Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-g8jcs Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-26T05:17:12.622Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The bidirectional association of 24-h activity rhythms and sleep with depressive symptoms in middle-aged and elderly persons

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  11 August 2021

Maud de Feijter
Affiliation:
Department of Epidemiology, Erasmus MC University Medical Center, Rotterdam, the Netherlands
Desana Kocevska
Affiliation:
Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Erasmus MC University Medical Center, Rotterdam, the Netherlands Department of Sleep and Cognition, Netherlands Institute for Neuroscience, Amsterdam, the Netherlands
M. Arfan Ikram
Affiliation:
Department of Epidemiology, Erasmus MC University Medical Center, Rotterdam, the Netherlands
Annemarie I. Luik*
Affiliation:
Department of Epidemiology, Erasmus MC University Medical Center, Rotterdam, the Netherlands Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Erasmus MC University Medical Center, Rotterdam, the Netherlands
*
Author for correspondence: Annemarie I. Luik, E-mail: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

In older populations disturbed 24-h activity rhythms, poor sleep, and depressive symptoms are often lingering and co-morbid, making treatment difficult. To improve insights into these commonly co-occurring problems, we assessed the bidirectional association of sleep and 24-h activity rhythms with depressive symptoms in middle-aged and elderly persons.

Methods

In 1734 participants (mean age: 62.3 ± 9.3 years, 55% women) from the prospective Rotterdam Study, 24-h activity rhythms and sleep were estimated with actigraphy (mean duration: 146 ± 19.6 h), sleep quality with the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index, and depressive symptoms with the Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression scale. Repeated measures were available for 947 participants (54%) over a median follow-up of 6 years (interquartile range = 5.6–6.3). Linear-mixed models were used to assess temporal associations of 24-h activity rhythms and sleep with depressive symptoms in both directions.

Results

High 24-h activity rhythm fragmentation (IV) (B = 1.002, 95% confidence interval (CI) = 0.641–1.363), long time in bed (TIB) (B = 0.111, 95% CI = 0.053–0.169), low sleep efficiency (SE) (B = −0.015, 95% CI = −0.020 to −0.009), long sleep onset latency (SOL) (B = 0.009, 95% CI = 0.006–0.012), and low self-rated sleep quality (B = 0.112, 95% CI = 0.0992–0.124) at baseline were associated with increasing depressive symptoms over time. Conversely, more depressive symptoms at baseline were associated with an increasing 24-h activity rhythm fragmentation (B = 0.002, 95% CI = 0.001–0.003) and TIB (B = 0.009, 95% CI = 0.004–0.015), and a decreasing SE (B = −0.140, 95% CI = −0.196 to −0.084), SOL (B = 0.013, 95% CI = 0.008–0.018), and self-rated sleep quality (B = 0.193, 95% CI = 0.171–0.215) over time.

Conclusion

This study demonstrates a bidirectional association of 24-h activity rhythms, actigraphy-estimated sleep, and self-rated sleep quality with depressive symptoms over a time frame of multiple years in middle-aged and elderly persons.

Type
Original Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Poor sleep and depressive symptoms are highly common (Franzen & Buysse, Reference Franzen and Buysse2008; Murphy & Peterson, Reference Murphy and Peterson2015), especially in middle-aged and elderly persons (Bao et al., Reference Bao, Han, Ma, Wang, Shi, Wang and Tang2017; Luik, Zuurbier, Hofman, Van Someren, & Tiemeier, Reference Luik, Zuurbier, Hofman, Van Someren and Tiemeier2013). Moreover, symptoms frequently co-occur (Franzen & Buysse, Reference Franzen and Buysse2008; Vandeputte & de Weerd, Reference Vandeputte and de Weerd2003), and are accompanied by high physical distress and poor quality of life (Franzen & Buysse, Reference Franzen and Buysse2008; Olfson, Wall, Liu, Morin, & Blanco, Reference Olfson, Wall, Liu, Morin and Blanco2018). The co-occurrence of poor sleep and depressive symptoms does not merely reflect high prevalence of these disorders in old age (Fang, Tu, Sheng, & Shao, Reference Fang, Tu, Sheng and Shao2019). Instead, a causal link is suggested by clinical trials showing that psychological treatment of insomnia reduces depressive symptoms (Gee et al., Reference Gee, Orchard, Clarke, Joy, Clarke and Reynolds2018) and conversely that psychological treatment of depressive symptoms improves sleep and insomnia (Yon et al., Reference Yon, Scogin, DiNapoli, McPherron, Arean, Bowman and Reynolds Iii2014). Furthermore, population-based studies reported poor sleep to be predictive for onset and recurrence of depression (Bao et al., Reference Bao, Han, Ma, Wang, Shi, Wang and Tang2017; Hertenstein et al., Reference Hertenstein, Feige, Gmeiner, Kienzler, Spiegelhalder, Johann and Riemann2019; Smagula et al., Reference Smagula, Boudreau, Stone, Reynolds Iii, Bromberger, Ancoli-Israel and Cauley2015), and depressive symptoms to be predictive for development and worsening of sleep disturbances (Bao et al., Reference Bao, Han, Ma, Wang, Shi, Wang and Tang2017; Sun et al., Reference Sun, Shi, Bao, Sun, Shi and Lu2018).

There are several hypotheses about what aspect of the brain could underlie these associations as a common cause or mechanism (Asarnow, Reference Asarnow2019; Bao et al., Reference Bao, Han, Ma, Wang, Shi, Wang and Tang2017; Beck, Reference Beck2013; Brudzynski, Reference Brudzynski2014; Swaab, Fliers, & Partiman, Reference Swaab, Fliers and Partiman1985; Vadnie & McClung, Reference Vadnie and McClung2017); for example, deterioration of the superchiasmatic nucleus (SCN), because of its prominent role in the circadian sleep-wake cycle and functioning of the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) gland (Asarnow, Reference Asarnow2019; Swaab et al., Reference Swaab, Fliers and Partiman1985). Additionally, the prefrontal cortex, because of its role cognitive functioning (Beck, Reference Beck2013), and the ascending reticular activating system, because of its role in health perception (Brudzynski, Reference Brudzynski2014), have also commonly been suggested as potentially underlying the sleep–depression link. Yet, to understand underlying mechanisms, we first need to improve our understanding of the bidirectional association between sleep and depressive symptoms within the same time-span and population.

