Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-l7hp2 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-25T19:09:00.813Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

18 - Affective Factors in Dark Creativity

from Part IV - Emotions and Creative Products

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  16 February 2023

Zorana Ivcevic
Affiliation:
Yale University, Connecticut
Jessica D. Hoffmann
Affiliation:
Yale University, Connecticut
James C. Kaufman
Affiliation:
University of Connecticut
Get access

Summary

Although recent research has begun examining the construct more widely, there exist several gaps in scholarship. One such area is the influence, association, and role of affective factors in determining whether and to what extent an entity engages in dark creativity. After introducing concepts like negative and malevolent creativity, this chapter reviews the existing literature on the link between affective factors and creativity. Thereafter, the features of dark personality traits, like psychopathy, are examined with reference to affective considerations such as low empathy in producing original harm. Owing to the relationship between dark creativity and moral concerns, we also examine how moral emotions like guilt and shame (or lack thereof) may contribute to an understanding of such creativity. The chapter concludes with suggestions for future research and avenues for interdisciplinary studies.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2023

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Abbink, K., & Sadrieh, A. (2009). The pleasure of being nasty. Economics Letters, 105(3), 306308. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.econlet.2009.08.024CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baas, M., De Dreu, C. K. W., & Nijstad, B. A. (2008). A meta-analysis of 25 years of mood-creativity research: Hedonic tone, activation, or regulatory focus? Psychological Bulletin, 134(6), 779806. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0012815Google Scholar
Baas, M., De Dreu, C. K. W., & Nijstad, B. A. (2011). Creative production by angry people peaks early on, decreases over time, and is relatively unstructured. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 47(6), 11071115. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2011.05.009Google Scholar
Baas, M., Roskes, M., Koch, S., Cheng, Y., & De Dreu, C. K. W. (2019). Why social threat motivates malevolent creativity. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 45(11), 15901602. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167219838551CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barsade, S. G., & Gibson, D. E. (2007). Why does affect matter in organizations? Academy of Management Perspectives, 21(1), 3659. https://doi.org/10.5465/AMP.2007.24286163Google Scholar
Baumeister, R. F., Stillwell, A. M., & Heatherton, T. F. (1994). Guilt: An interpersonal approach. Psychological Bulletin, 115(2), 243267. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.115.2.243Google Scholar
Bonetto, E., & Arciszewski, T. (2021). The creativity of conspiracy theories. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 54(4), 916924. jocb.497. https://doi.org/10.1002/jocb.497Google Scholar
Buddi, M. (2021, July 9). Hyderabad: Couple arrested for stealing jewlery from Covid patients at hospital. The Times of India.Google Scholar
Buncombe, A. (2011, October 23). MF Husain: Indian artist who spent his last five years in self-imposed exile after death threats from Hindu nationalists. Independent.Google Scholar
Cheng, R., LU, K., & HAO, N. (2021). The effect of anger on malevolent creativity and strategies for its emotion regulation. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 53(8), 847. https://doi.org/10.3724/sp.j.1041.2021.00847CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, K., & James, K. (1999). Justice and positive and negative creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 12(4), 311320. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326934crj1204_9CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cohen, T. R. (2010). Moral emotions and unethical bargaining: The differential effects of empathy and perspective taking in deterring deceitful negotiation. Journal of Business Ethics, 94(4), 569579. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10551-009-0338-zGoogle Scholar
Cohen, T. R., Panter, A. T., & Turan, N. (2012). Guilt proneness and moral character. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21(5), 355359. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721412454874Google Scholar
