal of the American Medical Association: Psychiatry, 70, 419426.Google Scholar
Crick, N. R., & Dodge, K. (1994). A review and reformulation of social-information-processing mechanisms in children’s social adjustment. Psychological Bulletin, 115, 74101.Google Scholar
Crick, N., & Dodge, K. (1996). Social information processing mechanisms in reactive and proactive aggression. Child Development, 67, 9931002.Google Scholar
Crick, N. R., & Dodge, K. A. (1999). “Superiority” is in the eye of the beholder: A comment on Sutton, Smith and Swettenham. Social Development, 8, 128131.Google Scholar
Dane, A. V., Marini, Z. A. Volk, A. A., & Vaillancourt, T. (2017). Physical and relational bullying and victimization: Differential relations with adolescent dating and sexual behavior. Aggressive Behavior, 43, 111122.Google Scholar
Dunbar, R. I. M. (2004). Gossip in an evolutionary perspective. Review of General Psychology, 8, 100110.Google Scholar
Elgar, F. J., Craig, W., Boyce, W., Morgan, A., & Vella-Zarb, R. (2009). Income inequality and school bullying: Multilevel study of adolescents in 37 countries. Journal of Adolescent Health, 45, 351359.Google Scholar
Ellis, B. J., del Guidice, M., Dishion, T. J., et al. (2012). The evolutionary basis of risk taking behavior: Implications for science, policy, and practice. Developmental Psychology, 48, 598623.Google Scholar
Ellis, B. J., Volk, A. A., Gonzalez, J.-M., & Embry, D. D. (2016). The meaningful roles intervention: An evolutionary approach to reducing bullying and increasing prosocial behavior. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 26, 622637.Google Scholar
Espelage, D., & Holt, M. K. (2007). Dating violence and sexual harassment across the bully–victim continuum among middle and high school students. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 36, 799811.Google Scholar
Farrell, A. H., della Cioppa, V., Volk, A. A., & Book, A. S. (2014). Predicting bullying heterogeneity with the HEXACO model of personality. International Journal of Advances in Psychology, 3, 3039.Google Scholar
Farrington, D. P., Lösel, F., Ttofi, M. M., & Theodorakis, N. (2012). School Bullying, Depression and Offending Behaviour Later in Life: An Updated Systematic Review of Longitudinal Studies. Stockholm: Swedish National Council for Crime Prevention.Google Scholar
Fekkes, M., Pijpers, F. I. M., & Verloove-Vanhorick, S. P. (2004). Bullying behaviour and associations with psychosomatic complaints and depression in victims. Journal of Pediatrics, 144, 1722.Google Scholar
Festl, R., Vogelgesang, J., Scharkow, M., & Quandt, T. (2017). Longitudinal patterns of involvement in cyberbullying: Results from a latent transition analysis. Computers in Human Behavior, 66, 715.Google Scholar
Forero, R., McLellan, L., Rissel, C., & Bauman, A. (1999). Bullying behaviour and psychosocial health among school students in New South Wales, Australia: Cross sectional survey. British Medical Journal, 319, 344348.Google Scholar
Fry, D. P. (2004). The Human Potential for Peace: Challenging the War Assumption. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Garandeau, C. F., Lee, I. A., & Salmivalli, C. (2014). Differential effects of the KiVa antibullying program on popular and unpopular bullies. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 35, 4450.Google Scholar
Gibb, Z. G., & Devereux, P. G. (2014). Who does that anyway? Predictors and personality correlates of cyberbullying in college. Computers in Human Behavior, 38, 816.Google Scholar
Gini, G. (2008). Associations between bullying behaviour, psychosomatic complaints, emotional and behavioural problems. Journal of Paediatrics and Child Health, 44, 492497.Google Scholar
Gini, G., & Pozzoli, T. (2009). Association between bullying and psychosomatic symptoms: A meta-analysis. Pediatrics, 123, 10591065.Google Scholar
Gray, P. (2011). The special value of children’s age-mixed play. American Journal of Play, 3, 500522.Google Scholar
Greene, M. B. (2006). Bullying in schools: A plea for a measure of human rights. Journal of Social Issues, 62, 6379.Google Scholar
Guerra, N., Williams, K. R., & Sadek, S. (2011). Understanding bullying and victimization during childhood and adolescence: A mixed methods study. Child Development, 82, 295310.Google Scholar
Hawley, P. (2003). Strategies of control, aggression, and morality in preschoolers: An evolutionary perspective. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 85, 213235.Google Scholar
Hawley, P., Little, T. D., & Rodkin, P., eds. (2007). Aggression and Adaptation: The Bright Side to Bad Behavior. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Haynie, D. L., Nansel, T., Eitel, P., et al. (2001). Bullies, victims, and bully/victims: Distinct groups of at-risk youth. Journal of Early Adolescence, 21, 2949.Google Scholar