Our understanding of the bidirectional association is hampered because most studies use cross-sectional data or assess only one direction of the association. To estimate possible bidirectionality, it is required to investigate both directions of the association within a single-study population, using similar measurements and covariates. Furthermore, research has mainly looked into the effect of sleep duration (Zhai, Zhang, & Zhang, Reference Zhai, Zhang and Zhang2015), overall self-rated sleep quality (Bao et al., Reference Bao, Han, Ma, Wang, Shi, Wang and Tang2017), or insomnia (Fang et al., Reference Fang, Tu, Sheng and Shao2019) when taking bidirectionality into account, neglecting other aspects of sleep, such as sleep efficiency (SE) and sleep onset latency (SOL), and 24-h activity rhythms which may play an important role as well (de Feijter, Lysen, & Luik, Reference de Feijter, Lysen and Luik2020; Luik et al., Reference Luik, Zuurbier, Direk, Hofman, Van Someren and Tiemeier2015). Investigating multiple aspects of sleep could provide new insights important to understand the biological mechanisms underlying the association between poor sleep and depressive symptoms, and help us to develop and improve non-pharmacological and pharmacological treatments for poor sleep and depression.

We assessed the bidirectional association of 24-h activity rhythms, actigraphy-estimated sleep, and self-rated sleep quality with depressive symptoms to gain more insight into the temporality of these associations. Specifically, we estimated how 24-h activity rhythms and sleep at baseline were associated with depressive symptoms over time and its change over time. Vice versa, we also assessed how depressive symptoms at baseline were associated with 24-h activity rhythms and sleep over time and change in 24-h activity rhythms and sleep over time. Moreover, as these associations may be dependent on several factors, we assessed whether these associations were independent of a range of factors commonly associated with poor sleep and depression. To this end, we used repeatedly collected data from the prospective Rotterdam Study, a large population-based cohort of middle-aged and elderly persons.

Methods

Participants and design

Participants were included from the Rotterdam Study, an ongoing population-based cohort of middle-aged and elderly inhabitants of Rotterdam, the Netherlands. The study started in 1990 to investigate prevalence, history, and risk factors of common disease in elderly. So far, 14 926 participants aged ⩾45 years were included. Further details of the study design have been described elsewhere (Ikram et al., Reference Ikram, Brusselle, Ghanbari, Goedegebure, Ikram, Kavousi and Voortman2020).

From December 2004 until April 2007, we invited 2614 participants to wear an actigraph for 1 week for our baseline assessment, 2071 (79%) agreed. Of those that agreed, 279 participants were excluded because of incomplete or invalid baseline actigraphy (<4 complete 24-h periods of actigraphy data or <4 complete nights of actigraphy and sleep diary). Another 20 participants were excluded because of incomplete data on depressive symptoms. Finally, we excluded 38 participants because of a Mini Mental State Examination (Tombaugh & McIntyre, Reference Tombaugh and McIntyre1992) score below 23 or missing score as a poor cognitive status might impair the ability to complete questionnaires validly. Therefore, a total of 1734 participants were included at baseline.

Between February 2011 and July 2014, we aimed to follow-up these 1734 persons. Of those, 100 persons died, 326 withdrew from the overall study or refused to participate in the actigraphy sub-study, and 167 were not invited due to logistic reasons, leaving 1141 persons that participated in the second round of the actigraphy sub-study. For 194 participants measurements were excluded because data on 24-h activity rhythm, sleep, or depressive symptoms were incomplete or invalid at follow-up. Therefore, repeated data were available for 947 participants (55%) with a median follow-up of 6 years [interquartile range (IQR) = 5.6–6.3]. A flow diagram of the study population is presented in online Supplementary Table S1.

The Rotterdam Study has been approved by the Medical Ethics Committee of the Erasmus MC (registration number MEC 02.1015) and by the Dutch Ministry of Health, Welfare and Sport (Population Screening Act WBO, license number 1071272-159521-PG). The Rotterdam Study has been entered into the Netherlands National Trial Register (NTR; http://www.trialregister.nl/trial/6645) meeting the requirements by the WHO International Clinical Trials Registry Platform (ICTRP; http://www.who.int/ictrp/network/primary-registries/) under shared catalog number NTR6831. All participants provided written informed consent to participate in the study and to have their information obtained from treating physicians. The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this study comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008.

Measurement of 24-h activity rhythms and sleep

All participants were asked to wear an actigraph on their non-dominant wrist for 7 consecutive days and nights, and complete a sleep diary at the same time. Additionally, participants were asked to press a marker button on the actigraph when they initiated sleep (time to bed) and when they got out of bed (get-up time). At baseline, we used the ActiWatch, model AW4 (Cambridge Technology Ltd, Cambridge, UK), and at follow-up either the ActiWatch or the GENEActiv (ActivInsight Ltd, Kimbolton, UK) was used. Recordings were sampled at 32 Hz (ActiWatch) or 50 Hz (GENEActiv) and scored for each 30-s epoch, taking into account the weighted scores of previous and following epochs. To distinguish sleep from wake a threshold of 20 was used for each 30-s epoch (Kosmadopoulos, Sargent, Darwent, Zhou, & Roach, Reference Kosmadopoulos, Sargent, Darwent, Zhou and Roach2014). The z-axis data of the tri-axial GENEActiv data were pre-analyzed to make records comparable with ActiWatch data (te Lindert & Van Someren, Reference te Lindert and Van Someren2013). Actigraphy is a validated and commonly used instrument to objectively estimate sleep (Morgenthaler et al., Reference Morgenthaler, Alessi, Friedman, Owens, Kapur, Boehlecke and Lee-Chiong2007), and 24-h activity rhythms (Ancoli-Israel et al., Reference Ancoli-Israel, Cole, Alessi, Chambers, Moorcroft and Pollak2003), also in population-based samples (Marino et al., Reference Marino, Li, Rueschman, Winkelman, Ellenbogen, Solet and Buxton2013).

The 24-h activity rhythm was estimated by analyzing actigraphy data with the nparACT R package (Blume, Santhi, & Schabus, Reference Blume, Santhi and Schabus2016). We calculated the interdaily stability (IS), which indicates the stability of the rhythm over days, and intradaily variability (IV), which indicates the fragmentation of the rhythm relative to its 24-h amplitude (Van Someren, Reference Van Someren1997).

