Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-cphqk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-01-31T08:05:13.005Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

4 - Risk-Taking, Risk Perception, and Risk Compensation in Times of COVID-19

from Part I - Evidence from Experiments and Behavioural Insights

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  aN Invalid Date NaN

Joan Costa-Font
Affiliation:
London School of Economics and Political Science
Matteo M. Galizzi
Affiliation:
London School of Economics and Political Science
Get access

Summary

Two years after the initial outbreak in 2020, SARS-CoV-2 continues to have a disruptive impact on day-to-day life for billions of people around the world. Despite high vaccination rates, high-income countries report record rates of infection with the Omicron variant in spring 2022. Adapting to the pandemic has led to significant behaviour change, such as increased working from home, social distancing, and mask-wearing. With this, perceptions of everyday situations (e.g., taking public transport or grocery shopping) have become risky in COVID-19 times. As risk tolerance is a key component to decision-making, changes in perceived risk may alter decision-making in the (post) COVID-19 world. In this chapter we summarise findings on risk-taking in times of COVID-19, with an aim to offer insights for policy purposes in this pandemic and for future pandemic preparedness. In particular, we summarise (i) the impact of COVID-19 on individual risk tolerance; (ii) the heterogeneity of risk tolerance during times of COVID-19; (iii) their relative effects on behaviour; and (iv) any evidence for risk compensation in the context of COVID-19.

Type
Chapter
Information
Behavioural Economics and Policy for Pandemics
Insights from Responses to COVID-19
, pp. 57 - 76
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2024