Hellfeldt, K., Gill, P. E., & Johansson, B. (2018). Longitudinal analysis of links between bullying victimization and psychosomatic maladjustment in Swedish schoolchildren. Journal of School Violence, 17, 8698.Google Scholar
Hong, J. S., & Espelage, D. L. (2012). A review of research on bullying and peer victimization in school: An ecological systems analysis. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 17, 311322.Google Scholar
Houser, J. J., Mayeux, L., & Cross, C. (2015). Peer status and aggression as predictors of dating popularity in adolescence. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 44, 683695.Google Scholar
Ingram, G. P. D. (2014). From hitting to tattling to gossip: An evolutionary rationale for the development of indirect aggression. Evolutionary Psychology, 12, 343363.Google Scholar
Ivarsson, T., Broberg, A. G., Arvidsson, T., & Gillberg, C. (2005). Bullying in adolescence: Psychiatric problems in victims and bullies as measured by the Youth Self Report (YSR) and the Depression Self-Rating Scale (DSRS). Nordic Journal of Psychiatry, 59, 365373.Google Scholar
Jolliffe, D., & Farrington, D. P. (2011). Is low empathy related to bullying after controlling for individual and social background variables? Journal of Adolescence, 34, 5971.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Juvonen, J., Graham, S., & Schuster, M. A. (2003). Bullying among young adolescents: The strong, the weak, and the troubled. Pediatrics, 112, 12311237.Google Scholar
Kaltiala-Heino, R., Rimpelä, M., Marttunen, M., Rimpelä, A., & Rantanen, P. (1999). Bullying, depression and suicidal ideation in Finnish adolescents: School survey. British Medical Journal, 319, 348351.Google Scholar
Kaukiainen, A., Björkqvist, K., Lagerspetz, K., et al. (1999). The relationship between social intelligence, empathy, and three types of aggression. Aggressive Behavior, 25, 8189.Google Scholar
Koh, J.-B., & Wong, J. S. (2017). Survival of the fittest and the sexiest: Evolutionary origins of adolescent bullying. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 32, 26682690.Google Scholar
Kolbert, J. B., & Crothers, L. M. (2003). Bullying and evolutionary psychology: The dominance hierarchy among students and implications for school personnel. Journal of School Violence, 2, 7391.Google Scholar
Kowalski, R. M., & Limber, S. P. (2013). Psychological, physical, and academic correlates of cyberbullying and traditional bullying. Journal of Adolescent Health, 53, S13S20.Google Scholar
Kowalski, R. M., Giumetti, G. W., Schroeder, A. N., & Lattanner, M. R. (2014). Bullying in the digital age: A critical review and meta-analysis of cyberbullying research among youth. Psychological Bulletin, 140, 10731137.Google Scholar
Kumpulainen, K., Räsänen, E., & Henttonen, I. (1999). Children involved in bullying: Psychological disturbance and the persistence of the involvement. Child Abuse & Neglect, 23, 12531262.Google Scholar
Le, H. T. H., Nguyen, H. T., Campbell, M. A., et al. (2017). Longitudinal associations between bullying and mental health among adolescents in Vietnam. International Journal of Public Health, 62(Suppl. 1), 5161.Google Scholar
Lereya, S. T., Samara, M., & Wolke, D. (2013). Parenting behavior and the risk of becoming a victim and a bully/victim: A meta-analysis study. Child Abuse & Neglect, 37, 10911108.Google Scholar
Liang, H., Flisher, A. J., & Lombard, C. J. (2007). Bullying, violence, and risk behavior in South African school students. Child Abuse & Neglect, 31, 161171.Google Scholar
Livingstone, S., & Smith, P. K. (2014). Research review: Harms experienced by child users of online and mobile technologies: The nature, prevalence and management of sexual and aggressive risks in the digital age. Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry, 55, 635654.Google Scholar
Mishna, F., Khoury-Kassabrib, M., Gadallaa, T., & Dacuika, J. (2012). Risk factors for involvement in cyber bullying: Victims, bullies and bully–victims. Children and Youth Services Review, 34, 6370.Google Scholar
Monks, C. P. (2011). Peer-victimisation in preschool. In Monks, C. P. & Coyne, I., eds., Bullying in Different Contexts. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, pp. 1235.Google Scholar
Monks, C. P., Smith, P. K., Naylor, P., et al. (2009). Bullying in different contexts: commonalities, differences and the role of theory. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 14, 146156.Google Scholar
Muñoz, L. C., Qualter, P., & Padgett, G. (2011). Empathy and bullying: Exploring the influence of callous–unemotional traits. Child Psychiatry & Human Development, 42, 183196.Google Scholar