To estimate sleep, data from the actigraphy were supplemented with information from the sleep diary or marker button. Time in bed (TIB) was calculated as the difference between time to bed and get-up time. If time to bed or get-up time was missing from the sleep diary, information from the actigraphy marker button was used. Total sleep time (TST) was calculated as the total duration of epochs scored as sleep. SE was defined as the proportion of TIB spent sleeping [100% × (TST/TIB)]. SOL was defined as the time it took participants to fall asleep from time to bed. Wake after sleep onset (WASO) was calculated as the total time of the epochs scored as wake between sleep start and sleep end.

Self-rated sleep quality was determined with the widely used Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index (PSQI), a 19-item self-report scale on sleep quality (Buysse et al., Reference Buysse, Reynolds Iii, Monk, Hoch, Yeager and Kupfer1991). The PSQI has also been validated for measuring self-rated sleep quality in population-based samples (Mollayeva et al., Reference Mollayeva, Thurairajah, Burton, Mollayeva, Shapiro and Colantonio2016). The total score was calculated as the sum of all items and ranges from 0 to 21, with a high score indicating poor sleep quality. For participants with 6 out of 7 valid PSQI component scores, a weighted global score was calculated by multiplying with 7/6. If less than 6 component scores were available, PSQI was set to missing.

Measurement of depressive symptoms

Depressive symptoms were assessed using Dutch version of the Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression scale (CES-D), which consists of 20 items (Beekman, Van Limbeek, Deeg, Wouters, & Van Tilburg, Reference Beekman, Van Limbeek, Deeg, Wouters and Van Tilburg1994; Radloff, Reference Radloff1977). The CES-D has been validated for use in middle-aged and older adults (Beekman et al., Reference Beekman, Van Limbeek, Deeg, Wouters and Van Tilburg1994) and population-based cohorts (Cosco, Prina, Stubbs, & Wu, Reference Cosco, Prina, Stubbs and Wu2017). The total score ranges from 0 to 60, with a higher score indicating more severe depressive symptoms. A weighted score was calculated if ⩾75% of the questions were completed, if less than 75% of the items was completed, CES-D was set to missing.

Other variables

Based on previous literature, the following variables were assessed as possible confounders: age, sex, employment, education, partnership, smoking behavior, coffee intake, alcohol intake, and body mass index (BMI), and general cognitive function (Luik et al., Reference Luik, Zuurbier, Hofman, Van Someren and Tiemeier2013; Sun et al., Reference Sun, Shi, Bao, Sun, Shi and Lu2018). During the home interview information on employment, education, partnership, smoking behavior, and cognitive status was obtained. Education was classified as primary education (primary), lower/intermediate general education or lower vocational education (low), intermediate vocational education or higher general education (middle), or higher vocational education or university (high). Smoking behavior was classified as never, former, or current smoker. Intake of coffee and alcohol were assessed in the sleep diary as cups per day after 6 PM. To calculate BMI (kg/m2), height and weight were assessed on calibrated scales at the research center without heavy clothing and shoes.

Additionally, we determined general cognitive function by means of the g-factor (Deary, Reference Deary2012). The g-factor was estimated using principal component analyses including the color-word interference subtask of the Stroop test (Houx, Jolles, & Vreeling, Reference Houx, Jolles and Vreeling1993), the Letter Digit Substitution Task (Lezak, Howieson, Loring, & Fischer, Reference Lezak, Howieson, Loring and Fischer2004), a verbal Word Fluency Test (Welsh et al., Reference Welsh, Butters, Mohs, Beekly, Edland, Fillenbaum and Heyman1994), the delayed recall score of a 15-word Word List Learning test (Bleecker, Bolla-Wilson, Agnew, & Meyers, Reference Bleecker, Bolla-Wilson, Agnew and Meyers1988), and Purdue pegboard test (Tiffin & Asher, Reference Tiffin and Asher1948).

Statistical analyses

Descriptives are presented as number with percentage for categorical variables and mean with standard deviation (s.d.) for numerical data. To assess non-response at follow-up, demographic and health characteristics of participants with repeated measurements (n = 947) were compared to those with only measurements at baseline (n = 787) using chi-squared, Mann–Whitney U tests, or independent sample t tests.

Sleep and 24-h activity rhythms were checked for outliers, above 4 s.d., were set to 4 s.d. from the mean in the same direction. SOL and depressive symptom scores were log-transformed when used as an outcome. For all covariates, missing values were less than 5%, these were handled using the multiple imputation MICE R package (Buuren & Groothuis-Oudshoorn, Reference Buuren and Groothuis-Oudshoorn2010). Multiple imputation resulted in five imputed datasets for which the pooled statistics are presented (Rubin, Reference Rubin2004). Based on eight independent exposures, we used a Bonferroni-corrected p value = 0.00625, based on an alpha of 0.05, to account for multiple testing. For all estimates, the 95% confidence intervals (CIs) were calculated and presented. Analyses were performed in R version R 3.6.3 (R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria; http://www.R-project.org).

Cross-sectional linear regression models were used to estimate the association of 24-h activity rhythms and sleep with depressive symptoms. All associations were analyzed using three different models: in model 1 we adjusted for sex and age only. In model 2 we adjusted for above-mentioned confounders, age, sex, cohort, employment, education, partnership, smoking, coffee intake, alcohol consumption, and BMI. In model 3 we additionally adjusted for general cognitive function, which could be a confounder or mediator. For TST and TIB, analyses were repeated using quadratic terms to assess non-linear associations.

Linear-mixed models were used to assess repeated measurements over time. To account for the within person correlation between measurements, a random intercept and slope were included in all linear-mixed models. All linear-mixed models included the baseline value of the determinant and an interaction term of the baseline determinant with time. The effect estimate of the baseline determinant describes how the determinant at baseline is associated with the outcome on average over time. The interaction term between the baseline determinant and time describes how the determinant at baseline is associated with changes in outcome over time. All associations were adjusted according to the above specified models 1, 2, and 3, with all models additionally adjusted for follow-up time and type of actigraphy recorder at follow-up. For TST and TIB, analyses were repeated using quadratic terms to assess non-linear associations.

We repeated all analyses with a recalculated depressive symptoms score, excluding the question assessing problems with sleep (question 11).