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Aksoy, B., Chadd, I., Osun, E. B., & Ozbay, E. Y. (2021). Behavioral changes of Mturkers during the COVID-19 pandemic. Social Science Research Network. https://ssrn.com/abstract=3920502 or http://dx.doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.3920502CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Allmand, G. (2020). Comparison of furloughed jobs data: May to July 2020. Comparison of furloughed jobs data – Office for National Statistics. www.ons.gov.uk/businessindustryandtrade/business/businessservices/articles/comparisonoffurloughedjobsdata/maytojuly2020Google Scholar
Alsharawy, A., Spoon, R., Smith, A., & Ball, S. B. (2021). Gender differences in fear and risk perception during the COVID-19 pandemic. Social Science Research Network. https://ssrn.com/abstract=3817792 or http://dx.doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.3817792CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Angrisani, M., Cipriani, M., Guarino, A., Kendall, R., & Ortiz de Zarate, J. (2020). Risk preferences at the time of COVID-19: An experiment with professional traders and students. Social Science Research Network. https://doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.3609586CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Aranguren, M. (2021). Face mask use conditionally decreases compliance with physical distancing rules against COVID-19: Gender differences in risk compensation pattern. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 56(4), 332–346. https://doi.org/10.1093/abm/kaab072Google Scholar
Bchir, M. A., & Willinger, M. (2013). Does the exposure to natural hazards affect risk and time preferences? Some insights from a field experiment in Perú. Economic Affairs. https://doi.org/10.1111/ecaf.12027Google Scholar
Binswanger, H. P. (1980). Attitudes toward risk: Experimental measurement in rural India. American Journal of Agricultural Economics. https://doi.org/10.2307/1240194CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blais, A.-R., & Weber, E. U. (2006). A Domain-Specific Risk-Taking (DOSPERT) scale for adult populations. Judgment and Decision Making, 1(1), 3347. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1930297500000334CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blanken, T. F., Tanis, C. C., Nauta, F. H., Dablander, F., Zijlstra, B. J. H., Bouten, et al. (2021). Promoting physical distancing during COVID-19: A systematic approach to compare behavioral interventions. Scientific Reports, 11(1), 19463. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-021-98964-zCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brewer, N. T., Cuite, C. L., Herrington, J. E., & Weinstein, N. D. (2007). Risk compensation and vaccination: Can getting vaccinated cause people to engage in risky behaviors? Annals of Behavioral Medicine. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02879925CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bu, D., Hanspal, T., Liao, Y., & Liu, Y. (2020). Risk taking, preferences, and beliefs: evidence from Wuhan (December 2020). SAFE Working Paper No. 301. Social Science Research Network. https://ssrn.com/abstract=3559870 or http://dx.doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.3559870CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Byrnes, J. P., Miller, D. C., & Schafer, W. D. (1999). Gender differences in risk taking: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.125.3.367CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cameron, L., & Shah, M. (2015). Risk-taking behavior in the wake of natural disasters. Journal of Human Resources. https://doi.org/10.3368/jhr.50.2.484CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cartaud, A., Quesque, F., & Coello, Y. (2020). Wearing a face mask against COVID-19 results in a reduction of social distancing. PloS One, 15(12), e0243023.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cassar, A., Healy, A., & von Kessler, C. (2017). Trust, risk, and time preferences after a natural disaster: Experimental evidence from Thailand. World Development. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.worlddev.2016.12.042CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chen, L. H., Baker, S. P., Braver, E. R., & Li, G. (2000). Carrying passengers as a risk factor for crashes fatal to 16- and 17-year-old drivers. JAMA, 283(12). https://doi.org/10.1001/jama.283.12.1578CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chuang, Y., & Liu, J. C. (2020). Who wears a mask? Gender differences in risk behaviors in the COVID-19 early days in Taiwan. Economics Bulletin, 40, 26192627.Google Scholar
Crosetto, P., & Filippin, A. (2013). The ‘bomb’ risk elicitation task. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11166-013-9170-zCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Croson, R., & Gneezy, U. (2009). Gender differences in preferences. Journal of Economic Literature. https://doi.org/10.1257/jel.47.2.448CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dohmen, T., Falk, A., Huffman, D., Sunde, U., Schupp, J., & Wagner, G. G. (2005). Individual risk attitudes: New evidence from a large, representative, experimentally-validated survey. Discussion Papers No. 511, DIW Berlin.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Drichoutis, A. C., & Nayga, R. M. (2022). On the stability of risk and time preferences amid the COVID-19 pandemic. Experimental Economics, 25, 759794. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10683-021-09727-6CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Drichoutis, A., & Vassilopoulos, A. (2016). Intertemporal stability of survey-based measures of risk and time preferences over a three-year course. Working Paper No. 2016-3. Agricultural University of Athens, Department of Agricultural Economics. https://EconPapers.repec.org/RePEc:aua:wpaper:2016-3Google Scholar
Eckel, C. C., El-Gamal, M. A., & Wilson, R. K. (2009). Risk loving after the storm: A Bayesian-Network study of Hurricane Katrina evacuees. Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jebo.2007.08.012CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eckel, C. C., & Grossman, P. J. (2002). Sex differences and statistical stereotyping in attitudes toward financial risk. Evolution and Human Behavior. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1090-5138(02)00097-1CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eger, L., Komárková, L., Egerová, D., & Mičík, M. (2021). The effect of COVID-19 on consumer shopping behaviour: Generational cohort perspective. Journal of Retailing and Consumer Services, 61. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jretconser.2021.102542CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellis, D. A., Wiseman, R., & Jenkins, R. (2015). Mental representations of weekdays. PloS ONE, 10(8). https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0134555CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fagereng, A., Guiso, L., & Pistaferri, L. (2016). Back to background risk? Discussion Papers no. 834. www.ssb.no/en/forskning/discussion-papers/back-to-background-riskGoogle Scholar