Nansel, T. R., Craig, W., Overpeck, M. D., Saluja, G., Ruan, W. J., & The Health Behaviour in School-aged Children Bullying Analyses Working Group. (2004). Cross-national consistency in the relationship between bullying behaviors and psychosocial adjustment. Archives of Pediatrics and Adolescent Medicine, 158, 730736.Google Scholar
Nordhagen, R., Nielsen, A., Stigum, H., & Köhler, L. (2005). Parental reported bullying among Nordic children: A population-based study. Child: Care, Health & Development, 31, 693701.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Olthof, T., Goossens, F. A., Vermande, M. M., Aleva, E. A., & van der Meulen, M. (2011). Bullying as strategic behavior: Relations with desired and acquired dominance in the peer group. Journal of School Psychology, 49, 339359.Google Scholar
Olweus, D. (1993). Bullying at School: What We Know and What We Can Do. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Olweus, D. (1999). Sweden. In Smith, P. K., Morita, Y., Junger-Tas, J., et al., eds., The Nature of School Bullying: A Cross-National Perspective. London/New York: Routledge, pp. 727.Google Scholar
O’Moore, M. (2000). Critical issues for teacher training to counter bullying and victimisation in Ireland. Aggressive Behavior, 26, 99111.Google Scholar
Peeters, M., Cillessen, A. H. N., & Scholte, R. H. J. (2010). Clueless or powerful? Identifying subtypes of bullies in adolescence. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 39, 10411052.Google Scholar
Reijntjes, A., Vermande, M., Goossens, F. A., et al. (2013a). Developmental trajectories of bullying and social dominance in youth. Child Abuse and Neglect, 37, 224234.Google Scholar
Reijntjes, A., Vermande, M., Olthof, T., et al. (2013b). Costs and benefits of bullying in the context of the peer group: A three wave longitudinal analysis. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 41, 12171229.Google Scholar
Rigby, K. (1997). Attitudes and beliefs about bullying among Australian school children. Irish Journal of Psychology, 18, 202220.Google Scholar
Rivers, I., Poteat, V. P., Noret, N., & Ashurst, N. (2009). Observing bullying at school: The mental health implications of witness status. School Psychology Quarterly, 24, 211223.Google Scholar
Salmivalli, C. (2010). Bullying and the peer group: A review. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 15, 112120.Google Scholar
Salmivalli, C., Kaukiainen, A., Kaistaniemi, L., & Lagerspetz, K. (1999). Self-evaluated self-esteem, peer-evaluated self-esteem, and defensive egotism as predictors of adolescents’ participation in bullying situations. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 25, 12681278.Google Scholar
Salmivalli, C., Lagerspetz, K., Björkqvist, K., Österman, K., & Kaukiainen, A. (1996). Bullying as a group process: Participant roles and their relations to social status within the group. Aggressive Behavior, 22, 115.Google Scholar
Schnohr, C., & Niclasen, B. V.-L. (2006). Bullying among Greenlandic school-children: Development since 1994 and relations to health and health behaviour. International Journal of Circumpolar Health, 65, 305312.Google Scholar
Smith, P. K. (2007). Why has aggression been thought of as maladaptive? In Hawley, P., Little, T. D, & Rodkin, P, eds., Aggression and Adaptation. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum, pp. 6583.Google Scholar
Smith, P. K. (2014). Understanding School Bullying: Its Nature & Prevention Strategies. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.Google Scholar
Smith, P. K. (2017). Bullying and theory of mind: A review. Current Psychiatry Reviews, 13, 9095.Google Scholar
Smith, P. K., ed. (2019). Making an Impact on School Bullying: Interventions and Recommendations. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Smith, P. K., Madsen, K., & Moody, J. (1999). What causes the age decline in reports of being bullied at school? Towards a developmental analysis of risks of being bullied. Educational Research, 41, 267285.Google Scholar
Smith, P. K., Shu, S., & Madsen, K. (2001). Characteristics of victims of school bullying: Developmental changes in coping strategies and skills. In Juvonen, J. & Graham, S., eds., Peer Harassment at School: The Plight of the Vulnerable and Victimised. New York: Guildford, pp. 332352.Google Scholar
Stein, J. A., Dukes, R. L., & Warren, J. I. (2007). Adolescent male bullies, victims, and bully–victims: A comparison of psychosocial and behavioral characteristics. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 32, 272282.Google Scholar
