Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-jkksz Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-24T02:43:33.893Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

South African youth’s understanding of feelings, thoughts and behaviours: Implications for the development of CBT-based mental health interventions

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  07 April 2022

Suzanne Human*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Stellenbosch University, Stellenbosch, South Africa
Hermine Gericke
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Stellenbosch University, Stellenbosch, South Africa
Maria Loades
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Bath, Bath, UK
Paul Stallard
Affiliation:
Department of Health, University of Bath, Bath, UK
Bronwynè J. Coetzee
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Stellenbosch University, Stellenbosch, South Africa
*
*Corresponding author. Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background:

Mental health problems are common in youth in low- and middle-income countries (LMICs), including South Africa. Preventative interventions, based on cognitive behaviour therapy (CBT), delivered in schools, have been found to alleviate symptoms of depression and anxiety in high income countries (HICs). However, less is known about whether youth in LMICs are able to engage with the core concepts of CBT.

Aims:

To explore how young people in the Western Cape, South Africa, understand key CBT concepts, such as feelings, thoughts and behaviours.

Method:

We interviewed 22 young people (10–15 years of age; mean age 11.6 years; SD = 1.0) recruited from two public primary schools in the Western Cape, South Africa. Interviews were audio-recorded, transcribed verbatim, translated from Afrikaans into English where necessary and analysed thematically using a deductive approach.

Results:

Young people described feelings as internal, private, and should only be shared with trusted others. They also described how feelings varied, depending on the situation. They found the concept of thoughts more challenging to describe. Youth were able to say what they do when they experience unpleasant feelings, and they linked their behaviours to their feelings and thoughts.

Conclusions:

In this cultural context, our qualitative investigation found evidence that young people were able to engage with abstract concepts including feelings and to some degree, thoughts. To ensure that CBT-based interventions are developmentally appropriate and accessible, psychoeducation may help youth distinguish between thoughts, feelings and behaviours, and a focus on identifying and naming feelings may be beneficial.

Type
Main
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence ((https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/)), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2022. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of the British Association for Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapies

Introduction

Worldwide, around one in eight children and adolescents experience mental health problems (Polanczyk et al., Reference Polanczyk, Salum, Sugaya, Caye and Rohde2015). In low- and middle-income countries (LMICs), such as South Africa, where children are exposed to multiple risk factors such as violence, child maltreatment, living in households affected by HIV/AIDS and poverty, the estimated prevalence of mental health problems is even higher (Flisher et al., Reference Flisher, Dawes, Kafaar, Lund, Sorsdahl, Myers, Thom and Seedat2012; Kieling et al., Reference Kieling, Baker-Henningham, Belfer, Conti, Ertem, Omigbodun, Rohde, Srinath, Ulkuer and Rahman2011; Lu et al., Reference Lu, Li and Patel2018; Patel et al., Reference Patel, Flisher, Hetrick and McGorry2007). In South Africa, estimated prevalence rates of anxiety or depression in children (age 10–12) are one in eight (Cortina et al., Reference Cortina, Fazel, Hlungwani, Kahn, Tollman, Cortina-Borja and Stein2013) and as many as four in ten 14- and 15-year-olds may have depression (Das-Munshi et al., Reference Das-Munshi, Lund, Mathews, Clark, Rothon and Stansfeld2016). Mental health problems interfere with children’s daily functioning, including education, social relationships and developmental progress (Belfer, Reference Belfer2008; Dalsgaard et al., Reference Dalsgaard, McGrath, Østergaard, Wray, Pedersen, Mortensen and Petersen2020; Finning et al., Reference Finning, Waite, Harvey, Moore, Davis and Ford2020; Giel et al., Reference Giel, De Arango, Climent, Harding, Ibrahim, Ladrido-Ignacio, Murthy, Salazar, Wig and Younis1981; Patel et al., Reference Patel, Flisher, Hetrick and McGorry2007; Rescorla et al., Reference Rescorla, Achenbach, Ivanova, Dumenci, Almqvist, Bilenberg, Bird, Chen, Dobrean, Döpfner, Erol, Fombonne, Fonseca, Frigerio, Grietens, Hannesdottir, Kanbayashi, Lambert, Larsson and Verhulst2007; Wickersham et al., Reference Wickersham, Dickson, Jones, Pritchard, Stewart, Ford and Downs2020). Long-term, if untreated, mental health problems tend to persist (American Psychiatric Association, 2013; Fombonne et al., Reference Fombonne, Simmons, Ford, Meltzer and Goodman2001).

