Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-94fs2 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-05T09:29:33.621Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

13 - Emotion and Memory

from Part II - Mechanisms of Cognitive Aging

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 May 2020

Ayanna K. Thomas
Affiliation:
Tufts University, Massachusetts
Angela Gutchess
Affiliation:
Brandeis University, Massachusetts
Get access

Summary

Emotional experiences are often more likely than neutral experiences to be remembered, or to be retrieved richly. In this chapter, we provide an overview of how the effects of emotion arise, emphasizing the effects that operate during the initial experience of the event (encoding), the storage and stabilization of the memory trace for that experience (consolidation), and the accessing of that trace (retrieval). We discuss how these effects of emotion can explain both why emotion enhances many aspects of memory throughout the adult life span and also why there are often age-by-valence interactions in memory, with older adults remembering information more positively than younger adults.

Type
Chapter
Information
The Cambridge Handbook of Cognitive Aging
A Life Course Perspective
, pp. 236 - 253
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2020

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Alger, S. E., Kensinger, E. A., & Payne, J. D. (2018). Preferential consolidation of emotionally salient information during a nap is preserved in middle age. Neurobiology of Aging, 68, 3447. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2018.03.030Google Scholar
Bergado, J. A., Lucas, M., & Richter-Levin, G. (2011). Emotional tagging – a simple hypothesis in a complex reality. Progress in Neurobiology, 94(1), 6476. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2011.03.004Google Scholar
Blick, K. A., & Howe, J. B. (1984). A comparison of the emotional content of dreams recalled by young and elderly women. Journal of Psychology, 116, 143146. doi: 10.1080/00223980.1984.9923629Google Scholar
Boshyan, J., Zebrowitz, L. A., Franklin, R. G., McCormick, C. M., & Carré, J. M. (2014). Age similarities in recognizing threat from faces and diagnostic cues. Journals of Gerontology, Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 69(5), 710718. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbt054CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bowen, H. J., Kark, S. M., & Kensinger, E. A. (2018). NEVER forget: Negative emotional valence enhances recapitulation. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 25(3), 870891. doi: 10.3758/s13423-017-1313-9Google Scholar
Bower, G. H., & Gilligan, S. G. (1979). Remembering information related to one’s self. Journal of Research in Personality, 13(4), 420432. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0092-6566(79)90005-9Google Scholar
Breslin, C. W., & Safer, M. A. (2013). Aging and long-term memory for emotionally valenced events. Psychology and Aging, 28, 346351. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0029554Google Scholar
Carstensen, L. L., Fung, H. H., & Charles, S. T. (2003). Socioemotional selectivity theory and the regulation of emotion in the second half of life. Motivation and Emotion, 27(2), 103123. http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/A:1024569803230Google Scholar
Clark-Foos, A., & Marsh, R. L. (2008). Recognition memory for valenced and arousing materials under conditions of divided attention. Memory, 16(5), 530537. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658210802007493CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Colegrove, F. W. (1899). Individual memories. American Journal of Psychology, 10, 228255. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/1412480Google Scholar
Comblain, C., D’Argembeau, A., & Van der Linden, M. (2005). Phenomenal characteristics of autobiographical memories for emotional and neutral events in older and younger adults. Experimental Aging Research, 31, 173189. doi: 10.1080/03610730590915010CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Craik, F. I. M. (1994). Memory changes in normal aging. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 3(5), 155158. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-8721.ep10770653Google Scholar
D’Argembeau, A., & Van der Linden, M. (2008). Remembering pride and shame: Self-enhancement and the phenomenology of autobiographical memory. Memory, 16, 538547. doi: 10.1080/09658210802010463CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davidson, P. S., Cook, S. P., & Glisky, E. L. (2006). Flashbulb memories for September 11th can be preserved in older adults. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 13, 196206. doi: 10.1080/13825580490904192Google Scholar
De Brigard, F., Giovanello, K. S., Stewart, G. W., et al. (2016). Characterizing the subjective experience of episodic past, future, and counterfactual thinking in healthy younger and older adults. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 69, 23582375. doi: 10.1080/17470218.2015.1115529Google Scholar
de Carvalho Myskiw, J., Benetti, F., & Izquierdo, I. (2013). Behavioral tagging of extinction learning. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 110, 10711076. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1220875110Google Scholar