Sutton, J., & Keogh, E. (2000). Social competition in school: Relationships with bullying, Machiavellianism and personality. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 70, 443456.Google Scholar
Sutton, J., Smith, P. K., & Swettenham, J. (1999a). Bullying and “theory of mind”: A critique of the “social skills deficit” view of anti-social behaviour. Social Development, 8, 117127.Google Scholar
Sutton, J., Smith, P. K., & Swettenham, J. (1999b). Social cognition and bullying: Social inadequacy or skilled manipulation? British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 17, 435450.Google Scholar
Sutton, J., Smith, P. K., & Swettenham, J. (1999c). Socially undesirable need not be incompetent: A response to Crick and Dodge. Social Development, 8, 132134.Google Scholar
Tani, F., Greenman, P. S., Schneider, B. H., & Fregoso, M. (2003). Bullying and the Big Five: A study of childhood personality and participant roles in bullying incidents. School Psychology International, 24, 131146.Google Scholar
Ttofi, M. M., Farrington, D. P., & Lösel, F. (2011). Do the victims of school bullies tend to become depressed later in life? A systematic review and meta-analysis of longitudinal studies. Journal of Aggression, Conflict and Peace Research, 3, 6373.Google Scholar
Ttofi, M. M., Farrington, D. P., & Lösel, F. (2012). School bullying as a predictor of violence later in life: A systematic review and meta-analysis of prospective longitudinal studies. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 17, 405418.Google Scholar
Vaillancourt, T., Hymel, S., & McDougall, P. (2003). Bullying is power: Implications for school-based intervention strategies. Journal of Applied School Psychology, 19, 157176.Google Scholar
Veenstra, R., Lindenberg, S., Oldehinkel, A.J., et al. (2005). Bullying and victimization in elementary schools: Victims, bully/victims, and uninvolved adolescents. Developmental Psychology, 41, 672682.Google Scholar
Volk, A., Craig, W., Boyce, W., & King, M. (2006). Adolescent risk correlates of bullying and different types of victimization. International Journal of Adolescent Medicine and Health, 18, 375386.Google Scholar
Volk, A. A., Camilleri, J. A., Dane, A. V., & Marini, Z. A. (2012). Is adolescent bullying an evolutionary adaptation? Aggressive Behavior, 38, 222238.Google Scholar
Volk, A. A., Dane, A. V., Marini, Z. A., & Vaillancourt, T. (2015). Adolescent bullying, dating, and mating: Testing an evolutionary hypothesis. Evolutionary Psychology, 13, 111.Google Scholar
Volk, A. A., Farrell, A. H., Franklin, P., Mularczyk, K. P., & Provenzano, D. A. (2016). Adolescent bullying in schools: An evolutionary perspective. In Geary, D. C. & Berch, D. B., eds., Evolutionary Perspectives on Child Development and Education. Basel: Springer, pp. 167191.Google Scholar
Wang, J., Nansel, T. R., & Iannotti, R. J. (2011). Cyber and traditional bullying: Differential association. Journal of Adolescent Health, 48, 415417.Google Scholar
Winsper, C., Lereya, T., Zanarini, M., & Wolke, D. (2012). Involvement in bullying and suicide-related behavior at 11 years: A prospective birth cohort study. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 51, 271282.Google Scholar
Wolke, D., & Lereya, S. T. (2015). Long-term effects of bullying. Archives of Disease in Childhood, 100, 879885.Google Scholar
Wolke, D., Tippett, N., & Dantchev, S. (2015). Bullying in the family: Sibling bullying. Lancet Psychiatry, 2, 917929.Google Scholar
Wolke, D., Woods, S., Bloomfield, I., & Karstadt, L. (2001). Bullying involvement in primary school and common health problems. Archives of Disease in Childhood, 85, 197201.Google Scholar
Yang, A., Li, X., & Salmivalli, C. (2016). Maladjustment of bully–victims: validation with three identification methods. Educational Psychology, 36, 13901407.Google Scholar
Yeager, D. S., Fong, C. J., Lee, H. Y., & Espelage, D. L. (2015). Declines in efficacy of anti-bullying programs among older adolescents: Theory and a three-level meta-analysis. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 37, 3651.Google Scholar
Zwierzynska, K., Wolke, D., & Lereya, T. S. (2013). Peer victimization in childhood and internalizing problems in adolescence: A prospective longitudinal study. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 41, 309323.Google Scholar

References

Adler, A. (1927). Understanding Human Nature. New York: Greenberg.Google Scholar
Adler, A. (1928). Characteristics of the first, second, and third child. Children, 3, 1452.Google Scholar
Altus, W. D. (1966). Birth order and its sequelae. Science, 151, 4449.Google Scholar
Argys, L. M., Rees, D. I., Averett, S. L., & Witoonchart, B. (2005). Birth order and risky adolescent behavior. Economic Inquiry, 44, 215233.Google Scholar