Results

At baseline, 1734 participants were included with a mean age of 62.3 ± 9.3 years and 55% women (see Table 1). Valid repeated measurements were obtained for 947 participants with a median follow-up time of 6 years (IQR = 5.6–6.3). Participants with only baseline measurements were older (t = 5.43, p < 0.001), more often women (χ2 = 8.96, p = 0.002), more often without a partner (χ2 = 15.17, p < 0.001), lower educated (χ2 = 17.65, p < 0.001), more often unemployed (χ2 = 6.11, p = 0.050), and overall less healthy compared to participants with repeated measurements (online Supplementary Table S1). Additionally, their 24-h activity rhythm was more disturbed and their sleep poorer (online Supplementary Table S1).

Table 1. Baseline characteristics of the study population at baseline (December 2004–April 2007), Rotterdam Study

s.d., standard deviation.

Sleep quality is missing for 29 participants (1.7%), partnership for 2 (0.1%), education for 14 (0.8%), alcohol and coffee intake for 36 (2.1%), BMI for 15 (0.9%), and general health score for 66 (3.8%). For other variables there are no missing values.

a Median and IQR.

b Assessed using the Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression scale.

c Assessed using the Mini-Mental State Exam.

d Alcohol and coffee intake were assessed using the actigraphy sleep dairy.

Cross-sectional associations

At baseline, a fragmented 24-h activity rhythm (B = 0.958, 95% CI = 0.581–1.336), long TIB (B = 0.113, 95% CI = 0.052–0.174), low SE (B = −0.014, 95% CI = −0.019 to −0.008), long SOL (B = 0.009, 95% CI = 0.006–0.012), and low self-rated sleep quality (where a high score reflects poor sleep quality, B = 0.116, 95% CI = 0.103–0.129) were associated with more log-transformed depressive symptoms when adjusted for confounders (model 2) (see Table 2). When assessing quadratic association, TST and TIB were not significantly associated with depressive symptoms.

Table 2. Cross-sectional associations of 24-h activity rhythms and sleep with depressive symptoms at baseline

CI, confidence interval for B.

B is the effect estimate of the determinant and β is the standardized effect estimate of the determinant. Effect estimates were obtained using cross-sectional linear regression models, adjusted for age, sex, cohort, actigraphy device at follow-up, employment, education, partnership, smoking, alcohol consumption, coffee intake, and BMI (model 2). Multiple testing-corrected p value = 0.00625.

Longitudinal associations

Poor sleep at baseline was associated with more depressive symptoms over 6 years of follow-up and slower increase of depressive symptoms over time (Table 3; online Supplementary Fig. S2). Each 1-point higher fragmentation of the 24-h activity rhythm was associated with 1.002 (95% CI = 0.641–1.363) more log-transformed depressive symptoms over 6 years of follow-up when adjusted for confounders (model 2). Similarly, a long TIB (B = 0.111, 95% CI = 0.053–0.169), low SE (B = −0.015, 95% CI = −0.020 to −0.009), long SOL (B = 0.009 (95% CI = 0.006–0.012), and high self-rated sleep quality score (where a high score reflects poor sleep quality, B = 0.112, 95% CI = 0.099–0.124) increased depressive symptoms. When assessing quadratic associations, TST and TIB were not significantly associated with depressive symptoms. With every 1-min longer SOL at baseline, the yearly increase of depressive symptoms was reduced with 0.001 (95% CI = −0.001 to 0.000) after adjustment for confounders (model 2). Similarly, with every 1-point higher self-rated sleep quality, the yearly increase of depressive symptoms was reduced with 0.005 (95% CI = −0.008 to −0.002). In other words, long SOL (online Supplementary Fig. S2F) and poor self-rated sleep (online Supplementary Fig. S2H) at baseline were associated with a less steep increase of depressive symptoms over time.

Table 3. Longitudinal association of 24-h activity rhythms and sleep with depressive symptoms over time

TST, total sleep time; TIB, time in bed; SE, sleep efficiency; WASO, wake after sleep onset; SOL, sleep onset latency; IS, interdaily stability; IV, intradaily variability; CI, confidence interval for B.

B is the effect estimate of the determinant and β is the standardized effect estimate of the determinant. Effect estimates were obtained using linear-mixed models, adjusted for age, sex, cohort, actigraphy device at follow-up, employment, education, partnership, smoking, alcohol consumption, coffee intake, and BMI (model 2). Estimates should be interpreted as changes in the log-transformed depressive symptoms score. Multiple testing-corrected p value = 0.00625.

Vice versa, having more depressive symptoms at baseline was also associated with decreased sleep over 6 years of follow-up and slower worsening of sleep over time (Table 4; online Supplementary Fig. S3). A 1-point higher depressive symptoms score at baseline was associated with a 0.002 (95% CI = 0.001–0.003) increased fragmentation of the 24-h activity rhythm over 6 years of follow-up when adjusted for confounders (model 2). Similarly, having more depressive symptoms was associated with increased TIB (B = 0.009, 95% CI = 0.004–0.015), decreased SE (B = −0.140, 95% CI = −0.196 to −0.084), increased log-transformed SOL (B = 0.013, 95% CI = 0.008–0.018), and decreased self-rated sleep quality score (B = 0.193, 95% CI = 0.171–0.215, a high score reflects poor sleep quality). Although TST was not significantly associated with a higher intercept, with every 1-point higher depressive symptom score at baseline the yearly decrease of TST was reduced with approximately 0.002 h (95% CI = 0.001–0.003) after adjustment for confounders (model 2). Similar results were observed for SE (B = 0.022, 95% CI = 0.011–0.033) and self-rated sleep quality (B = −0.014, 95% CI = −0.019 to −0.009). In other words, more depressive symptoms at baseline were associated with a less steep decrease in TST (online Supplementary Fig. S3D) and a less steep increase in SE (online Supplementary Fig. S3E), and self-rated sleep quality score (online Supplementary Fig. S3H) over time.

Table 4. Longitudinal association of depressive symptoms with 24-h activity rhythms and sleep over time

TST, total sleep time; TIB, time in bed; SE, sleep efficiency; WASO, wake after sleep onset; SOL, sleep onset latency; IS, interdaily stability; IV, intradaily variability; CI, confidence interval for B.

B is the effect estimate of the determinant and β is the standardized effect estimate of the determinant. Effect estimates were obtained using linear-mixed models, adjusted for age, sex, cohort, actigraphy device at follow-up, employment, education, partnership, smoking, alcohol consumption, coffee intake, and BMI (model 2). Estimates should be interpreted as changes in the log-transformed depressive symptoms score. Multiple testing-corrected p value = 0.00625.

