Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-lnqnp Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-27T17:22:12.314Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

32 - The apraxias

from PART III - DISORDERS OF MOTOR CONTROL

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 August 2016

Ramón C. Leiguarda
Affiliation:
Raúl Carrea Institute of Neurological Research, FLENI, Buenos Aires, Argentina
Arthur K. Asbury
Affiliation:
University of Pennsylvania School of Medicine
Guy M. McKhann
Affiliation:
The Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine
W. Ian McDonald
Affiliation:
University College London
Peter J. Goadsby
Affiliation:
University College London
Justin C. McArthur
Affiliation:
The Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine
Get access

Summary

Apraxia is a term used to denote a wide spectrum of higherorder motor disorders owing to acquired brain disease affecting the performance of skilled, learned movements with or without preservation of the ability to perform the same movement outside the clinical setting in the appropriate situation or environment. The disturbance of purposive movements cannot be termed apraxia, however, if it results from a language comprehension disorder or from dementia, or if the patient suffers from any elementary motor or sensory deficit (i.e. paresis, dystonia, ataxia) which could fully explain the abnormal motor behaviour (Heilman & Rothi, 1985; Roy & Square, 1985; De Renzi, 1989). The praxic disorder may affect various body parts such as the eyes, face, trunk, or limbs, and may involve both sides of the body (i.e. ideational and ideomotor apraxias), preferentially one side (i.e. limb-kinetic apraxia), or, alternatively, interlimb coordination, as in the case of apraxia of gait.

Apraxias are poorly recognized but common disorders that can result from a wide variety of focal (i.e. stroke, trauma) or diffuse brain damage (i.e. corticobasal degeneration, Alzheimer's disease) (Heilman & Rothi, 1985; Freund, 1992). There are two main reasons why apraxia may go unrecognized. Firstly, many patients with apraxia, particularly ideomotor apraxia, show a voluntary–automatic dissociation, which means that the patient does not complain about the deficit because the execution of the movement in the natural context is relatively well preserved, and the deficit appears mainly in the clinical setting when the patient is required to represent explicitly the content of the action outside the situational props. Secondly, although in apraxic and aphasic patients specific functions are selectively affected, language and praxic disturbances frequently coexist and the former may interfere with the proper evaluation of the latter (Freund, 1992).

Limb apraxias

Liepmann (1920) posited that the idea of the action, or movement formula, containing the space–time form picture of the movement, was stored in the left parietal lobe. In order to carry out a skilled movement, the space–time plan has to be retrieved and associated via cortical connections with the innervatory pattern stored in the left sensorimotorium that conveys the information to the left primary motor area. When the left limb performs the movement, the information has to be transmitted from the left to the right sensorimotorium through the corpus callosum to activate, thereafter, the right motor cortex.

Type
Chapter
Information
Diseases of the Nervous System
Clinical Neuroscience and Therapeutic Principles
, pp. 462 - 476
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2002

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×