Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-t5tsf Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-20T04:39:05.427Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

19 - To Know or Not to Know: Consciousness, Meta-consciousness, and Motivation

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  04 August 2010

Jonathan W. Schooler
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Pittsburgh
Charles A. Schreiber
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Pittsburgh
Joseph P. Forgas
Affiliation:
University of New South Wales, Sydney
Kipling D. Williams
Affiliation:
University of New South Wales, Sydney
Simon M. Laham
Affiliation:
University of New South Wales, Sydney
Get access

Summary

INTRODUCTION

Consciousness is invariably a slippery construct, but especially so when it comes to considering its relationship to motivation. To take an everyday example, consider the case of scratching an insect bite. When bitten by a mosquito, one knows that scratching will ultimately just increase the itching. Sometimes one simply gives in to the urge and deliberately scratches the darn thing, knowing full well the subsequent price that will be paid. In this case, it is clear that the motivation to scratch was fully conscious. But what about the case in which one suddenly catches oneself scratching the forbidden itch? There is this moment of realization to the effect, “Ahh, that feels good, but oh, I didn't want to do that.” In one sense, such actions lack conscious awareness, as the action that one is engaged in is precisely counter to one's goals. Moreover, there is the sense in which one has “caught” oneself “absentmindedly” engaging in the forbidden behavior. In another sense, however, the action and its concomitant motivation are conscious. That is, after one catches oneself, there is an appreciation of the pleasure that the scratching afforded as it temporarily relieved the itch. In short, when one catches oneself scratching an itch, there is a distinct sense that both the motivation to scratch (the itch) and the action itself (scratching) have been consciously experienced.

Type
Chapter
Information
Social Motivation
Conscious and Unconscious Processes
, pp. 351 - 372
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2004

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Adams, H. E., Wright, L. W., & Lohr, B. A. (1996). Is homophobia associated with homosexual arousal?Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 105(3), 440–445CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Asendorph, J. B., & Scherer, K. R. (1983). The discrepant repressor: Differentiation between low anxiety, high anxiety, and repression of anxiety by autonomic-facial-verbal patterns of behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 1334–1346CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bargh, J. A. (1997). Advances in social cognition. In R. S. Wyer, Jr. (Ed.), The automaticity of everyday life (pp. 1–61). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum
Baumeister, R. F., Dale, K., & Sommer, K. L. (1998). Freudian defense mechanisms and empirical findings in modern social psychology: Reaction formation, projection, displacement, undoing, isolation, sublimation, and denial. Journal of Personality, 66(6), 1081–1124CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berkowitz, L., Jaffee, S., Jo, E., & Troccoli, B. T. (2000). On the correction of feeling-induced judgmental biases. In J. P. Forgas (Ed.), Feeling and thinking: The role of affect in social cognition (pp. 131–152). New York: Cambridge University Press
Bond, C. F. (1981). Dissonance and the pill: An interpersonal simulation. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 7(3), 398–403CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brandimonte, M. A., Schooler, J. W., & Gabbino, P. (1997). Attentuating verbal overshadowing through visual retrieval cues. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 23, 915–931Google Scholar
Brown, C., & Lloyd-Jones, T. J. (2003). Verbal overshadowing of multiple face and car recognition: Effects of within versus across-category verbal descriptions. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 17, 183–201CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. S. (1990) Origins and functions of positive and negative affect: A control-process view. Psychological Review, 197, 19–35CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cooper, J., Zanna, M. P., & Taves, P. A. (1978). Arousal as a necessary condition for attitude change following induced compliance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36(10), 1101–1106CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dweck, C. S. (1999). Self-theories: Their role in motivation, personality, and development. Philadelphia: Psychology Press
Ekman, P. (1992). Telling lies: Clues to deceit in the marketplace, politics, and marriage. New York: W. W. Norton
Giambra, L. M. (1995). A laboratory method for investigating influences on switching attention to task-unrelated imagery and thought. Consciousness and Cognition, 4, 1–21CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jacoby, L. L., Yonelinas, A. P., & Jennings, J. M. (1997). The relation between conscious and unconscious (automatic) influences: A declaration of independence. In J. C. Cohen & J. W. Schooler (Eds.), Scientific approaches to consciousness (pp. 13–48). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum
McLeod, P., Reed, N., & Dienes, Z. (2003, July). What implicit knowledge and motor skill: People do not know about how they catch a ball. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the European Society of Philosophy and Psychology Congress, Turin, Italy
Meissner, C. A., & Brigham, J. C. (2001) A meta-analysis of the verbal overshadowing effect in face identification. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 15, 603–616CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meissner, C. A., Brigham, J. C., & Kelley, C. M. (2001). The influence of retrieval processes in verbal overshadowing. Memory and Cognition, 29(1), 176–186CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Melcher, J., & Schooler, J. W. (1996). The misremembrance of wines past: Verbal and perceptual expertise differentially mediate verbal overshadowing of taste. Journal of Memory and Language, 35, 231–245CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schooler, J. W. (2001). Discovering memories in the light of meta-consciousness. Journal of Aggression, Maltreatment and Trauma, 4, 105–136CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schooler, J. W. (2002) Re-representing consciousness: Dissociations between consciousness and meta-consciousness. Trends in Cognitive Science, 6, 339–344CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schooler, J. W., Ariely, D., & Loewenstein, G. (2003). The pursuit and assessment of happiness may be self-defeating. In J. Carrillo & I. Brocas (Eds.), Psychology and economics (pp. 41–70). Oxford: Oxford University Press
Schooler, J. W., & Engstler-Schooler, T. Y. (1990). Verbal overshadowing of visual memories: Some things are better left unsaid. Cognitive Psychology, 17, 36–71CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schooler, J. W., Fiore, S. M., & Brandimonte, M. A. (1997). At a loss from words: Verbal overshadowing of perceptual memories. In D. L. Medin (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (pp. 293–334). San Diego: Academic PressCrossRef
Schooler, J. W., Ohlsson, S., & Brooks, K. (1993). Thoughts beyond words: When language overshadows insight. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 122, 166–183CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schooler, J. W., Reichle, E. D., & Halpern, D. V. (in press). Zoning out during reading: Evidence for dissociations between experience and meta-consciousness. In D. T. Levin (Ed.), Thinking and seeing: Visual metacognition in adults and children. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
Sieck, W. R., Quinn, C. N., & Schooler, J. W. (1999). Justification effects on the judgment of analogy. Memory and Cognition, 27, 844–855CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wegner, D. M. (1994). Ironic processes of mental control. Psychological Review, 101, 34–52CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilson, T. D., Lisle, D. J., Schooler, J. W., Hodges, S. D., Klaaren, K. J., & LaFleur, S. J. (1993). Introspecting about reasons can reduce post-choice satisfaction. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 19, 331–339CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wilson, T. D., & Schooler, J. W. (1991). Thinking too much: Introspection can reduce the quality of preferences and decisions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 181–192CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×