Cooper, J. M., & Silvia, P. J. (2009). Opposing art: Rejection as an action tendency of hostile aesthetic emotions. Empirical Studies of the Arts, 27(1), 109126. https://doi.org/10.2190/em.27.1.fCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cropley, D. H. (2010). Summary – The dark side of creativity: A differentiated model. In Cropley, D. H., Cropley, A. J., Kaufman, J. C., & Runco, M. A. (Eds.), The Dark Side of Creativity (pp. 360373). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Cropley, D. H., & Cropley, A. J. (2013). Creativity and Crime: A Psychological Analysis. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Cropley, D. H., Kaufman, J. C., & Cropley, A. J. (2008). Malevolent creativity: A functional model of creativity in terrorism and crime. Creativity Research Journal, 20(2), 105115. https://doi.org/10.1080/10400410802059424Google Scholar
Cuff, B. M. P., Brown, S. J., Taylor, L., & Howat, D. J. (2016). Empathy: A review of the concept. Emotion Review, 8(2), 144153. https://doi.org/10.1177/1754073914558466Google Scholar
Davis, M. A. (2009). Understanding the relationship between mood and creativity: A meta-analysis. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 108(1), 2538. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.obhdp.2008.04.001Google Scholar
De Dreu, C. K. W., Baas, M., & Nijstad, B. A. (2008). Hedonic tone and activation level in the mood-creativity link: Toward a dual pathway to creativity model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94(5), 739756. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.94.5.739Google Scholar
De Lara, P. Z. M. (2007). Relationship between organizational justice and cyberloafing in the workplace: Has “anomia” a say in the matter? Cyberpsychology and Behavior, 10(3), 464470. https://doi.org/10.1089/cpb.2006.9931Google Scholar
Decety, J., & Cowell, J. M. (2014). Friends or foes: Is empathy necessary for moral behavior? Perspectives on Psychological Science, 9(5), 525537. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691614545130Google Scholar
Du, Y., Yang, Y., Wang, X., et al. (2021). A positive role of negative mood on creativity: The opportunity in the crisis of the COVID-19 epidemic. Frontiers in Psychology, 11(January), 114. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.600837Google Scholar
Erzi, S. (2020). Dark Triad and schadenfreude: Mediating role of moral disengagement and relational aggression. Personality and Individual Differences, 157(January), 109827. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2020.109827Google Scholar
Fang, X., Chen, L., Wang, J., Zhang, Q., & Mo, L. (2020). Do all types of compassion increase prosocial lying? Psychology Research and Behavior Management, 13, 437451. https://doi.org/10.2147/PRBM.S238246Google Scholar
Forgeard, M. J. C., & Mecklenburg, A. C. (2013). The two dimensions of motivation and a reciprocal model of the creative process. Review of General Psychology, 17(3), 255266. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0032104CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Forsyth, L., Anglim, J., March, E., & Bilobrk, B. (2021). Dark Tetrad personality traits and the propensity to lie across multiple contexts. Personality and Individual Differences, 177(110792). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2021.11079Google Scholar
Fouché, P., du Plessis, R., & van Niekerk, R. (2017). Levinsonian seasons in the life of Steve Jobs: A psychobiographical case study. Indo-Pacific Journal of Phenomenology, 17(1), 118. https://doi.org/10.1080/20797222.2017.1331970CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fredrickson, B. L. (2001). The role of positive emotions in positive psychology: The broaden-and-build theory of positive emotions. American Psychologist, 56(3), 218226. https://doi.org/10.1037//0003-066x.56.3.218Google Scholar
Friedman, R. S., Förster, J., & Denzler, M. (2007). Interactive effects of mood and task framing on creative generation. Creativity Research Journal, 19(2–3), 141162. https://doi.org/10.1080/10400410701397206Google Scholar
Furnham, A., Richards, S. C., & Paulhus, D. L. (2013). The Dark Triad of personality: A 10 year review. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 7(3), 199216. https://doi.org/10.1111/spc3.12018CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gaspar, J. P., & Schweitzer, M. E. (2013). The emotion deception model: A review of deception in negotiation and the role of emotion in deception. Negotiation and Conflict Management Research, 6(3), 160179. https://doi.org/10.1111/ncmr.12010CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gino, F., & Shea, C. (2012). Deception in negotiations: The role of emotions. In Bolton, G. E. & Croson, R. T. A. (Eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Economic Conflict Resolution (pp. 4760). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Gino, F., & Wiltermuth, S. S. (2014). Evil genius? How dishonesty can lead to greater creativity. Psychological Science, 25(4), 973981. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797614520714Google Scholar