Analyses adjusted for sex and age only showed similar results (data not shown). Results did not change substantially when estimates were additionally adjusted for general cognitive function (model 3, online Supplementary Tables S2 and S3).

Sensitivity analyses

Results did not substantially change when analyses were repeated using the depressive symptoms score without the sleep item (data not shown).

Discussion

We demonstrated a bidirectional association of 24-h activity rhythms and sleep with depressive symptoms in a population-based cohort of middle-aged and elderly persons. For 24-h activity rhythms, only fragmentation of the 24-h activity rhythm was bidirectionally associated with more depressive symptoms. For sleep, we observed that long TIB, low actigraphy-estimated SE, long actigraphy-estimated SOL, and poor self-rated sleep quality were bidirectionally associated with more depressive symptoms.

Within a single-study population, we demonstrated a temporal association of fragmented 24-h activity rhythms and sleep with depressive symptoms over time, and vice versa, indicating a bidirectional association. This is in line with previous studies which have showed either direction (Fang et al., Reference Fang, Tu, Sheng and Shao2019; Maglione et al., Reference Maglione, Ancoli-Israel, Peters, Paudel, Yaffe, Ensrud and Stone2014), but never assessed both directions using objectively estimated sleep measures in a single-study population (Maglione et al., Reference Maglione, Ancoli-Israel, Peters, Paudel, Yaffe, Ensrud and Stone2014; Smagula et al., Reference Smagula, Krafty, Taylor, Martire, Schulz and Hall2017). A bidirectional association of 24-h activity rhythms and sleep with depressive symptoms seems plausible. On the one hand, fragmented 24-h activity rhythms and sleep disturbances may cause depressive symptoms through internal desynchronization of 24-h rhythms (Pandi-Perumal et al., Reference Pandi-Perumal, Moscovitch, Srinivasan, Spence, Cardinali and Brown2009), impaired Rapid Eye Movement (REM) sleep (Wang et al., Reference Wang, Li, Zhang, Zhang, Qu and Huang2015), or impaired cognitive function (Helfrich et al., Reference Helfrich, Lendner, Mander, Guillen, Paff, Mnatsakanyan and Knight2019; Muzur, Pace-Schott, & Hobson, Reference Muzur, Pace-Schott and Hobson2002) for example. On the other hand, depressed people often experience more stress and worry, potentially linked to a moor pore function of the pre-frontal cortex (Belleau, Treadway, & Pizzagalli, Reference Belleau, Treadway and Pizzagalli2019; Liu et al., Reference Liu, Ge, Leng, Pan, Fan, Yang and Cui2017), which could also lead to difficulties in falling asleep, lower SE, and poorer self-rated sleep quality (Hall et al., Reference Hall, Casement, Troxel, Matthews, Bromberger, Kravitz and Buysse2015; Van Laethem, Beckers, van Hooff, Dijksterhuis, & Geurts, Reference Van Laethem, Beckers, van Hooff, Dijksterhuis and Geurts2016). In addition, depressed people may experience difficulties in performing daily tasks for a long period of time (Liu, Peng, Zhang, & Tang, Reference Liu, Peng, Zhang and Tang2018), which could lead to less intensive activity peaks and more fragmentation of 24-h activity rhythms. Of note, within the sleep–depression link, cognitive function is often deemed particularly relevant, as both concepts have direct relations with cognition (Beck, Reference Beck2013; Belleau et al., Reference Belleau, Treadway and Pizzagalli2019; Helfrich et al., Reference Helfrich, Lendner, Mander, Guillen, Paff, Mnatsakanyan and Knight2019). Yet, in our study cognitive function at baseline did not affect the associations of sleep with depression or vice versa. It might be that our general indicator of cognitive function was not sufficiently able to pick up subtle changes in cognition, or that instead of general cognition, one particular construct such as executive function affects the association of sleep and depressive symptoms.

The observed bidirectional association between poor sleep and depressive symptoms could also be explained by a common cause, a third factor that explains changes in the 24-h activity rhythm and sleep as well as depressive symptoms. One such common cause could be degeneration of the SCN, the biological clock of the brain, which is an important cause of increased fragmentation of the 24-h activity rhythm and other sleep problems (Swaab et al., Reference Swaab, Fliers and Partiman1985) and is associated with depressive disorders (Costa, Carvalho, & Fernandes, Reference Costa, Carvalho and Fernandes2013; Vadnie & McClung, Reference Vadnie and McClung2017). Degeneration of the SCN hampers functioning of structures that affect both sleep and mood, such as the HPA-axis (Asarnow, Reference Asarnow2019; Murri et al., Reference Murri, Pariante, Mondelli, Masotti, Atti, Mellacqua and Zanetidou2014). The HPA-axis is associated with both sleep disturbances and mood disorders via elevation of cortisol levels and hyperarousal (Bao et al., Reference Bao, Han, Ma, Wang, Shi, Wang and Tang2017). Of note, degeneration of the SCN could also reflect associated degeneration in other parts of the brain such as the diencephalon, telencephalon, which includes the previously described prefrontal cortex, and the reticular formation in the brain stem (Mitrushina & Tomaszewski, Reference Mitrushina and Tomaszewski2019; Musiek et al., Reference Musiek, Bhimasani, Zangrilli, Morris, Holtzman and Ju2018). This degeneration in other parts of the brain might lead to altered cognitive functioning or health perception which might in turn affect both sleep and depression (Leng, Musiek, Hu, Cappuccio, & Yaffe, Reference Leng, Musiek, Hu, Cappuccio and Yaffe2019; Mitrushina & Tomaszewski, Reference Mitrushina and Tomaszewski2019; Musiek et al., Reference Musiek, Bhimasani, Zangrilli, Morris, Holtzman and Ju2018).

Our results furthermore demonstrate that poor sleep at baseline is associated with a slower increase of depressive symptoms. Vice versa, having more depressive symptoms at baseline was also associated with a slower worsening of sleep. As we only have measured sleep and depression at two time points it is difficult to explain these somewhat counterintuitive findings. It could be hypothesized that persons with poor sleep at baseline also have more depressive symptoms at baseline and therefore have less possibility for an increase of depressive symptoms or worsening of sleep. Yet, this seems unlikely in our population-based sample which shows lower scores on sleep quality and depression than clinical samples. In addition, people with poor sleep at baseline still seem to have stable high depressive symptoms over time. Similarly, people with more depressive symptoms at baseline seem to have stable poor sleep over time.