Denburg, N. L., Buchanan, T. W., Tranel, D., & Adolphs, R. (2003). Evidence for preserved emotional memory in normal older persons. Emotion, 3(3), 239253. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.3.3.239CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dennis, N. A., & Cabeza, R. (2008). Neuroimaging of healthy cognitive aging. In Craik, F. I. M. & Salthouse, T. A. (Eds.), The handbook of aging and cognition, 3rd ed. (pp. 154). New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Dunsmoor, J. E., Murty, V. P., Davachi, L., & Phelps, E. A. (2015). Emotional learning selectively and retroactively strengthens memories for related events. Nature, 520, 345348. doi: 10.1038/nature14106Google Scholar
Fernandes, M., Ross, M., Wiegand, M., & Schryer, E. (2008). Are the memories of older adults positively biased? Psychology and Aging, 23, 297306. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.23.2.297Google Scholar
Fields, E. C., & Kuperberg, G. R. (2012). It’s all about you: An ERP study of emotion and self-relevance in discourse. NeuroImage, 62(1), 562574. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.05.003Google Scholar
Ford, J. H., DiBiase, H. D., & Kensinger, E. A. (2017). Finding the good in the bad: Age and event experience relate to the focus on positive aspects of a negative event. Cognition and Emotion, 32, 414421. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2017.1301387Google Scholar
Ford, J. H., & Kensinger, E. A. (2014). The relation between structural and functional connectivity depends on age and on task goals. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 112. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2014.00307Google Scholar
Ford, J. H., & Kensinger, E. A. (2017). Prefrontally-mediated alterations in the retrieval of negative events: Links to memory vividness across the adult lifespan. Neuropsychologia, 102, 8294. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.06.001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ford, J. H., & Kensinger, E. A. (2018). Older adults use a prefrontal regulatory mechanism to reduce negative memory vividness of a highly emotional real-world event. NeuroReport, 29(13), 11291134. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0000000000001084Google Scholar
Ford, J. H., Morris, J. A., & Kensinger, E. A. (2014). Neural recruitment and connectivity during emotional memory retrieval across the adult life span. Neurobiology of Aging, 35, 27702784. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2014.05.029CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frey, U., & Morris, R. G. (1997). Synaptic tagging and long-term potentiation. Nature, 385, 533536. doi: 10.1038/385533a0Google Scholar
Gallo, D. A. (2013). Retrieval expectations affect false recollection: Insights from a criterial recollection task. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 22, 316323. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0963721413481472Google Scholar
Gallo, D. A., Korthauer, L. E., McDonough, I. M., Teshale, S., & Johnson, E. L. (2011). Age-related positivity effects and autobiographical memory detail: Evidence from a past/future source memory task. Memory, 19, 641652. doi: 10.1080/09658211.2011.595723Google Scholar
Ge, R., Fu, Y., Wang, D., Yao, L., & Long, Z. (2014). Age-related alterations of brain network underlying the retrieval of emotional autobiographical memories: An fMRI study using independent component analysis. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 117. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00629Google Scholar
Goldin, P. R., McRae, K., Ramel, W., & Gross, J. J. (2008). The neural bases of emotion regulation: Reappraisal and suppression of negative emotion. Biological Psychiatry, 63(6), 577586. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.05.031Google Scholar
Gong, X., Fu, Y., Wang, D., Franz, E., & Long, Z. (2014). Remoteness modulates the effects of emotional valence on the neural network of autobiographical memory in older females. International Journal of Aging and Human Development, 79, 2354. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.2190/AG.79.1.bGoogle Scholar
Gregory, M. D., Agam, Y., Selvadurai, C., et al. (2014). Resting state connectivity immediately following learning correlates with subsequent sleep-dependent enhancement of motor task performance. NeuroImage, 102, 666673. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.08.044Google Scholar
Grieve, S. M., Clark, C. R., Williams, L. M., Peduto, A. J., & Gordon, E. (2005). Preservation of limbic and paralimbic structures in aging. Human Brain Mapping, 25(4), 391401. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20115Google Scholar
Gutchess, A., & Kensinger, E. A. (2018). Shared mechanisms may support mnemonic benefits from self-referencing and emotion. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 22(8), 712724. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2018.05.001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gutchess, A. H., Kensinger, E. A., & Schacter, D. L. (2010). Functional neuroimaging of self-referential encoding with age. Neuropsychologia, 48(1), 211219. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.09.006Google Scholar