Göritz, A. S., & Moser, K. (2006). Web-based mood induction. Cognition and Emotion, 20(6), 887896. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930500405386Google Scholar
Graham, J., Haidt, J., Koleva, S., et al. (2013). Moral foundations theory: The pragmatic validity of moral pluralism. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 47, 55130. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-407236-7.00002-4CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gutworth, M. B., Cushenbery, L., & Hunter, S. T. (2018). Creativity for deliberate harm: Malevolent creativity and social information processing theory. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 52(4), 305322. https://doi.org/10.1002/jocb.155Google Scholar
Haidt, J. (2003). The moral emotions. In Davidson, R. J., Scherer, K. R., & Goldsmith, H. H. (Eds.), Handbook of Affective Sciences (pp. 852870). Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/mnras/stx1358Google Scholar
Haidt, J., Koller, S. H., & Dias, M. G. (1993). Affect, culture, and morality, or is it wrong to eat your dog? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65(4), 613628. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.65.4.613Google Scholar
Harris, D. J., & Reiter-Palmon, R. (2015). Fast and furious: The influence of implicit aggression, premeditation, and provoking situations on malevolent creativity. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 9(1), 5464. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0038499Google Scholar
Harris, D. J., Reiter-Palmon, R., & Kaufman, J. C. (2013). The effect of emotional intelligence and task type on malevolent creativity. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 7(3), 237244. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0032139Google Scholar
Heym, N., Firth, J., Kibowski, F., et al. (2019). Empathy at the heart of darkness: Empathy deficits that bind the dark triad and those that mediate indirect relational aggression. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 10, 110. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00095Google Scholar
Hui, P. P., Chiu, W. C. K., Pang, E., Coombes, J., & Tse, D. Y. P. (2021). Seeing through and breaking through: The role of perspective taking in the relationship between creativity and moral reasoning. Journal of Business Ethics, 0123456789. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10551-021-04876-3Google Scholar
Ivcevic, Z., Brackett, M. A., & Mayer, J. D. (2007). Emotional intelligence and emotional creativity. Journal of Personality, 75(2), 199236. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.2007.00437.xGoogle Scholar
Ivcevic, Z., & Hoffmann, J. (2019). Emotions and creativity: From process to person and product. In Kaufman, J. C. & Sternberg, R. J. (Eds.), The Cambridge Handbook of Creativity (pp. 273295). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
James, K., Clark, K., & Cropanzano, R. (1999). Positive and negative creativity in groups, institutions, and organizations: A model and theoretical extension. Creativity Research Journal, 12(3), 211226. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326934crj1203Google Scholar
James, K., & Taylor, A. (2010). Positive creativity and negative creativity (and unintended consequences). In Cropley, D. H., Cropley, A. J., Kaufman, J. C., & Runco, M. A. (Eds.), The Dark Side of Creativity (pp. 3356). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Jonason, P. K., & Krause, L. (2013). The emotional deficits associated with the Dark Triad traits: Cognitive empathy, affective empathy, and alexithymia. Personality and Individual Differences, 55(5), 532537. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2013.04.027Google Scholar
Kapoor, H. (2015). The creative side of the Dark Triad. Creativity Research Journal, 27(1), 5867. https://doi.org/10.1080/10400419.2014.961775Google Scholar
Kapoor, H., & Karandikar, S. (2019). Darkness all around: Humor, personality, and creativity. In-Mind Magazine. www.in-mind.org/blog/post/darkness-all-around-humor-personality-and-creativityGoogle Scholar
Kapoor, H., & Kaufman, J. C. (2020). Meaning-making through creativity during COVID-19. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, 595990. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.595990Google Scholar
Kapoor, H., & Kaufman, J. C. (2021). Unbound: The relationship between creativity, moral foundations, and dark personality. The Journal of Creative Behavior. https://doi.org/10.1002/jocb.523Google Scholar
