Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-t7fkt Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-26T22:41:30.479Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

20 - The Adaptive Problem of Exploiting Resources

Human Foraging Behavior in Patchy Environments

from Part V - Evolution and Cognition

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 March 2020

Lance Workman
Affiliation:
University of South Wales
Will Reader
Affiliation:
Sheffield Hallam University
Jerome H. Barkow
Affiliation:
Dalhousie University, Nova Scotia
Get access

Summary

Over evolutionary time, humans have had to solve problems regarding many important foraging activities, such as deciding where to find crucial resources, when to move on to more resource-rich locations with higher intake rates, and how well past foraging success might predict the future likelihood of return. This chapter will argue that these reoccurring foraging behaviors of our ancestral past left an eminent footprint in our evolved cognitive system – and specifically in our information processing mechanisms that deal with risk and uncertainty. This chapter on evolution, cognition, and decision-making will review empirical work from animal behavior, biological anthropology, and evolutionary psychology to show that our mind possesses various cognitive foraging adaptations that coevolved with the statistical regularities of natural resource environments.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2020

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Alphen, J. J. M., Bernstein, C., & Driessen, G. (2003). Information acquisition and time allocation in insect parasitoids. Trends in Ecology and Evolution, 18, 8187.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition. Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Association Publishing.Google Scholar
Barrett, H. C. (2018). The varieties of foraging experience. Evolutionary Behavioral Sciences, 12, 135138.Google Scholar
Bell, W. J. (1991). Searching Behaviour: The Behavioural Ecology of Finding Resources. New York: Chapman & Hall.Google Scholar
Binford, L. (1990). Mobility, housing, and environment: A comparative study. Journal of Anthropological Research, 46, 119152.Google Scholar
Blanchard, T. C., Wilke, A., & Hayden, B. Y. (2014). Hot hand bias in rhesus monkeys. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Learning and Cognition, 40, 280286.Google Scholar
Castel, A. D., Drolet Rossi, A., & McGillivray, S. (2012). Beliefs about the “hot hand” in basketball across adult life span. Psychology and Aging, 27, 601605.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Charnov, E. L. (1976). Optimal foraging: The marginal value theorem. Theoretical Population Biology, 9, 129136.Google Scholar
Croson, R., & Sundali, J. (2005). The gambler’s fallacy and the hot hand: Empirical data from Casinos. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 30, 195209.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Czienskowski, U. (2005a). The Fishing Task Experiment [computer software]. Berlin: Max Planck Institute for Human Development.Google Scholar
Czienskowski, U. (2005b). The Word Puzzle Experiment [computer software]. Berlin: Max Planck Institute for Human Development.Google Scholar
Falk, R. (1975). Perception of randomness. Unpublished doctoral dissertation (in Hebrew, with English abstract), Hebrew University of Jerusalem.Google Scholar
Falk, R., & Konold, C. (1997). Making sense of randomness: Implicit encoding as a basis for judgment. Psychological Review, 104, 301318.Google Scholar
Fawcett, T. W., Fallenstein, B., Higginson, A. D., et al. (2014). The evolution of decision rules in complex environments. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18, 153161.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gilovich, T., Vallone, R., & Tversky, A. (1985). The hot hand in basketball: On the misperception of random sequences. Cognitive psychology, 17, 295314.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haselton, M. G., Bryant, G. A., Wilke, A., et al. (2009). Adaptive rationality: An evolutionary perspective on cognitive bias. Social Cognition, 27, 733763.Google Scholar
Hill, K., & Hurtado, A. M. (1996). Ache Life History: The Ecology and Demography of a Foraging People. New York: Aldine de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Hills, T. T., Todd, P. M., & Jones, M. N. (2015). Foraging in semantic fields: How we search through memory. Topics in Cognitive Science, 7, 513534.Google Scholar
Hodgins, D. C., Stea, J. N., & Grant, J. E. (2011). Gambling disorders. Lancet, 378, 18741884.Google Scholar
Hutchinson, J. M. C., & Gigerenzer, G. (2005). Simple heuristics and rules of thumb: Where psychologists and behavioral biologists might meet. Behavioural Processes, 69, 97124.Google Scholar
Hutchinson, J. M. C., Wilke, A., & Todd, P. M. (2008). Patch leaving in humans: Can a generalist adapt its rules to dispersal of items across patches? Animal Behaviour, 75, 13311349.Google Scholar
Iwasa, Y., Higashi, M., & Yamamura, N. (1981). Prey distribution as a factor determining the choice of optimal foraging strategy. American Naturalist, 117, 710723.Google Scholar
Kelly, R. L. (1995). The Foraging Spectrum. Washington, DC: Smithsonian Institution Press.Google Scholar
Krause, J., & Ruxton, G. D. (2002). Living in Groups: Oxford Series in Ecology and Evolution. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Krebs, J. R., Ryan, J. C., & Charnov, E. L. (1974). Hunting by expectation or optimal foraging? A study of patch use by chickadees. Animal Behavior, 22, 953964.Google Scholar
Legge, E. L. G., Spetch, M. L., Cenker, A., et al. (2012). Not all locations are created equal: Exploring how adults hide and search for objects. PLOS ONE, 7, e36993.Google Scholar