Barclay, K. J., & Kolk, M. (2015). Birth order and mortality: A population-based cohort study. Demography, 52, 613639.Google Scholar
Barni, D., Roccato, M., Vieno, A., & Alfieri, S. (2014). Birth order and conservatism: A multilevel test of Sulloway’s “born to rebel” thesis. Personality and Individual Differences, 66, 5863.Google Scholar
Baskett, L. M. (1985). Sibling status: Adult expectations. Developmental Psychology, 21, 441445.Google Scholar
Beck, E., Burnet, K. L., & Vosper, J. (2006). Birth-order effects on facets of extraversion. Personality and Individual Differences, 40, 953959.Google Scholar
Beer, J., & Horn, J. M. (2000). The influence of rearing order on personality development within two adoption cohorts. Journal of Personality, 68, 789819.Google Scholar
Belmont, L., & Marolla, F. A. (1973). Birth order, family size, and intelligence. Science, 182, 10961101.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blanchard, R. (1997). Birth order and sibling sex ratio in homosexual versus heterosexual males and females. Annual Review of Sex Research, 8, 2767.Google Scholar
Blanchard, R. (2004). Quantitative and theoretical analyses of the relation between older brothers and homosexuality in men. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 230, 173187.Google Scholar
Bleske-Rechek, A., & Kelley, J. A. (2014). Birth order and personality: A within-family test using independent self-reports from both firstborn and laterborn siblings. Personality and Individual Differences, 56, 1518.Google Scholar
Block, J. (1995). A contrarian view of the Five-Factor approach to personality description. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 187215.Google Scholar
Boone, J. L. (1986). Parental investment and elite family structure in preindustrial states: A case study of late medieval-early modern Portuguese genealogies. American Anthropologist, 88, 859878.Google Scholar
Brim, O. G. Jr. (1958). Family structure and sex role learning by children: A further analysis of Helen Koch’s data. Sociometry, 21, 116.Google Scholar
Bu, F. (2016). Examining sibling configuration effects on young people’s educational aspiration and attainment. Advances in Life Course Research, 27, 6979.Google Scholar
Carette, B., Anseel, F., & Van Yperen, N. W. (2011). Born to learn or born to win? Birth order effects on achievement goals. Journal of Research in Personality, 45, 500503.Google Scholar
Clark, C., & Clark, J. (2008). Women at the Polls: The Gender Gap, Cultural Politics, and Contested Constituencies in the United States. Newcastle upon Tyne: Cambridge Scholars Publishing.Google Scholar
Clark, R. D., & Rice, G. A. (1982). Family constellations and eminence: The birth orders of Nobel Prize winners. Journal of Psychology, 110, 281287.Google Scholar
Claxton, R. (1994). Empirical relationships between birth order and two types of parental feedback. Psychological Record, 44, 475487.Google Scholar
Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1992). NEO PI-R Professional Manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.Google Scholar
Courtiol, A., Raymond, M., & Faurie, C. (2009). Birth order affects behaviour in the investment game: Firstborns are less trustful and reciprocate less. Animal Behaviour, 78, 14051411.Google Scholar
Daly, M., & Wilson, M. (1988). Homicide. New York: Aldine de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Damian, R. I., & Roberts, B. W. (2015). The associations of birth order with personality and intelligence in a representative sample of U.S. high school students. Journal of Research in Personality, 58, 96105.Google Scholar
Darwin, C. (1859). On The Origin of Species by Means of Natural Selection, or Preservation of Favoured Races in the Struggle for Life. London: John Murray.Google Scholar
Dixon, M. M., Reyes, C. J., Leppert, M. F., & Pappas, L. M. (2008). Personality and birth order in large families. Personality and Individual Differences, 44, 119128.Google Scholar
Downey, D. (2001). Number of sibling and intellectual development: The resource dilution explanation. American Psychologist, 56, 497504.Google Scholar
Drummond, H., & García-Chevelas, C. (1989). Food shortage influences sibling aggression in the blue-footed booby. Animal Behavior, 37, 806818.Google Scholar
Duby, G. (1977). The Chivalrous Society, trans. by C. Poston. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.Google Scholar
Ernst, C., & Angst, J. (1983). Birth Order: Its Influence on Personality. Berlin/New York: Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar
Feingold, A. (1994). Gender differences in personality: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 116, 429456.Google Scholar