Falk, A., Becker, A., Dohmen, T., Enke, B., Huffman, D., & Sunde, U. (2018). Global evidence on economic preferences. Quarterly Journal of Economics. https://doi.org/10.1093/qje/qjy013CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fan, Y., Orhun, A. Y., & Turjeman, D. (2020). Heterogeneous actions, beliefs, constraints and risk tolerance during the COVID-19 pandemic. Working Paper 27211, NBER Working Paper Series. https://doi.org/10.3386/w27211CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fedrigo, V., Galizzi, M. M., Guenther, B., Jenkins, R., & Sanders, J. (2023). Weakened weekdays: Lockdown disrupts the weekly cycle of risk tolerance. Scientific Reports, 13, 21147.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ferrín, M. (2022). Reassessing gender differences in COVID-19 risk perception and behavior. Social Science Quarterly. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1111/ssqu.13116CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gneezy, U., & Potters, J. (1997). An experiment on risk taking and evaluation periods. The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 112(2), 631645. www.jstor.org/stable/2951248CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grable, J., & Lytton, R. H. (1999). Financial risk tolerance revisited: The development of a risk assessment instrument. Financial Services Review, 8, 163181.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Guenther, B., Galizzi, M. M., & Sanders, J. G. (in prep). P-DOSPERT: A new scale to assessing risk behaviours in times of a pandemic.Google Scholar
Guenther, B., Galizzi, M. M., & Sanders, J. G. (2021). Heterogeneity in risk-taking during the COVID-19 Pandemic: Evidence from the UK Lockdown. Frontiers in Psychology, 12. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.643653CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Guiso, L., & Paiella, M. (2008). Risk aversion, wealth, and background risk. Journal of the European Economic Association. https://doi.org/10.1162/JEEA.2008.6.6.1109CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harrison, G., Hofmeyr, A., Kincaid, H., Monroe, B., Ross, D., Schneider, M., & Swarthout, J. (2022). Subjective beliefs and economic preferences during the COVID-19 pandemic. Experimental Economics. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10683-021-09738-3CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heo, W., Rabbani, A., & Grable, J. E. (2021). An evaluation of the effect of the COVID-19 pandemic on the risk tolerance of financial decision makers. Finance Research Letters. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.frl.2020.101842CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hey, J. D., & Orme, C. (1994). Investigating generalizations of expected utility theory using experimental data. Econometrica, 62(6), 12911326. https://doi.org/10.2307/2951750CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Holt, C. A., & Laury, S. K. (2002). Risk aversion and incentive effects. American Economic Review. https://doi.org/10.1257/000282802762024700CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Houston, D. J., & Richardson, L. E. (2007). Risk compensation or risk reduction? Seatbelts, state laws, and traffic fatalities. Social Science Quarterly. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-6237.2007.00510.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Iorfa, S. K., Ottu, I. F. A., Oguntayo, R., Ayandele, O., Kolawole, S. O., Gandi, J. C., et al. (2020). COVID-19 knowledge, risk perception, and precautionary behavior among Nigerians: A moderated mediation approach. Frontiers In Psychology, 11, 566773. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.566773CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1979). Prospect theory: An analysis of decision under risk. Econometrica, 47, 263292. https://doi.org/10.2307/1914185CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kasting, M. L., Shapiro, G. K., Rosberger, Z., Kahn, J. A., & Zimet, G. D. (2016). Tempest in a teapot: A systematic review of HPV vaccination and risk compensation research. Human Vaccines and Immunotherapeutics. https://doi.org/10.1080/21645515.2016.1141158CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kimball, M. S. (1990). Precautionary saving in the small and in the large. Econometrica. https://doi.org/10.2307/2938334CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lejuez, C. W., Aklin, W. M., Jones, H. A., Strong, D. R., Richards, J. B., Kahler, C. W., & Read, J. P. (2003). The balloon analogue risk task (BART) differentiates smokers and nonsmokers. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology. https://doi.org/10.1037/1064-1297.11.1.26CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lejuez, C. W., Read, J. P., Kahler, C. W., Richards, J. B., Ramsey, S. E., Stuart, G. L., et al. Evaluation of a behavioral measure of risk taking: the Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 8(2), 7584. DOI: 10.1037//1076-898x.8.2.75.Google Scholar
Lejuez, C. W., Richards, J. B., Read, J. P., Kahler, C. W., Ramsey, S. E., Stuart, G. L., et al. (2002). Evaluation of a behavioral measure of risk taking: The balloon analogue risk task (BART). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied. https://doi.org/10.1037/1076-898X.8.2.75Google ScholarPubMed
Levym, D. T., & Miller, T. (2000). Review: Risk compensation literature – The theory and evidence. Journal of Crash Prevention and Injury Control, 2(1). https://doi.org/10.1080/10286580008902554CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liebst, L. S., Ejbye-Ernst, P., de Bruin, M., Thomas, J., & Lindegaard, M. R. (2022). No evidence that mask-wearing in public places elicits risk compensation behavior during the COVID-19 pandemic. Scientific Reports, 12(1), 1511. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-05270-3Google Scholar
Lohmann, P., Gsottbauer, E., You, J., & Kontoleon, A. (2023). Anti-social behaviour and economic decision-making: Panel experimental evidence in the wake of COVID-19. Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization, 206, 136171. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jebo.2022.12.007CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Loose, T., Wittmann, M., & Vásquez-Echeverría, A. (2022). Disrupting times in the wake of the pandemic: Dispositional time attitudes, time perception and temporal focus. Time & Society, 31(1), 110131.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luckman, A., Zeitoun, H., Isoni, A., Loomes, G., Vlaev, I., Powdthavee, N., & Read, D. (2021). Risk compensation during COVID-19: The impact of face mask usage on social distancing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 27(4), 722.Google ScholarPubMed