Kapoor, H., & Khan, A. (2017). Deceptively yours: Valence-based creativity and deception. Thinking Skills and Creativity, 23, 199206. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tsc.2016.12.006Google Scholar
Kumar, R. (1997). The role of affect in negotiations. The Journal of Applied Behavioral Science, 33(1), 84100. https://doi.org/10.1177/0021886397331007Google Scholar
Leith, K. P., & Baumeister, R. F. (1998). Empathy, shame, guilt, and narratives of interpersonal conflicts: Guilt-prone people are better at perspective taking. Journal of Personality, 66(1), 137. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-6494.00001Google Scholar
Lupoli, M. J., Jampol, L., & Oveis, C. (2017). Lying because we care: Compassion increases Prosocial lying. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 146(7), 10261042. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000315Google Scholar
Mai, K. M., Ellis, A. P. J., & Welsh, D. T. (2015). The gray side of creativity: Exploring the role of activation in the link between creative personality and unethical behavior. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 60, 7685. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2015.05.004CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marcusson-Clavertz, D., Kjell, O. N. E., Persson, S. D., & Cardeña, E. (2019). Online validation of combined mood induction procedures. PLoS ONE, 14(6), 116. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0217848Google Scholar
Mayer, J. D., Caruso, D. R., & Salovey, P. (1999). Emotional intelligence meets traditional standards for an intelligence. Intelligence, 27(4), 267298. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0160-2896(99)00016-1Google Scholar
McCornack, S. A. (1992). Information manipulation theory. Communication Monographs, 59(1), 116. https://doi.org/10.1080/03637759209376245Google Scholar
Menninghaus, W., Wagner, V., Wassiliwizky, E., et al. (2019). What are aesthetic emotions? Psychological Review, 126(2), 171195. https://doi.org/10.1037/rev0000135CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miao, C., Humphrey, R. H., Qian, S., & Pollack, J. M. (2019). The relationship between emotional intelligence and the dark triad personality traits: A meta-analytic review. Journal of Research in Personality, 78, 189197. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2018.12.004Google Scholar
Nagler, U. K. J., Reiter, K. J., Furtner, M. R., & Rauthmann, J. F. (2014). Is there a “dark intelligence”? Emotional intelligence is used by dark personalities to emotionally manipulate others. Personality and Individual Differences, 65, 4752. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2014.01.025CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Olekalns, M., & Smith, P. L. (2009). Mutually dependent: Power, trust, affect and the use of deception in negotiation. Journal of Business Ethics, 85(3), 347365. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10551-008-9774-4Google Scholar
Pajevic, M., Vukosavljevic-Gvozden, T., Stevanovic, N., & Neumann, C. S. (2018). The relationship between the Dark Tetrad and a two-dimensional view of empathy. Personality and Individual Differences, 123, 125130. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2017.11.009Google Scholar
Paulhus, D. L. (2014). Toward a taxonomy of dark personalities. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 23(6), 421426. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721414547737Google Scholar
Perchtold-Stefan, C. M., Fink, A., Rominger, C., & Papousek, I. (2020a). Creative, antagonistic, and angry? Exploring the roots of malevolent creativity with a real-world idea generation task. Journal of Creative Behavior. https://doi.org/10.1002/jocb.484Google Scholar
Perchtold-Stefan, C. M., Fink, A., Rominger, C., & Papousek, I. (2020b). Motivational factors in the typical display of humor and creative potential: The case of malevolent creativity. Frontiers in Psychology, 11. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01213Google Scholar
Perchtold-Stefan, C. M., Fink, A., Rominger, C., & Papousek, I. (2021). Failure to reappraise: Malevolent creativity is linked to revenge ideation and impaired reappraisal inventiveness in the face of stressful, anger-eliciting events. Anxiety, Stress, & Coping, 34(4), 437439. https://doi.org/10.1080/10615806.2021.1918682Google Scholar
Plucker, J. A., Beghetto, R. A., & Dow, G. T. (2004). Why isn’t creativity more important to educational psychologists? Potentials, pitfalls, and future directions in creativity research. Educational Psychologist, 39(2), 8396. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326985ep3902_1Google Scholar