Gutchess, A. H., Kensinger, E. A., Yoon, C., & Schacter, D. L. (2007). Ageing and the self-reference effect in memory. Memory, 15(8), 822837. doi: 10.1080/09658210701701394CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hashtroudi, S., Johnson, M. K., Vnek, N., & Ferguson, S. A. (1994). Aging and the effects of affective and factual focus on source monitoring and recall. Psychology and Aging, 9, 160170. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.9.1.160Google Scholar
Hess, T. M. (2014). Selective engagement of cognitive resources: Motivational influences on older adults’ cognitive functioning. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 9, 388407. doi: 10.1177/1745691614527465Google Scholar
Hirst, W., Phelps, E. A., Meksin, R., et al. (2015). A ten-year follow-up of a study of memory for the attack of September 11, 2001: Flashbulb memories and memories for flashbulb events. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 144, 604623. doi: 10.1037/xge0000055Google Scholar
Holland, C. A., & Rabbitt, P. M. (1990). Autobiographical and text recall in the elderly: An investigation of a processing resource deficit. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology A, 42, 441470. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/14640749008401232Google Scholar
Isaacowitz, D. M., Toner, K., Goren, D., & Wilson, H. R. (2008). Looking while unhappy: Mood-congruent gaze in young adults, positive gaze in older adults. Psychological Science, 19(9), 848853. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02167.xGoogle Scholar
Isaacowitz, D. M., Wadlinger, H. A., Goren, D., & Wilson, H. R. (2006). Selective preference in visual fixation away from negative images in old age? An eye-tracking study. Psychology and Aging, 21(1), 4048. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.21.1.40Google Scholar
Janssen, S. M., Rubin, D. C., & St. Jacques, P. L. (2011). The temporal distribution of autobiographical memory: Changes in reliving and vividness over the life span do not explain the reminiscence bump. Memory and Cognition, 39, 111. doi: 10.3758/s13421-010-0003-xGoogle Scholar
Johnson, M. K., Kuhl, B. A., Mitchell, K. J., Ankudowich, E., & Durbin, K. A. (2015). Age-related differences in the neural basis of the subjective vividness of memories: Evidence from multivoxel pattern classification. Cognitive, Affective, and Behavioral Neuroscience, 15, 644661. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13415-015-0352-9Google Scholar
Jones, B. J., Schultz, K. S., Adams, S., Baran, B., & Spencer, R. M. C. (2016). Emotional bias of sleep-dependent processing shifts from negative to positive with aging. Neurobiology of Aging, 45, 178189. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2016.05.019Google Scholar
Kalpouzos, G., Fischer, H., Rieckmann, A., Macdonald, S. W., & Bäckman, L. (2012). Impact of negative emotion on the neural correlates of long-term recognition in younger and older adults. Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience, 6, 125. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnint.2012.00074Google Scholar
Kapucu, A., Rotello, C. M., Ready, R. E., & Seidl, K. N. (2008). Response bias in “remembering” emotional stimuli: A new perspective on age differences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34, 703711. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.34.3.703Google Scholar
Kennedy, Q., Mather, M., & Carstensen, L. (2004). The role of motivation in the age-related positivity effect in autobiographical memory. Psychological Science, 15, 208214. doi: 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.01503011.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kensinger, E. A. (2008). Age differences in memory for arousing and nonarousing emotional words. Journals of Gerontology, Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 63(1), P13P18. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/geronb/63.1.P13Google Scholar
Kensinger, E. A. (2009). How emotion affects older adults’ memories for event details. Memory, 17, 208219. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09658210802221425CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kensinger, E. A. (2015). The future can shape memory for the present. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 19, 179180. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2015.02.008CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kensinger, E. A., Gutchess, A. H., & Schacter, D. L. (2007). Effects of aging and encoding instructions on emotion-induced memory trade-offs. Psychology and Aging, 22(4), 781795. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.22.4.781Google Scholar
Kensinger, E. A., Krendl, A. C., & Corkin, S. (2006). Memories of an emotional and a nonemotional event: Effects of aging and delay interval. Experimental Aging Research, 32, 2345. doi: 10.1080/01902140500325031CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kensinger, E. A., & Schacter, D. L. (2008). Neural processes supporting young and older adults’ emotional memories. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20(7), 11611173. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2008.20080Google Scholar
