Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-2brh9 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-26T16:09:49.675Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Dissociative identity disorder needs re-examination

COMMENTARY ON… DISSOCIATIVE IDENTITY DISORDER

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 August 2019

Peter Tyrer*
Affiliation:
Emeritus Professor of Community Psychiatry in the Centre for Psychiatry at Imperial College, London, and Honorary Professor in Psychiatry at the University of Nottingham, UK.
*
Correspondence Professor Peter Tyrer, Imperial College, 7th Floor, Commonwealth Building, Hammersmith Hospital, London W12 0NN, UK. Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Summary

Dissociative identity disorder (DID) is as real as any other psychiatric disorder but has been over-diagnosed by gullible clinicians, especially in forensic settings. Its classification has been poor, but the new ICD-11 classification, especially of partial DID, should help research and practice.

DECLARATION OF INTEREST

None.

Type
Commentaries
Copyright
Copyright © The Royal College of Psychiatrists 2019. 

Paris (Reference Paris2019) presents a strong argument against the casual use of the diagnosis of dissociative identity disorder (DID) in forensic settings, but goes much further in hoping that the concept of DID can be ‘consigned to history’. This reminds me of Aubrey Lewis's magisterial rejoinder to Eliot Slater's follow-up studies of hysteria that suggested the diagnosis did not exist: ‘A tough old word like hysteria dies very hard. It tends to outlive its obituarists’ (Reference LewisLewis 1975). Fifteen years ago Piper & Merskey (Reference Piper and Merskey2004) made similar predictions about DID, but it remains very much alive despite all the criticism.

Why is this? In responding it is fair to quote the over-used mantra, ‘absence of evidence is not evidence of absence’. Paris is right to point out that there are no randomised trials of DID; virtually all the descriptions are case studies and there is sometimes a high level of gullibility in interpreting the evidence from these. This is particularly relevant in interpreting DID in forensic practice, but it would be wrong to suggest that practitioners in the area are unaware of the differences between simulation and truth (Brand Reference Brand, Webermann and Frankel2016).

Confusing literature

There is a great deal of literature on DID, much of it poor, speculative and dominated by a few noisy product champions, and the recent review by Dorahy et al (Reference Dorahy, Brand and Sar2014) describing it as ‘a complex, valid and not uncommon disorder, associated with developmental and cultural variables, that is amenable to psychotherapeutic intervention’ goes beyond the available data. But some facts are incontrovertible. DID is found not only in ‘patients who have been coached’ (Paris Reference Paris2019), is often associated with early trauma and may last for many years in the absence of any obvious motivation explaining persistence.

Trauma-dissociation is over-stated

Where the research data have been overblown is in adopting the trauma-dissociation model as the only cause. Such cause has not been demonstrated; in my view, it could only be properly evaluated by a large cohort study starting in infancy and some are now planned (Huntjens Reference Huntjens, Rijkeboer and Arntz2019). Lynn et al (Reference Lynn, Lilienfeld and Merckelbach2014), after reviewing the current evidence, conclude that ‘the field should now abandon the simple trauma-dissociation model and embrace multifactorial models that accommodate the diversity of causes’. Trauma alone will not do, and even though it dominates the literature on the subject it is freely acknowledged that this cannot be the only precipitant, as trauma lies behind a panoply of mental disorders (Temple Reference Temple2019).

DID in the courtroom: case law is not enough

As for research on DID in forensic psychiatry, something must be done to improve the evidence base. There is a tendency for the discipline in general to accept a lower standard of evidence to merit acceptance (Tyrer Reference Tyrer, Duggan and Cooper2015). It is not helped by evidence in criminal law being decided by single cases. In the rest of medicine evidence is decided by group studies; single cases are often outliers. In forensic work, however, to slightly misquote Tennyson, ‘so careless of the group it seems, so careful of the single case’. And the sheer excitement and plausibility created by those who claim DID in court just adds to the drama in criminal cases and is exploited unmercifully in works of fiction.

A clinical case for partial DID

DID is also described in the new ICD-11 classification. Apart from full dissociation of personality, in which there is no apparent knowledge of the other personality, a condition that is rare, there is also a description of partial dissociative identity disorder that many will find helpful. Again, this has limited verification, but in clinical experience is much more common. It is well described in the words of the classification:

‘Partial dissociative identity disorder is characterized by disruption of identity in which there are two or more distinct personality states (dissociative identities) associated with marked discontinuities in the sense of self and agency. Each personality state includes its own pattern of experiencing, perceiving, conceiving, and relating to self, the body, and the environment. One personality state is dominant and normally functions in daily life, but is intruded upon by one or more non-dominant personality states (dissociative intrusions). These intrusions may be cognitive, affective, perceptual, motor, or behavioural. They are experienced as interfering with the functioning of the dominant personality state and are typically aversive. The non-dominant personality states do not recurrently take executive control of the individual's consciousness and functioning, but there may be occasional, limited and transient episodes in which a distinct personality state assumes executive control to engage in circumscribed behaviours, such as in response to extreme emotional states or during episodes of self-harm or the re-enactment of traumatic memories’ (6B65 Partial dissociative identity disorder, World Health Organization 2019).

In my own clinical practice, partial DID makes a lot of sense. The ‘non-dominant’ personality can create havoc by temporarily taking over, particularly at times of stress. One of these ‘non-dominants’ I know, a truculent adolescent, managed to dispose of her husband's car keys when on a trip out, and the normal dominant personality spent many fruitless hours looking for them, completely spoiling the occasion. Is this partial DID or just plain unadulterated spite? I do not know. More research is needed, but in the end I predict that kernel of truth will remain.

Footnotes

See this issue.

References

Brand, BL, Webermann, AR, Frankel, AS (2016) Assessment of complex dissociative disorder patients and simulated dissociation in forensic contexts. International Journal of Law and Psychiatry, 49(Pt B): 197204.Google Scholar
Dorahy, MJ, Brand, BL, Sar, V, et al. (2014) Dissociative identity disorder: an empirical overview. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 48: 402–17.Google Scholar
Huntjens, RJC, Rijkeboer, MM, Arntz, A (2019) Schema therapy for Dissociative Identity Disorder (DID): rationale and study protocol. European Journal of Psychotraumatology, 10: 1571377.Google Scholar
Lewis, A (1975) The survival of hysteria. Psychological Medicine, 5: 912.Google Scholar
Lynn, SJ, Lilienfeld, SO, Merckelbach, H, et al. (2014) The trauma model of dissociation: inconvenient truths and stubborn fictions. Psychological Bulletin, 140: 896910.Google Scholar
Paris, J (2019) Dissociative identity disorder: validity and use in the criminal justice system. BJPsych Advances, this issue.Google Scholar
Piper, A, Merskey, H (2004) The persistence of folly: critical examination of dissociative identity disorder. Part II. The defence and decline of multiple personality or dissociative identity disorder. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 49: 678–83.Google Scholar
Temple, MJ (2019) Understanding, identifying and managing severe dissociative disorders in general psychiatric settings. BJPsych Advances, 25: 1425.Google Scholar
Tyrer, P, Duggan, C, Cooper, S, et al. (2015) The lessons and legacy of the programme for dangerous and severe personality disorders. Personality and Mental Health, 9: 98106.Google Scholar
World Health Organization (2019) ICD-11. WHO ( https://icd.who.int).Google Scholar
Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.