Commonly, a discrepancy between objective and self-rated measures is observed for the association of sleep with depression (Van Den Berg et al., Reference Van Den Berg, Van Rooij, Vos, Tulen, Hofman, Miedema and Tiemeier2008), where self-rated measures reflect more of the subjective experience and tend to be more strongly related to depressive symptoms (Maglione et al., Reference Maglione, Ancoli-Israel, Peters, Paudel, Yaffe, Ensrud and Stone2014; Mitrushina & Tomaszewski, Reference Mitrushina and Tomaszewski2019). Dysfunction of the reticular activating system, important for a person's consciousness and health perception (Brudzynski, Reference Brudzynski2014; Hudson, Reference Hudson2009), could potentially lead to a heightened consciousness about their sleep and depressive symptoms. Based on our standardized results it seems that subjective, or self-rated, sleep indeed shows stronger temporal associations for sleep and depressive symptoms than purely objectively estimated measures such as sleep duration and sleep fragmentation. Additionally, aspects of sleep that integrate information from the sleep diary, which is self-reported, such as SOL and TIB, also seem to be more strongly associated with depressive symptoms.

Several limitations need to be taken into account. First, we used two different actigraphy devices at follow-up. Although we have controlled for this in our analyses this might have induced measurement error. Second, our nonresponse analyses revealed differences between participants with repeated measurements and participants with only a baseline measurement, possibly introducing selection bias. Finally, although having repeated measurements is a strength, we were only able to include two time points, 5 to 9 years apart. We could therefore not take into account fluctuations in depressive symptoms and sleep occurring between these two time points. Third, the number of reported depressive symptoms is relatively low in our sample, about 9% of our study population reached the cut-off for clinically relevant depressive symptoms, which fits with the population-based design. Our approach however does allow us to take into account subclinical symptoms of depression which are more frequent at older ages. Finally, the effect sizes in our study are relatively small, in part as a consequence of the population-based setting, which might limit the clinical relevance. Nevertheless, having repeated objective sleep measurements and repeated depressive symptom assessments over this period of time is unique in this field and allowed us to assess bidirectionality within one study population.

In our study population of middle-aged and elderly we demonstrated a bidirectional association between fragmented 24-h activity rhythms, low actigraphy-estimated SE, long actigraphy-estimated SOL, and poor self-rated sleep quality and depressive symptoms over time. This bidirectionality in both objectively estimated aspects of sleep as well as self-rated sleep quality with depressive symptoms over time should be taken into account when assessing underlying mechanisms and when developing and improving non-pharmacological and pharmacological prevention strategies and treatments for poor sleep and depression.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S003329172100297X.

Acknowledgements

We are grateful to all the participants of the Rotterdam Study, the staff from the Rotterdam Study, and the participating general practitioners and pharmacists.

Financial support

The Rotterdam Study is funded by Erasmus Medical Center and Erasmus University, Rotterdam, the Netherlands Organization for the Health Research and Development (ZonMw), the Research Institute for Diseases in the Elderly (RIDE), the Ministry of Education, Culture and Science, the Ministry of Health, Welfare and Sports, the European Commission (DG XII), and the Municipality of Rotterdam. The work of Desana Kocevska was supported by a NWA Startimpuls KNAW 2017 Grant (AZ/3137).

Conflict of interest

None.