Førland, T. E., Korsvik, T. R., & Christophersen, K-A. (2012). Brought up to rebel in the sixties: Birth order irrelevant, parental worldview decisive. Political Psychology, 33, 825838.Google Scholar
Freese, J., Powell, B., & Steelman, L. C. (1999). Rebel without a cause or effect: Birth order and social attitudes. American Sociological Review, 64, 207231.Google Scholar
Galton, F. (1874). English Men of Science. London: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Hamilton, W. (1964). The genetical evolution of social behavior. Parts 1 and II. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 7, 116; 1752.Google Scholar
Harris, J. R. (1998). The Nurture Assumption: Why Children Turn Out the Way They Do. New York: Free Press.Google Scholar
Harris, J. R. (2000). Context-specific learning, personality, and birth order. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 9, 174177.Google Scholar
Healey, M., & Ellis, B. (2007). Birth order, conscientiousness, and openness to experience: Tests of the family-niche model of personality using a within-family methodology. Evolution and Human Behavior, 38, 5559.Google Scholar
Herlihy, D. (1977). Family and property in Renaissance Florence. In Herlihy, D & Udovitch, A. L, eds., The Medieval City. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press, pp. 324.Google Scholar
Herrera, N. C., Zajonc, R. B., Wieczorkowska, G., & Cichomski, B. (2003). Beliefs about birth rank and their reflection in reality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 142150.Google Scholar
Hertwig, R., Davis, J. N., & Sulloway, F. J. (2002). Parental investment: How an equity motive can produce inequality. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 728745.Google Scholar
Hilton, I. (1967). Differences in the behavior of mothers toward first- and later-born children. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 7, 282290.Google Scholar
Horton, S. (1988). Birth order and child nutritional status: evidence from the Philippines. Economic Development and Cultural Change, 36, 341354.Google Scholar
Hrdy, S., & Judge, D. S. (1993). Darwin and the puzzle of primogeniture. Human Nature, 4, 145.Google Scholar
Jang, K. L., Livesley, W. J., Angeleitner, A., Riemann, R., & Vernon, P. A. (2002). Genetic and environmental influences on the covariance of facets defining the domains of the five-factor model of personality. Personality and Individual Differences, 33, 83101.Google Scholar
Jefferson, T., Herbst, J. H., & McCrae, R. R. (1998). Associations between birth order and personality traits: Evidence from self-reports and observer ratings. Journal of Research in Personality, 32, 498509.Google Scholar
Kidwell, J. S. (1982). The neglected birth order: Middleborns. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 44, 225235.Google Scholar
Kobayashi, T., Hasegawa, T., Hiraiwa-Hasegawa, M., & Kurashima, O. (2001). The effect of birth order on personality and familial sentiment in Japan. Poster presented at the 13th conference of the Human Behavior and Evolution Society, University College of London, London.Google Scholar
Koch, H. (1955). Some personality correlates of sex, sibling position, and sex of sibling among five- and six-year-old children. Genetic Psychology Monographs, 52, 350.Google Scholar
Koch, H. (1956). Attitudes of young children toward their peers as related to certain characteristics of their siblings. Psychological Monographs, 70, 141.Google Scholar
Krishnamurthy, K. S., Shaanker, R. U., & Ganeshaiah, K. N. (1997). Seed abortion in an animal dispersed species, Syzgium cuminii (L.). Skeels (Myrtaceae): The chemical basis. Current Science, 73, 869873.Google Scholar
Kristensen, P., & Bjerkedal, T. (2007). Explaining the relation between birth order and intelligence. Science, 316, 1717.Google Scholar
Kuhn, T. S. (1962). The Structure of Scientific Revolutions. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Kulik, L. (2004). The impact of birth order on intergenerational transmission of attitudes from parents to adolescent sons: The Israeli case. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 33, 149157.Google Scholar
Lehmann, J.-Y. K., Nuevo-Chiquero, A., & Vidal-Fernandez, M. (2018). The early origins of birth order differences in children’s outcomes and parental behavior. Journal of Human Resources, 53, 123156.Google Scholar
Lindert, P. H. (1977). Sibling position and achievement. Journal of Human Resources, 12, 198219.Google Scholar
Loehlin, J. C. (1992). Genes and Environment in Personality Development. Newbury Park, CA: Sage Publications.Google Scholar