Livoreil, B., & Giraldeau, L.-A. (1997). Patch departure decisions by spice finches foraging singly or in groups. Animal Behavior, 54, 967977.Google Scholar
Mata, R., Wilke, A., & Czienskowski, U. (2009). Cognitive aging and adaptive foraging behavior. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 64, 474481.Google Scholar
Mata, R., Wilke, A., & Czienskowski, U. (2013). Foraging across the life span: Is there a reduction in exploration with aging? Frontiers in Neuroscience, 7, 53.Google Scholar
McNamara, J. M. (1982). Optimal patch use in a stochastic environment. Theoretical Population Biology, 21, 269288.Google Scholar
McNamara, J. M., & Houston, A. I. (1985). Optimal foraging and learning. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 117, 231249.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nickerson, R. S. (2002). The production and perception of randomness. Psychological Review, 109, 330357.Google Scholar
Nowak, M. A., & Sigmund, K. (1993). A strategy of win-stay, lose-shift that outperforms tit-for-tat in the Prisoner’s Dilemma game. Nature, 364, 5658.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oskarsson, A. T., van Boven, L., McClelland, G. H., & Hastie, R. (2009). What’s next? Judging sequences of binary events? Psychological Bulletin, 135, 262285.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pirolli, P. (2007). Information Foraging Theory: Adaptive Interaction with Information. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Raab, M., Gula, B., & Gigerenzer, G. (2012). The hot hand exists in volleyball and is used for allocation decisions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 18, 8194.Google ScholarPubMed
Reifman, A. (2011). Hot Hand: The Statistics behind Sports’ Greatest Streaks. Dulles, VA: Potomac Books.Google Scholar
Scheibehenne, B., Wilke, A., & Todd, P. M. (2011). Expectations of clumpy resources influence predictions of sequential events. Evolution and Human Behavior, 32, 326333.Google Scholar
Shaffer, H. J., Hall, M. N., & Bilt, J. V. (1997). Estimating the Prevalence of Disordered Gambling Behavior in the United States and Canada: A Meta-analysis. Boston, MA: Harvard Medical School.Google Scholar
Talbot, K. J., Legge, E. L. G., Bulkito, V., & Spetch, M. L. (2009). Hiding and searching strategies of adult humans in a virtual and a real-space room. Learning and Motivation, 40, 221233.Google Scholar
Taylor, L. R. (1961). Aggregation, variance and the mean. Nature, 189, 732735.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Taylor, L. R., Woiwod, I. P., & Perry, J. N. (1978). The density-dependence of spatial behaviour and the rarity of randomness. Journal of Animal Ecology, 47, 383406.Google Scholar
Todd, P. M., & Gigerenzer, G. (2012). What is ecological rationality? In Todd, P. M., Gigerenzer, G., & The ABC Research Group, eds., Ecological Rationality: Intelligence in the World. New York: Oxford University Press, pp. 332.Google Scholar
Todd, P. M., Hills, T. T., & Robbins, T. W., eds. (2012). Cognitive Search: Evolution, Algorithms, and the Brain (Strüngmann Forum Reports, Vol. 9). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Tooby, J., & DeVore, I. (1987). The reconstruction of hominid behavioral evolution through strategic modeling. In Kinsey, W. G., ed., The Evolution of Primate Behavior: Primate Models. New York: State University of New York Press, pp. 183237.Google Scholar
Waage, J. K. (1979). Foraging for patchily-distributed hosts by parasitoid, Nemeritis canescens. Journal of Animal Ecology, 48, 353371.Google Scholar
Wajnberg, E., Fauvegue, X., & Pons, O. (2000). Patch leaving decision rules and the marginal value theorem: An experimental analysis and a simulation model. Behavioral Ecology, 11, 577586.Google Scholar
Wajnberg, E., Gonsard, P.-A., Tabone, E., et al. (2003). A comparative analysis of patch-leaving decision rules in a parasitoid family. Journal of Animal Ecology, 72, 618626.Google Scholar
Wilke, A. (2006). Evolved responses to an uncertain world. Unpublished PhD thesis, Free University of Berlin.Google Scholar
Wilke, A., & Barrett, H. C. (2009). The hot hand phenomenon as a cognitive adaptation to clumped resources. Evolution and Human Behavior, 30, 161169.Google Scholar
Wilke, A., & Mata, R. (2012). Cognitive bias. In Ramachandran, V. S., ed., Encyclopedia of Human Behavior, 2nd ed. Maryland Heights, MO: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Wilke, A., & Todd, P. M. (2010). Past and present environments: The evolution of decision making. Psicothema, 22, 48.Google Scholar
Wilke, A., Hutchinson, J. M. C., Todd, P. M., & Czienskowski, U. (2009). Fishing for the right words: Decision rules for human foraging behavior in internal search tasks. Cognitive Science, 33, 497529.Google Scholar
Wilke, A., Scheibehenne, B., Gaissmaier, W., McCanney, P., & Barrett, H. C. (2014). Illusionary pattern detection in habitual gamblers. Evolution and Human Behavior, 35, 291297.Google Scholar
Wilke, A., Minich, S., Panis, M., et al. (2015). A game of hide and seek: Expectations of clumpy resources influence hiding and searching patterns. PLoS ONE, 10, e0130976.Google Scholar
Wilke, A., Lydick, J., Bedell, V., et al. (2018). Spatial dependency in local resource distributions. Evolutionary Behavioral Sciences, 12, 163172.Google Scholar
Winterhalder, B., & Smith, E. A. (1981). Hunter–Gatherer Foraging Strategies: Ethnographic and Archaeological Analyses. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
World Health Organization (1992). ICD-10 Classifications of Mental and Behavioural Disorder: Clinical Descriptions and Diagnostic Guidelines. Geneva: WHO Press.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×