There is good evidence from high-income countries (HICs) that interventions based on cognitive behaviour therapy (CBT) are effective in treating anxiety and depression in children (Creswell et al., Reference Creswell, Waite and Cooper2014; Klein et al., Reference Klein, Jacobs and Reinecke2007; Reynolds et al., Reference Reynolds, Wilson, Austin and Hooper2012; Weisz et al., Reference Weisz, McCarty and Valeri2006). Similarly, a review of 45 indicated school-based interventions, predominantly conducted in HICs, showed that school-based interventions are also effective at reducing symptoms of depression and anxiety amongst children (Gee et al., Reference Gee, Reynolds, Carroll, Orchard, Clarke, Martin, Wilson and Pass2020). There is some evidence of similar effectiveness of these interventions in LMICs, although data are limited (Yatham et al., Reference Yatham, Sivathasan, Yoon, da Silva and Ravindran2018). While these outcomes are promising, early intervention is needed to reduce the severity and/or the persistence of primary mental health disorders, as well as to prevent the onset of secondary disorders (De Girolamo et al., Reference De Girolamo, Dagani, Purcell, Cocchi and McGorry2012). This is especially important in LMICs where the mental health needs of children are neglected (Kieling et al., Reference Kieling, Baker-Henningham, Belfer, Conti, Ertem, Omigbodun, Rohde, Srinath, Ulkuer and Rahman2011), due to the scarcity of resources, and a lack of evidence-based interventions (Belfer, Reference Belfer2008; Patel et al., Reference Patel, Flisher, Nikapota and Malhotra2008).

Universal interventions are provided to a whole population group, rather than to specific individuals with symptoms (Mrazek and Haggerty, Reference Mrazek and Haggerty1994), with the aim of preventing mental health problems or addressing mental health problems early on. In the UK, universal, school-based interventions, aimed at promoting emotional or mental well-being or preventing mental health difficulties, delivered to youth aged 5–16 years, have shown neutral to small effects (Mackenzie and Williams, Reference Mackenzie and Williams2018). A systematic review of 118 school-based depression and/or anxiety prevention interventions (mostly conducted in HICs) found that compared with universal interventions for depression, targeted interventions (delivered to young people at risk of developing depression, or with symptoms) resulted in significantly greater effect sizes (Werner-Seidler et al., Reference Werner-Seidler, Spanos, Calear, Perry, Torok, O’Dea, Christensen and Newby2021). In LMICs, universal school-based mental health interventions have shown promising outcomes for anxiety and depression, although it has to be noted that the evidence is limited, and few studies have detailed how the interventions were developed or adapted for the local context (Bradshaw et al., Reference Bradshaw, Gericke, Coetzee, Stallard, Human and Loades2021).

More high-quality, large-scale studies are needed to explore any possible long-term benefits of universal, school-based mental health interventions (Mackenzie and Williams, Reference Mackenzie and Williams2018). Even though the effects of school-based depression and/or anxiety universal or targeted interventions seem to be small, such early, preventative mental health interventions could still have a substantial impact on the greater public’s mental health when delivered at scale (Werner-Seidler et al., Reference Werner-Seidler, Spanos, Calear, Perry, Torok, O’Dea, Christensen and Newby2021). The impact of school-based early interventions (whether universal or targeted) could be enhanced by continuing to improve and refine such interventions and by embedding interventions in a sustainable way within schools (Werner-Seidler et al., Reference Werner-Seidler, Spanos, Calear, Perry, Torok, O’Dea, Christensen and Newby2021). Indeed, the practical constraints that effect implementation need to be considered. Schools may find it easier to timetable universal interventions for whole classes rather than arrange for targeted interventions involving a few students from multiple classes. To date, no studies have examined the effectiveness of school-based early interventions (whether universal or targeted) in South Africa. In this context, it could be argued that the focus should be on low-cost, universally available, accessible resources, which are developmentally appropriate and empower children and young people (Flisher et al., Reference Flisher, Dawes, Kafaar, Lund, Sorsdahl, Myers, Thom and Seedat2012; Patel et al., Reference Patel, Flisher, Nikapota and Malhotra2008).