Marcus, J. L., Glidden, D. V., Mayer, K. H., Liu, A. Y., Buchbinder, S. P., Amico, K. R., et al. (2013). No evidence of sexual risk compensation in the iPrEx trial of daily oral HIV preexposure prophylaxis. PloS One, 8(12), e81997.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Markiewicz, Ł., & Weber, E. U. (2013). DOSPERT’s gambling risk-taking propensity scale predicts excessive stock trading. Journal of Behavioral Finance, 14(1), 6578. https://doi.org/10.1080/15427560.2013.762000CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mutz, M., & Gerke, M. (2021). Sport and exercise in times of self-quarantine: How Germans changed their behaviour at the beginning of the Covid-19 pandemic. International Review for the Sociology of Sport, 56(3), 305316. https://doi.org/10.1177/1012690220934335CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ogden, R. S. (2021). The passage of time during the UK Covid-19 lockdown. PLoS ONE, 15(7), e0235871. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0235871Google Scholar
Page, L., Savage, D. A., & Torgler, B. (2014). Variation in risk seeking behaviour following large losses: A natural experiment. European Economic Review. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.euroecorev.2014.04.009CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Powell, M., & Ansic, D. (1997). Gender differences in risk behaviour in financial decision-making: An experimental analysis. Journal of Economic Psychology, 18(6), 605628. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0167-4870(97)00026-3CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rolison, J. J., Hanoch, Y., & Wood, S. (2012). Risky decision making in younger and older adults: The role of learning. Psychology and Aging, 27(1), 129140. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0024689CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roszkowski, M. J., Delaney, M. M., & Cordell, D. M. (2009). Intraperson consistency in financial risk tolerance assessment: Temporal stability, relationship to total score, and effect on criterion-related validity. Journal of Business and Psychology, 24(4), 455467. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10869-009-9115-3CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sahm, C. R. (2012). How much does risk tolerance change? The Quarterly Journal of Finance, 2(4), 1250020. https://doi.org/10.1142/S2010139212500206CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Said, F., Afzal, U., & Turner, G. (2015). Risk taking and risk learning after a rare event: Evidence from a field experiment in Pakistan, Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 118, 167183. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jebo.2015.03.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sanders, J. G., & Jenkins, R. (2016). Weekly fluctuations in risk tolerance and voting behaviour. PLoS ONE, 11(7), e0159017. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0159017CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shachat, J., Walker, M., & Wei, L. (2021). How the onset of the Covid-19 pandemic impacted pro-social behaviour and individual preferences: Experimental evidence from China. Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization, 190, 480–494. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jebo.2021.08.001190.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Serafini, G., Parmigiani, B., Amerio, A., Aguglia, A., Sher, L., & Amore, M. (2020). The psychological impact of COVID-19 on the mental health in the general population. QJM, 113(8), 531537. https://doi.org/10.1093/qjmed/hcaa201CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Seres, G., Balleyer, A., Cerutti, N., Friedrichsen, J., & Süer, M. (2021). Face mask use and physical distancing before and after mandatory masking: No evidence on risk compensation in public waiting lines. Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization, 192, 765781. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jebo.2021.10.032CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Slovic, P., & Peters, E. (2006). Risk perception and affect. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 15(6), 322325. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8721.2006.00461.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Snowberg, E., & Yariv, L. (2021). Testing the waters: Behavior across participant pools. American Economic Review, 111(2), 687719. https://doi.org/10.1257/aer.20181065CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sun, L.-X., Chen, L.-L., Chen, W.-Y., Zhang, M.-X., Yang, M.-G., Mo, L.-C., et al. (2022). Association between health behaviours and the COVID-19 vaccination: Risk compensation among healthcare workers in Taizhou, China. Human Vaccines & Immunotherapeutics, 18(1), 2029257. https://doi.org/10.1080/21645515.2022.2029257CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tee, M. L., Tee, C. A., Anlacan, J. P., Aligam, K. J. G., Reyes, P. W. C., Kuruchittham, V., & Ho, R. C. (2020). Psychological impact of COVID-19 pandemic in the Philippines. Journal of Affective Disorders, 277, 379391. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2020.08.043CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Trimpop, R. M. (1994). What is risk taking behavior. In The Psychology of Risk Taking Behavior (vol. 107, pp. 114). North-Holland. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0166-4115(08)61295-9Google Scholar
Vieider, F. M., Lefebvre, M., Bouchouicha, R., Chmura, T., Hakimov, R., Krawczyk, M., & Martinsson, P. (2015). Common components of risk and uncertainty attitudes across contexts and domains: Evidence from 30 countries. Journal of the European Economic Association, 13(3), 421452. https://doi.org/10.1111/jeea.12102CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wadud, Z., Rahman, S. M., & Enam, A (2022). Face mask mandates and risk compensation: An analysis of mobility data during the COVID-19 pandemic in Bangladesh. BMJ Global Health, 7(1), e006803. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmjgh-2021-006803CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weber, E. U., Blais, A. R., & Betz, N. E. (2002). A domain-specific risk-attitude scale: Measuring risk perceptions and risk behaviors. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 15(4), 263290. https://doi.org/10.1002/bdm.414CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wilde, G. J. S. (1998). Risk homeostasis theory: An overview. Injury Prevention, 4(2), 8991. https://doi.org/10.1136/ip.4.2.89CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yan, Y., Malik, A. A., Bayham, J., Fenichel, E. P., Couzens, C., & Omer, S. B. (2021). Measuring voluntary and policy-induced social distancing behavior during the COVID-19 pandemic. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 118(16), e2008814118. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.2008814118Google ScholarPubMed

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×