References

Ancoli-Israel, S., Cole, R., Alessi, C., Chambers, M., Moorcroft, W., & Pollak, C. P. (2003). The role of actigraphy in the study of sleep and circadian rhythms. Sleep, 26(3), 342392.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Asarnow, L. D. (2019). Depression and sleep: What has the treatment research revealed and could the HPA axis be a potential mechanism? Current Opinion in Psychology, 34, 112116. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2019.12.002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bao, Y. P., Han, Y., Ma, J., Wang, R. J., Shi, L., Wang, T. Y., … Tang, X. D. (2017). Cooccurrence and bidirectional prediction of sleep disturbances and depression in older adults: Meta-analysis and systematic review. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 75, 257273. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.01.032CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beck, J. S. (2013). Terapia cognitivo-comportamental. Porto Alegre, Portugal: Artmed Editora.Google Scholar
Beekman, A. T. F., Van Limbeek, J., Deeg, D. J. H., Wouters, L., & Van Tilburg, W. (1994). Een screeningsinstrument voor depressie bij ouderen in de algemene bevolking: De bruikbaarheid van de center for epidemiologic studies depression scale (CES-D). Tijdschrift voor Gerontologie en Geriatrie, 25, 95103.Google Scholar
Belleau, E. L., Treadway, M. T., & Pizzagalli, D. A. (2019). The impact of stress and major depressive disorder on hippocampal and medial prefrontal cortex morphology. Biological Psychiatry, 85(6), 443453.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bleecker, M. L., Bolla-Wilson, K., Agnew, J., & Meyers, D. A. (1988). Age-related sex differences in verbal memory. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 44(3), 403411.3.0.CO;2-0>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blume, C., Santhi, N., & Schabus, M. (2016). ‘nparACT’ package for R: A free software tool for the non-parametric analysis of actigraphy data. MethodsX, 3, 430435. doi: 10.1016/j.mex.2016.05.006CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brudzynski, S. M. (2014). The ascending mesolimbic cholinergic system – A specific division of the reticular activating system involved in the initiation of negative emotional states. Journal of Molecular Neuroscience, 53(3), 436445.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buuren, S. V., & Groothuis-Oudshoorn, K. (2010). mice: Multivariate imputation by chained equations in R. Journal of Statistical Software, 45(3), 168.Google Scholar
Buysse, D. J., Reynolds Iii, C. F., Monk, T. H., Hoch, C. C., Yeager, A. L., & Kupfer, D. J. (1991). Quantification of subjective sleep quality in healthy elderly men and women using the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index (PSQI). Sleep, 14(4), 331338.Google ScholarPubMed
Cosco, T. D., Prina, M., Stubbs, B., & Wu, Y.-T. (2017). Reliability and validity of the center for epidemiologic studies depression scale in a population-based cohort of middle-aged US adults. Journal of Nursing Measurement, 25(3), 476485.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Costa, I. C., Carvalho, H. N., & Fernandes, L. (2013). Aging, circadian rhythms and depressive disorders: A review. American Journal of Neurodegenerative Disease, 2(4), 228246.Google Scholar
Deary, I. J. (2012). Intelligence. Annual Review of Psychology, 63(1), 453482. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-120710-100353CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de Feijter, M., Lysen, T. S., & Luik, A. I. (2020). 24-h Activity rhythms and health in older adults. Current Sleep Medicine Reports, 6, 7683. doi: 10.1007/s40675-020-00170-2CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fang, H., Tu, S., Sheng, J., & Shao, A. (2019). Depression in sleep disturbance: A review on a bidirectional relationship, mechanisms and treatment. Journal of Cellular and Molecular Medicine, 23(4), 23242332. doi: 10.1111/jcmm.14170CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Franzen, P. L., & Buysse, D. J. (2008). Sleep disturbances and depression: Risk relationships for subsequent depression and therapeutic implications. Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience, 10(4), 473481.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gee, B., Orchard, F., Clarke, E., Joy, A., Clarke, T., & Reynolds, S. (2018). The effect of non-pharmacological sleep interventions on depression symptoms: A meta-analysis of randomised controlled trials. Sleep Medicine Reviews, 43, 118128.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hall, M. H., Casement, M. D., Troxel, W. M., Matthews, K. A., Bromberger, J. T., Kravitz, H. M., … Buysse, D. J. (2015). Chronic stress is prospectively associated with sleep in midlife women: The SWAN sleep study. Sleep, 38(10), 16451654. doi: 10.5665/sleep.5066CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Helfrich, R. F., Lendner, J. D., Mander, B. A., Guillen, H., Paff, M., Mnatsakanyan, L., … Knight, R. T. (2019). Bidirectional prefrontal-hippocampal dynamics organize information transfer during sleep in humans. Nature Communications, 10(1), 116.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hertenstein, E., Feige, B., Gmeiner, T., Kienzler, C., Spiegelhalder, K., Johann, A., … Riemann, D. (2019). Insomnia as a predictor of mental disorders: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Sleep Medicine Reviews, 43, 96105.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Houx, P. J., Jolles, J., & Vreeling, F. W. (1993). Stroop interference: Aging effects assessed with the Stroop color-word test. Experimental Aging Research, 19(3), 209224.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hudson, A. J. (2009). Consciousness: Physiological dependence on rapid memory access. Frontiers in Bioscience (Landmark Ed), 14, 27792800.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ikram, M. A., Brusselle, G., Ghanbari, M., Goedegebure, A., Ikram, M. K., Kavousi, M., … Voortman, T.. (2020). Objectives, design and main findings until 2020 from the Rotterdam Study. European Journal of Epidemiology, 35(5), 483517. doi: 10.1007/s10654-020-00640-5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kosmadopoulos, A., Sargent, C., Darwent, D., Zhou, X., & Roach, G. D. (2014). Alternatives to polysomnography (PSG): A validation of wrist actigraphy and a partial-PSG system. Behavior Research Methods, 46(4), 10321041. doi: 10.3758/s13428-013-0438-7CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leng, Y., Musiek, E. S., Hu, K., Cappuccio, F. P., & Yaffe, K. (2019). Association between circadian rhythms and neurodegenerative diseases. The Lancet Neurology, 18(3), 307318. doi: 0.1016/S1474-4422(18)30461-7CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lezak, M. D., Howieson, D. B., Loring, D. W., & Fischer, J. S. (2004). Neuropsychological assessment. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Liu, W., Ge, T., Leng, Y., Pan, Z., Fan, J., Yang, W., & Cui, R. (2017). The role of neural plasticity in depression: from hippocampus to prefrontal cortex. Neural Plasticity, 2017, 6871089.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liu, Y., Peng, T., Zhang, S., & Tang, K. (2018). The relationship between depression, daytime napping, daytime dysfunction, and snoring in 0.5 million Chinese populations: Exploring the effects of socio-economic status and age. BMC Public Health, 18(1), 759. doi: 10.1186/s12889-018-5629-9CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luik, A. I., Zuurbier, L. A., Direk, N., Hofman, A., Van Someren, E. J. W., & Tiemeier, H. (2015). 24-hour Activity rhythm and sleep disturbances in depression and anxiety: A population-based study of middle-aged and older persons. Depression and Anxiety, 32(9), 684692. doi: 10.1002/da.22355CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luik, A. I., Zuurbier, L. A., Hofman, A., Van Someren, E. J. W., & Tiemeier, H. (2013). Stability and fragmentation of the activity rhythm across the sleep-wake cycle: The importance of age, lifestyle, and mental health. Chronobiology International, 30(10), 12231230. doi: 10.3109/07420528.2013.813528CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maglione, J. E., Ancoli-Israel, S., Peters, K. W., Paudel, M. L., Yaffe, K., Ensrud, K. E., & Stone, K. L. (2014). Subjective and objective sleep disturbance and longitudinal risk of depression in a cohort of older women. Sleep, 37(7), 11791187. doi: 10.5665/sleep.3834CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marino, M., Li, Y., Rueschman, M. N., Winkelman, J. W., Ellenbogen, J. M., Solet, J. M., … Buxton, O. M. (2013). Measuring sleep: Accuracy, sensitivity, and specificity of wrist actigraphy compared to polysomnography. Sleep, 36(11), 17471755.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mitrushina, M., & Tomaszewski, R. (2019). The effect of subjective perception of cognition on emotional functioning in adults with long-term neurological and neuropsychiatric disorders. Disability and Rehabilitation, 42(22), 31253141. doi: 10.1080/09638288.2019.1585490Google ScholarPubMed