Irrespective of whether the aim is to develop a CBT-based universal intervention or a CBT-based targeted intervention, it is important to know what youth’s understanding of key CBT-based concepts are, so that young people can be socialized to the CBT model (Mahoney-Davies et al., Reference Mahoney-Davies, Roberts-Collins, Russell and Loades2017). For children to engage in and benefit from CBT-based interventions they have to be able to identify and monitor feelings, thoughts and behaviours; challenge and test thoughts; develop alternative ways of coping with unpleasant feelings; and learn new behaviours (Albano and Kendall, Reference Albano and Kendall2002; Reynolds et al., Reference Reynolds, Girling, Coker and Eastwood2006). Given the early age of onset of many mental health problems (Kessler et al., Reference Kessler, Angermeyer, Anthony, De Graaf, Demyttenaere, Gasquet, De Girolamo, Gluzman, Gureje, Haro, Kawakami, Karam, Levinson, Medina Mora, Oakley Browne, Posada-Villa, Stein, Adley Tsang, Aguilar-Gaxiola, Alonso and Ustün2007), the primary school years could be an opportune time for the implementation of preventive interventions like universal school-based approaches (Yap and Jorm, Reference Yap and Jorm2015). Children aged 7–11 years fall into the concrete operational stage of cognitive development (Louw & Louw, Reference Louw and Louw2014; Piaget, Reference Piaget1972). While children could possibly begin to think more systematically and logically during this stage, this is not a given, and their ability to think abstractly and hypothetically could still be rather limited. At around 11–12 years of age, children typically enter the formal operational stage of cognitive development during which time they could begin to develop the capacity for more abstract thinking (Louw and Louw, Reference Louw and Louw2014; Piaget, Reference Piaget1972). Again, this development from concrete to more abstract thinking is not necessarily an inevitable process of childhood development. As such, this raises the question of the extent to which young people in the primary school years can engage with abstract concepts and CBT-based interventions, especially in a LMIC such as South Africa where exposure to risk factors could have an influence on children’s development.

Research conducted in HICs showed that compared with 7- and 8-year-olds, 10- and 11-year-olds performed better when asked to discriminate between thoughts and behaviours (Quakley et al., Reference Quakley, Coker, Palmer and Reynolds2003). Results of another HIC study showed that children as young as 6 or 7 were able to discriminate amongst thoughts, feelings and behaviours, although those ‘at risk’ of mental health problems performed significantly poorer (Reynolds et al., Reference Reynolds, Girling, Coker and Eastwood2006). Research has shown that most 8- to 12-year-olds can identify emotional difficulties displayed by hypothetical peers in vignettes (Georgakakou-Koutsonikou et al., Reference Georgakakou-Koutsonikou, Taylor and Williams2019). Simple, concrete cues, including stories, have also been shown to improve children’s (aged 4–7 years) performance in terms of discriminating amongst thoughts, feelings and behaviours (Quakley et al., Reference Quakley, Reynolds and Coker2004). Concrete methods of introducing abstract concepts can be incorporated in CBT-based interventions to make these more accessible to children (Quakley et al., Reference Quakley, Reynolds and Coker2004; Reynolds et al., Reference Reynolds, Girling, Coker and Eastwood2006).

Little is known about the extent to which South African primary school-aged youth, who are in a context of heightened risk for mental health problems, can engage with the core principles of CBT. One South African study showed that children (aged 7–10; n = 42) could name different feelings (Human, Reference Human2018; Loxton and Human, Reference Loxton and Human2017), while another study showed that some children (aged 10–13; n = 52) had difficulty distinguishing between thoughts and feelings (Loxton and Webber, Reference Loxton and Webber2015; Webber, Reference Webber2016). While South African children (age 9–13; n = 16) with visual impairments could differentiate between feelings and behaviours, they struggled to identify thoughts (Visagie et al., Reference Visagie, Loxton, Stallard and Silverman2017). To know whether universal CBT-based interventions in primary schools in South Africa may be appropriate, and to inform adaptations to interventions, we need to know more about how young people in the general population in South Africa understand feelings, thoughts and behaviours, and whether they are able to discriminate among these. Therefore, in this qualitative study, we aimed to explore how youth in this context understand key CBT concepts in order to inform the development of a CBT-based universal mental health programme.