Puthillam, A., Karandikar, S., & Kapoor, H. (2019). I see how you feel: How the Dark Triad recognizes emotions. Current Psychology, 1–8. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-019-00359-xGoogle Scholar
Reisenzein, R. (1994). Pleasure-arousal theory and the intensity of emotions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67(3), 525539. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.67.3.525Google Scholar
Rhodes, M. (1961). An analysis of creativity. The Phi Delta Kappan, 42(7), 305310. www.jstor.org/stable/20342603Google Scholar
Schindler, I., Hosoya, G., Menninghaus, W., et al. (2017). Measuring aesthetic emotions: A review of the literature and a new assessment tool. PLoS ONE, 12(6), e0178899. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0178899CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scrima, F., Miceli, S., Caci, B., & Cardaci, M. (2022). The relationship between fear of COVID-19 and intention to get vaccinated. The serial mediation roles of existential anxiety and conspiracy beliefs. Personality and Individual Differences, 184(January), 111188. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2021.111188Google Scholar
Shen, W., Yuan, Y., Yi, B., Liu, C., & Zhan, H. (2019). A theoretical and critical examination on the relationship between creativity and morality. Current Psychology, 38, 469485. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-017-9613-9Google Scholar
Silvia, P. J. (2009). Looking past pleasure: Anger, confusion, disgust, pride, surprise, and other unusual aesthetic emotions. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 3(1), 4851. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0014632Google Scholar
Silvia, P. J., & Brown, E. M. (2007). Anger, disgust, and the negative aesthetic emotions: Expanding an appraisal model of aesthetic experience. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 1(2), 100106. https://doi.org/10.1037/1931-3896.1.2.100Google Scholar
Storme, M., Celik, P., & Myszkowski, N. (2021). Creativity and unethicality: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 15(4), 664672. https://doi.org/10.1037/aca0000332Google Scholar
Tangney, J. P., Stuewig, J., & Mashek, D. J. (2007). Moral emotions and moral behavior. Annual Review of Psychology, 58(1), 345372. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.56.091103.070145Google Scholar
Times News Network. (2021, July 10). Thief pretends to puke on BMTC bus passenger, aides steal Rs 50k. The Times of India.Google Scholar
Van Kleef, G. A., Anastasopoulou, C., & Nijstad, B. A. (2010). Can expressions of anger enhance creativity? A test of the emotions as social information (EASI) model. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 46(6), 10421048. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2010.05.015Google Scholar
Wagemans, F. M. A., Brandt, M. J., & Zeelenberg, M. (2018). Disgust sensitivity is primarily associated with purity-based moral judgments. Emotion, 18(2), 277289. https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000359Google Scholar
Wai, M., & Tiliopoulos, N. (2012). The affective and cognitive empathic nature of the dark triad of personality. Personality and Individual Differences, 52(7), 794799. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2012.01.008Google Scholar
Walczyk, J. J., Runco, M. A., Tripp, S. M., & Smith, C. E. (2008). The creativity of lying: Divergent thinking and ideational correlates of the resolution of social dilemmas. Creativity Research Journal, 20(3), 328342. https://doi.org/10.1080/10400410802355152Google Scholar
Walker, L. J. (1980). Cognitive and perspective-taking prerequisites for moral development. Child Development, 51(1), 131139.Google Scholar
Wang, B., & Wei, P. (2021). Moral emotion: A new perspective on the relationship between morality and creativity. Advances in Psychological Science, 29(2), 268275.Google Scholar
Westermann, R., Spies, K., Stahl, G., & Hesse, F. W. (1996). Relative effectiveness and validity of mood induction procedures: A meta-analysis. European Journal of Social Psychology, 26, 557580.Google Scholar
Wu, W., Su, Y., Huang, X., Liu, W., & Jiang, X. (2020). The dark triad, moral disengagement, and social entrepreneurial intention: Moderating roles of empathic concern and perspective taking. Frontiers in Psychology, 11. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01520Google Scholar
Xu, L., Chen, G., & Li, B. (2020). Sadness empathy facilitates prosocial lying. Social Behavior and Personality, 47(9), 111. https://doi.org/10.2224/SBP.8371Google Scholar
Yang, J.-S., & Hung, H. V. (2015). Emotions as constraining and facilitating factors for creativity: Companionate love and anger. Creativity and Innovation Management, 24(2), 217230. https://doi.org/10.1111/caim.12089Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×