Knight, M., Seymour, T. L., Gaunt, J. T., et al. (2007). Aging and goal-directed emotional attention: Distraction reverses emotional biases. Emotion, 7(4), 705714. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.7.4.705Google Scholar
Kok, A. (1997). Event-related-potential (ERP) reflections of mental resources: A review and synthesis. Biological Psychology, 45(1), 1956. doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/S0301-0511(96)05221-0Google Scholar
Kukolja, J., Göreci, D. Y., Onur, Ö. A., Riedl, V., & Fink, G. R. (2016). Resting-state fMRI evidence for early episodic memory consolidation: Effects of age. Neurobiology of Aging, 45, 197211. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2016.06.004CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lang, F. R., & Carstensen, L. L. (2002). Time counts: Future time perspective, goals, and social relationships. Psychology and Aging, 17, 125139. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.17.1.125CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Langeslag, S. J. E., & van Strien, J. W. (2009). Aging and emotional memory: The co-occurrence of neurophysiological and behavioral positivity effects. Emotion, 9(3), 369377. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0015356Google Scholar
Leahy, F., Ridout, N., & Holland, C. (2018). Memory flexibility training for autobiographical memory as an intervention for maintaining social and mental well-being in older adults. Memory, 7, 113. doi: 10.1080/09658211.2018.1464582Google Scholar
Leclerc, C. M., & Kensinger, E. A. (2011). Neural processing of emotional pictures and words: A comparison of young and older adults. Developmental Neuropsychology, 36, 519538. doi: 10.1080/87565641.2010.549864CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levine, B., Svoboda, E., Hay, J. F., Winocur, G., & Moscovitch, M. (2002). Aging and autobiographical memory: Dissociating episodic from semantic retrieval. Psychology and Aging, 17, 677689. doi: 10.1037//0882-7974.17.4.677Google Scholar
Luchetti, M., & Sutin, A. R. (2018). Age differences in autobiographical memory across the adult lifespan: Older adults report stronger phenomenology. Memory, 26, 117130. doi: 10.1080/09658211.2017.1335326Google Scholar
Macrae, C. N., Moran, J. M., Heatherton, T. F., Banfield, J. F., & Kelley, W. M. (2004). Medial prefrontal activity predicts memory for self. Cerebral Cortex, 14(6), 647654. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhh025Google Scholar
Mather, M. (2012). The emotion paradox in the aging brain. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1251, 3349. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2012.06471.xGoogle Scholar
Mather, M., Canli, T., English, T., et al. (2004). Amygdala responses to emotionally valenced stimuli in older and younger adults. Psychological Science, 15(4), 259263. doi: 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00662.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mather, M., & Carstensen, L. L. (2005). Aging and motivated cognition: The positivity effect in attention and memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(10), 496502. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.08.005Google Scholar
Mather, M., & Johnson, M. K. (2000). Choice-supportive source monitoring: Do our decisions seem better to us as we age? Psychology and Aging, 15, 596606. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.15.4.596Google Scholar
Mather, M., & Knight, M. R. (2006). Angry faces get noticed quickly: Threat detection is not impaired among older adults. Journals of Gerontology, Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 61(1), 5457. doi: 10.1093/geronb/61.1.p54Google Scholar
Mather, M., & Sutherland, M. R. (2011). Arousal-biased competition in perception and memory. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 6(2), 114133. doi: 10.1177/1745691611400234Google Scholar
McReynolds, J. R., & McIntyre, C. K. (2012). Emotional modulation of the synapse. Reviews in the Neurosciences, 23(5–6), 449461. doi: 10.1515/revneuro-2012-0047CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mickley, K. R., & Kensinger, E. A. (2009). Phenomenological characteristics of emotional memories in younger and older adults. Memory, 17, 528543. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09658210902939363Google Scholar
Miller, E. K. (2000). The prefrontal cortex and cognitive control. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 1(1), 5965. doi: 10.1038/35036228Google Scholar
Murphy, N. A., & Isaacowitz, D. M. (2008). Preferences for emotional information in older and younger adults: A meta-analysis of memory and attention tasks. Psychology and Aging, 23(2), 263286. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.23.2.263Google Scholar
Murty, V. P., Sambataro, F., Das, S., et al. (2009). Age-related alterations in simple declarative memory and the effect of negative stimulus valence. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21, 19201933. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21130Google Scholar
Neisser, U., & Harsch, N. (1992). Phantom flashbulbs: False recollections of hearing the news about Challenger. In Winograd, E. & Neisser, U. (Eds.), Emory symposia in cognition, Affect and accuracy in recall: Studies of “flashbulb” memories (Vol. 4, pp. 932). New York: Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511664069.003Google Scholar