Mollayeva, T., Thurairajah, P., Burton, K., Mollayeva, S., Shapiro, C. M., & Colantonio, A. (2016). The Pittsburgh sleep quality index as a screening tool for sleep dysfunction in clinical and non-clinical samples: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Sleep Medicine Reviews, 25, 5273.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morgenthaler, T., Alessi, C., Friedman, L., Owens, J., Kapur, V., Boehlecke, B., … Lee-Chiong, T. (2007). Practice parameters for the use of actigraphy in the assessment of sleep and sleep disorders: An update for 2007. Sleep, 30(4), 519529.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Murphy, M. J., & Peterson, M. J. (2015). Sleep disturbances in depression. Sleep Medicine Clinics, 10(1), 1723. doi: 10.1016/j.jsmc.2014.11.009CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Murri, M. B., Pariante, C., Mondelli, V., Masotti, M., Atti, A. R., Mellacqua, Z., … Zanetidou, S. (2014). HPA axis and aging in depression: Systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 41, 4662. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2013.12.004CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Musiek, E. S., Bhimasani, M., Zangrilli, M. A., Morris, J. C., Holtzman, D. M., & Ju, Y.-E. S. (2018). Circadian rest-activity pattern changes in aging and preclinical Alzheimer disease. JAMA Neurology, 75(5), 582590.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Muzur, A., Pace-Schott, E. F., & Hobson, J. A. (2002). The prefrontal cortex in sleep. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6(11), 475481.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Olfson, M., Wall, M., Liu, S.-M., Morin, C. M., & Blanco, C. (2018). Insomnia and impaired quality of life in the United States. The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 79(5), 17m12020. doi: 10.4088/JCP.17m12020CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pandi-Perumal, S. R., Moscovitch, A., Srinivasan, V., Spence, D. W., Cardinali, D. P., & Brown, G. M. (2009). Bidirectional communication between sleep and circadian rhythms and its implications for depression: Lessons from agomelatine. Progress in Neurobiology, 88(4), 264271. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2009.04.007CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Radloff, L. S. (1977). The CES-D scale: A self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Applied Psychological Measurement, 1(3), 385401.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rubin, D. B. (2004). Multiple imputation for nonresponse in surveys (Vol. 81). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Smagula, S. F., Boudreau, R. M., Stone, K., Reynolds Iii, C. F., Bromberger, J. T., Ancoli-Israel, S., … Cauley, J. A. (2015). Latent activity rhythm disturbance sub-groups and longitudinal change in depression symptoms among older men. Chronobiology International, 32(10), 14271437. doi: 10.3109/07420528.2015.1102925CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smagula, S. F., Krafty, R. T., Taylor, B. J., Martire, L. M., Schulz, R., & Hall, M. H. (2017). Rest–activity rhythm and sleep characteristics associated with depression symptom severity in strained dementia caregivers. Journal of Sleep Research, 26(6), 718725. doi: 10.1111/jsr.12549CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sun, Y., Shi, L., Bao, Y., Sun, Y., Shi, J., & Lu, L. (2018). The bidirectional relationship between sleep duration and depression in community-dwelling middle-aged and elderly individuals: Evidence from a longitudinal study. Sleep Medicine, 52, 221229. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2018.03.011CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swaab, D. F., Fliers, E., & Partiman, T. S. (1985). The suprachiasmatic nucleus of the human brain in relation to sex, age and senile dementia. Brain Research, 342(1), 3744. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(85)91350-2CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
te Lindert, B. H. W., & Van Someren, E. J. W. (2013). Sleep estimates using microelectromechanical systems (MEMS). Sleep, 36(5), 781789. doi: 10.5665/sleep.2648CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tiffin, J., & Asher, E. J. (1948). The Purdue Pegboard: Norms and studies of reliability and validity. Journal of Applied Psychology, 32(3), 234.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tombaugh, T. N., & McIntyre, N. J. (1992). The mini-mental state examination: A comprehensive review. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 40(9), 922935. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.1992.tb01992.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vadnie, C. A., & McClung, C. A. (2017). Circadian rhythm disturbances in mood disorders: Insights into the role of the suprachiasmatic nucleus. Neural Plasticity, 2017, 1504507. doi: 10.1155/2017/1504507CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van Den Berg, J. F., Van Rooij, F. J. A., Vos, H., Tulen, J. H. M., Hofman, A., Miedema, H. M., … Tiemeier, H. (2008). Disagreement between subjective and actigraphic measures of sleep duration in a population-based study of elderly persons. Journal of Sleep Research, 17(3), 295302. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2869.2008.00638.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vandeputte, M., & de Weerd, A. (2003). Sleep disorders and depressive feelings: A global survey with the beck depression scale. Sleep Medicine, 4(4), 343345. doi: 10.1016/s1389-9457(03)00059-5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van Laethem, M., Beckers, D. G. J., van Hooff, M. L. M., Dijksterhuis, A., & Geurts, S. A. E. (2016). Day-to-day relations between stress and sleep and the mediating role of perseverative cognition. Sleep Medicine, 24, 7179. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2016.06.020CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van Someren, E. J. W. (1997). Actigraphic monitoring of movement and rest-activity rhythms in aging, Alzheimer's disease, and Parkinson's disease. IEEE Transactions on Rehabilitation Engineering, 5(4), 394398. doi: 10.1109/86.650297CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wang, Y.-Q., Li, R., Zhang, M.-Q., Zhang, Z., Qu, W.-M., & Huang, Z.-L. (2015). The neurobiological mechanisms and treatments of REM sleep disturbances in depression. Current Neuropharmacology, 13(4), 543553. doi: 10.2174/1570159x13666150310002540CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Welsh, K. A., Butters, N., Mohs, R. C., Beekly, D., Edland, S., Fillenbaum, G., & Heyman, A. (1994). The consortium to establish a registry for Alzheimer's disease (CERAD). Part V. A normative study of the neuropsychological battery. Neurology, 44(4), 609609.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yon, A., Scogin, F., DiNapoli, E. A., McPherron, J., Arean, P. A., Bowman, D., … Reynolds Iii, C. F. (2014). Do manualized treatments for depression reduce insomnia symptoms? Journal of Clinical Psychology, 70(7), 616630.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zhai, L., Zhang, H., & Zhang, D. (2015). Sleep duration and depression among adults: A meta-analysis of prospective studies. Depression and Anxiety, 32(9), 664670. doi: 10.1002/da.22386CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Baseline characteristics of the study population at baseline (December 2004–April 2007), Rotterdam Study

Figure 1

Table 2. Cross-sectional associations of 24-h activity rhythms and sleep with depressive symptoms at baseline

Figure 2

Table 3. Longitudinal association of 24-h activity rhythms and sleep with depressive symptoms over time

Figure 3

Table 4. Longitudinal association of depressive symptoms with 24-h activity rhythms and sleep over time

Supplementary material: File

de Feijter et al. supplementary material

de Feijter et al. supplementary material 1

Download de Feijter et al. supplementary material(File)
File 500.5 KB
Supplementary material: File

de Feijter et al. supplementary material

de Feijter et al. supplementary material 2

Download de Feijter et al. supplementary material(File)
File 481.7 KB
Supplementary material: File

de Feijter et al. supplementary material

de Feijter et al. supplementary material 3

Download de Feijter et al. supplementary material(File)
File 24.5 KB