MacAndrew, F. T., King, J. C., & Honoroff, L. R. (2002). A sociological analysis of namesaking patterns in 322 American families. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 32, 851864.Google Scholar
Manaster, G. J., Rhodes, C., Marcus, M. B., & Chan, J. C. (1998). The role of birth order in the acculturation of Japanese Americans. Psychologia, 41, 155170.Google Scholar
Marini, V. A., & Kurtz, J. E. (2011). Birth order differences in normal personality traits: Perspectives from within and outside the family. Personality and Individual Differences, 51, 910914.Google Scholar
Michalski, R. L., & Shackelford, T. K. (2002). An attempted replication of the relationship between birth order and personality. Journal of Research in Personality, 36, 182186.Google Scholar
Mischel, W., & Shoda, Y. (1995). A cognitive–affective system theory of personality: Reconceptualizing situations, dispositions, dynamics, and invariance in personality structure. Psychological Review, 102, 246268.Google Scholar
Mock, D. W., Drummond, H., & Stinson, C. H. (1990). Avian siblicide. American Scientist, 78, 438449.Google Scholar
Modin, B. (2002). Birth order and mortality: A life-long follow-up of 14,200 boys and girls born in early 20th century Sweden. Social Science & Medicine, 54, 10511064.Google Scholar
Musun-Miller, L. (1993). Sibling status effects: Parents’ perceptions of their own children. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 154, 189198.Google Scholar
Numbers, R. L. (1998). Darwinism Comes to America. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Nyman, L. (1995). The identification of birth order personality attributes. Journal of Psychology, 129, 5159.Google Scholar
Paulhus, D. L., Chen, D., & Trapnell, P. D. (1999). Birth order and personality within families. Psychological Science, 10, 482488.Google Scholar
Price, J. (2008). Parent–child quality time: Does birth order matter? Journal of Human Resources, 43, 240265.Google Scholar
Rodgers, J. L., Cleveland, H. H., van den Oord, E., & Rowe, D. C. (2000). Resolving the debate over birth order, family size, and intelligence. American Psychologist, 55, 599612.Google Scholar
Rohde, P. A., Atzwanger, K., Butovskaya, M., et al. (2003). Perceived parental favoritism, closeness to kin, and the rebel of the family: The effects of birth order and sex. Evolution and Human Behavior, 24, 261276.Google Scholar
Rohrer, J. M., Egloff, B., & Schmukle, S. (2015). Examining the effects of birth order on personality. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 112, 1422414229.Google Scholar
Rosenblatt, P. C., & Skoogberg, E. L. (1974). Birth order in cross-cultural perspective. Developmental Psychology, 10, 4854.Google Scholar
Ross, L., & Nisbett, R. E. (1991). The Person and the Situation: Perspectives on Social Psychology. New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Saad, G., Gill, T., & Nataraajan, R. (2005). Are laterborns more nonconforming and open to innovation than firstborns? A Darwinian perspective. Journal of Business Research, 58, 902909.Google Scholar
Salmon, C. A. (1998). The evocative nature of kin terminology in political rhetoric. Politics and the Life Sciences, 17, 5157.Google Scholar
Salmon, C. A. (1999). On the impact of sex and birth order on contact with kin. Human Nature, 10, 183197.Google Scholar
Salmon, C. A. (2003). Birth order and relationships. Human Nature, 14, 7388.Google Scholar
Salmon, C. A., & Daly, M. (1998). Birth order and familial sentiment: Middleborns are different. Evolution and Human Behavior, 19, 299312.Google Scholar
Salmon, C. A., Shackelford, T. K., & Michalski, R. L. (2012). Birth order, sex of child, and perceptions of parental favoritism. Personality and Individual Differences, 52, 357362.Google Scholar
Saroglou, V., & Fiasse, L. (2003). Birth order, personality, and religion: A study among young adults from a three-sibling family. Personality and Individual Differences, 35, 1929.Google Scholar
Schachter, F. F. (1982). Sibling deidentification and split-parent identifications: A family tetrad. In Lamb, M. E. & Sutton-Smith, B., eds., Sibling Relationships: Their Nature and Significance across the Lifespan. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum, pp. 123152.Google Scholar
Schachter, F. F., Gilutz, G., Shore, E., & Adler, M. (1978). Sibling deidentification judged by mothers: Cross-validation and developmental studies. Child Development, 49, 543546.Google Scholar
Schooler, C. (1972). Birth order effects: Not here, not now! Psychological Bulletin, 78, 161175.Google Scholar
Skinner, G. W. (1992). Seek a loyal subject in a filial son: Family roots of political orientation in Chinese society. In Institute of Modern History, Academia Sinica, ed., Family Process and Political Process in Modern Chinese History, Vol. 2. Taipei: Chiang Ching-kou Foundation for International Scholarly Exchange, pp. 943993.Google Scholar