Ochsner, K. N., & Gross, J. J. (2007). The neural architecture of emotion regulation. In Gross, J. J. (Ed.), Handbook of emotion regulation (pp. 87109). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Ouidette, D., & Paller, K. A. (2013). Upgrading the sleeping brain with targeted memory reactivation. Trends in Cognitive Science, 17(3), 142149. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.01.006Google Scholar
Pace-Schott, E. F., & Spencer, R. M. (2015). Sleep-dependent memory consolidation in healthy aging and mild cognitive impairment. Current Topics in Behavioral Neurosciences, 25, 307330. doi: 10.1007/7854_2014_300Google Scholar
Payne, J. D., Chambers, A. M., & Kensinger, E. A. (2012). Sleep promotes lasting changes in selective memory for emotional scenes. Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience, 6, 112. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2012.00108Google Scholar
Payne, J. D., & Kensinger, E. A. (2018). Stress, sleep, and the selective consolidation of emotional memories. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 19, 3643. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2017.09.006Google Scholar
Phillips, M. L., Ladouceur, C. D., & Drevets, W. C. (2008). A neural model of voluntary and automatic emotion regulation: Implications for understanding the pathophysiology and neurodevelopment of bipolar disorder. Molecular Psychiatry, 13, 833857. doi: 10.1038/mp.2008.65Google Scholar
Piolino, P., Desgranges, B., Benali, K., & Eustache, F. (2002). Episodic and semantic remote autobiographical memory in ageing. Memory, 10, 239257. doi: 10.1080/09658210143000353CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reed, A. E., Chan, L., & Mikels, J. A. (2014). Meta-analysis of the age-related positivity effect: Age differences in preferences for positive over negative information. Psychology and Aging, 29(1), 115. doi: 10.1037/a0035194Google Scholar
Richter-Levin, G., & Akirav, I. (2003). Emotional tagging of memory formation – in the search for neural mechanisms. Brain Research Reviews, 43(3), 247256. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.brainresrev.2003.08.005Google Scholar
Rubin, D. C., & Schulkind, M. D. (1997). Distribution of important and word-cued autobiographical memories in 20-, 35-, and 70-year-old adults. Psychology and Aging, 12, 524535. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.12.3.524Google Scholar
Russell, J. A. (1980). A circumplex model of affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39(6), 11611178. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0077714Google Scholar
Schlagman, S., Kliegel, M., Schulz, J., & Kvavilashvili, L. (2009). Differential effects of age on involuntary and voluntary autobiographical memory. Psychology and Aging, 24, 397411. doi: 10.1037/a0015785Google Scholar
Siedlecki, K. L., Hicks, S., & Kornhauser, Z. G. (2015). Examining the positivity effect in autobiographical memory across adulthood. International Journal of Aging and Human Development, 80, 213232. doi: 10.1177/0091415015590311Google Scholar
Singer, J., Rexhaj, B., & Baddeley, J. (2007). Older, wiser and happier? Comparing older adults’ and college students’ self-defining memories. Memory, 15, 886898. doi: 10.1080/09658210701754351Google Scholar
Spencer, W. D., & Raz, N. (1995). Differential effects of aging on memory for content and context: A meta-analysis. Psychology and Aging, 10, 527539. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.10.4.527Google Scholar
St. Jacques, P. L., Dolcos, F., & Cabeza, R. (2009). Effects of aging on functional connectivity of the amygdala for subsequent memory of negative pictures: A network analysis of functional magnetic resonance imaging data. Psychological Science, 20(1), 7484. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02258.xGoogle Scholar
Uzer, T., & Gulgoz, S. (2015). Socioemotional selectivity in older adults: Evidence from the subjective experience of angry memories. Memory, 23, 888900. doi: 10.1080/09658211.2014.936877Google Scholar
Vuilleumier, P. (2005). How brains beware: Neural mechanisms of emotional attention. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(12), 585594. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.10.011Google Scholar
Wang, D. Y., Liu, D. Q., Li, S. F., & Zang, Y. F. (2012). Increased local synchronization of resting-state fMRI signal after episodic memory encoding reflects off-line memory consolidation. Neuroreport, 23, 873878. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0b013e3283587c96CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yonelinas, A. P., & Ritchey, M. (2015). The slow forgetting of emotional episodic memories: An emotional binding account. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 19, 259267. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2015.02.009Google Scholar
Zhou, H., Guo, J., Ma, X., et al. (2017). Self-reference emerges earlier than emotion during an implicit self-referential emotion processing task: Event-related potential evidence. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 11, 111. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2017.00451Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×