Skinner, N. F., & Fox-Francoeur, C. A. (2010). Personality implications of adaption-innovation: V. Birth order as a determinant of cognitive style. Social Behavior and Personality, 38, 237240.Google Scholar
Suitor, J. J., & Pillemer, K. (2007). Mother’s favoritism in later life: The role of children’s birth order. Research on Aging, 29, 3255.Google Scholar
Sulloway, F. J. (1995). Birth order and evolutionary psychology. Psychological Inquiry, 6, 7580.Google Scholar
Sulloway, F. J. (1996). Born to Rebel: Birth Order, Family Dynamics, and Creative Lives. New York: Pantheon.Google Scholar
Sulloway, F. J. (2001). Birth order, sibling competition, and human behavior. In Holcomb, H. R. III, ed., Conceptual Challenges in Evolutionary Psychology: Innovative Research Strategies. Dordrecht/Boston, MA: Kluwer Academic Publishers, pp. 3983.Google Scholar
Sulloway, F. J. (2007). Birth order and intelligence. Science, 317, 17111712.Google Scholar
Sulloway, F. J. (2009). Sources of scientific innovation: A meta-analytic approach. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 4, 455459.Google Scholar
Sulloway, F. J. (2010). Why siblings are like Darwin’s Finches: Birth order, sibling competition, and adaptive divergence within the family. In Buss, D. M. & Hawley, P. H., eds., The Evolution of Personality and Individual Differences. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 86119.Google Scholar
Sulloway, F. J. (2014). Openness to scientific innovation. In Simonton, D. K., ed., The Handbook of Genius. Oxford: Wiley-Blackwell, pp. 546563.Google Scholar
Sulloway, F. J., & Zweigenhaft, R. L. (2010). Birth order and risk taking in athletics: A meta-analysis and study of major league baseball players. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 14, 402416.Google Scholar
Surbey, M. K. (1998). Parent and offspring strategies in the transition at adolescence. Human Nature, 9, 6794.Google Scholar
Sutton-Smith, B., & Rosenberg, B. G. (1970). The Sibling. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.Google Scholar
Theroux, N. L. (1993). Birth order and its relationship to academic achievement and selected personality traits. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of California, Los Angeles.Google Scholar
Trivers, R. L. (1972). Parental investment and sexual selection. In Campbell, B., ed., Sexual Selection and the Descent of Man, 1871–1971. Chicago, IL: Aldine-Atherton, pp. 136170.Google Scholar
Trivers, R. L. (1974). Parent–offspring conflict. American Zoologist, 14, 249264.Google Scholar
Turner, F. M. (1990). The Victorian crisis of faith and the faith that was lost. In Helmstadter, R. J. & Lightman, B., eds., Victorian Faith in Crisis: Essays on Continuity and Change in Nineteenth-Century Religious Belief. London: Macmillan, pp. 938.Google Scholar
Voland, E. (1988). Differential infant and child mortality in evolutionary perspective: Data from the late 17th to 19th century Ostfriesland (Germany). In Betzig, L., Borgerhoff Mulder, M., & Turke, P., eds., Human Reproduction Behaviour: A Darwinian Perspective. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, pp. 253262.Google Scholar
Voland, E. (1990). Differential reproductive success within the Krummhorn population (Germany, 18th and 19th centuries). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 26, 5472.Google Scholar
Wichman, A. L., Rodgers, J. L., & MacCallum, R. C. (2006). A multilevel approach to the relationship between birth order and intelligence. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32, 117127.Google Scholar
Zajonc, R. B. (1976). Family configuration and intelligence. Science, 192, 227236.Google Scholar
Zajonc, R. B., & Mullally, P. R. (1997). Birth order: Reconciling conflicting effects. American Psychologist, 52, 685699.Google Scholar
Zajonc, R. B., & Sulloway, F. J. (2007). The confluence model: Birth order as a within-family or between-family dynamic? Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33, 11871194.Google Scholar
Ziv, I., & Hermel, O. (2011). Birth order effects on the separation process in young adults: An evolutionary and dynamic approach. American Journal of Psychology, 124, 261273.Google Scholar
Zweigenhaft, R. L. (2002). Birth order effects and rebelliousness: Political activism and involvement with marijuana. Political Psychology, 23, 219233.Google Scholar
Zweigenhaft, R. L., & Von Ammon, J. (2000). Birth order and civil disobedience: A test of Sulloway’s “born to rebel” hypothesis. Journal of Social Psychology, 140, 624627.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×