Method

Research design

Findings reported in this paper form part of a larger project aimed at developing and assessing the acceptability and feasibility of a CBT-based psychoeducational intervention to support the psychological well-being of young people in primary schools in the Western Cape, South Africa (trial registration number PACTR202004803366609). The first phase of this project was a qualitative study with stakeholders to clarify whether young people were able to understand the core concepts of CBT. Youth were asked what ‘feelings’ and ‘thoughts’ meant to them; what makes them feel happy, sad, angry, and scared/anxious/nervous; what thoughts they have when they experience these feelings; and what they do when they have these feelings.

Research setting

This qualitative study took place at two public (government-funded) primary schools situated in two urban areas in the Western Cape province of South Africa, and in collaboration with a local non-government organisation (NGO), which operates within schools in the Western Cape to improve the social and emotional well-being of children and promote supportive school communities. The two schools were randomly chosen (using computer randomisation) from a list of schools (n = 21) within which the NGO operates. Both schools have a staff-to-learner ratio of approximately 40 learners to every 1 staff member. Both schools are part of South Africa’s National School Nutrition Programme, which was introduced in 1994 (Devereux et al., Reference Devereux, Hochfeld, Karriem, Mensah, Morahanye, Msimango, Mukubonda, Naicker, Nkomo, Sanders and Sanousi2018). Public schools are eligible for funding for this feeding programme when most of the learners come from low socio-economic status families. For more socio-demographic details about the two areas in which the two schools are situated, please refer to Table 1.

Table 1. Demographic information (2011 national census) on the areas where the two schools are situated, compared with provincial (Western Cape) and national averages

* These classifications for ethnicity are the classifications as used in the most recent South African census of 2011.

As can be seen from Table 1, the vast majority (94.4 and 91.3%) of individuals living in the two areas where the schools are located are Afrikaans speaking. Almost a quarter (24.1 and 21.7%) of the population in the two areas are young people (aged 0–14 years). The majority of individuals live in formal dwellings, with access to electricity and flush toilets.

Participants and recruitment

We invited all young people in grades 5–7 (approximately 10–14 years of age), attending the two schools, to participate in our study. With the respective schools’ permission, invitation letters were handed out to approximately 680 young people during regular school hours. The letters provided youth with information about the study and invited them to take part in one-off individual in-person interviews. Young people who wanted to take part were asked to inform their educator or the NGO office manager at their school. All interested youth were then provided with a parental/caregiver consent form and asked to return the signed consent form to their educator or the NGO office manager at their school.

Procedures

Young people were interviewed in 2019 during regular school hours at an NGO counselling room or an empty classroom at school. Written informed parental/caregiver consent as well as written informed child assent were obtained for all participants. Interviews were conducted in Afrikaans (n = 20) or English (n = 2), depending on the individual participant’s preferred language. The average length of an interview was 36.6 minutes (ranging from 21.2 to 59.0 minutes). Young people were interviewed by trained psychology graduates (S.H. and H.G.). Young people could choose whether to be interviewed in Afrikaans and/or English. Afrikaans and English are the two predominant languages spoken in the two areas and the two schools (see Table 1). Afrikaans is the first language of the two researchers (S.H. and H.G.) who conducted the interviews. Both interviewers are also fluent in English. This reduced the likelihood of translational issues influencing participants’ responses. In cases where young people expressed information of a sensitive and/or traumatic nature, the interviewers responded in line with the policies and procedures of the collaborating NGO. All such incidents were also reported to, and reviewed by, the principal investigator (B.C.) of the larger project and the REC: Human Research (Humanities) who approved the study. Where necessary, young people were referred to the NGO, who agreed to offer psycho-social support to participants, if needed, and free of charge.

Materials

Demographic questionnaire

We asked participants to provide us with information on their gender, age and first language, as well as their current grade (class) at school.

Semi-structured interview schedule

We used a semi-structured interview schedule to guide the face-to face interviews. Interviews began with a general greeting and some ice-breaker questions. We asked young people what the words ‘feelings’ and ‘thoughts’ meant to them, if they could name some feelings and how they would explain ‘feelings’ and ‘thoughts’ to a friend. Following these questions, young people were presented with two short stories that they could either read out loud themselves or have read to them. Previous research has shown that stories are a child-friendly and non-threatening way of talking to young people about abstract concepts such as feelings, thoughts and behaviours (Human, Reference Human2018; Loxton and Human, Reference Loxton and Human2017; Quakley, Reference Quakley2001; Quakley et al., Reference Quakley, Coker, Palmer and Reynolds2003; Quakley et al., Reference Quakley, Reynolds and Coker2004). Each short story contained a description of an event, what a character thought in relation to the event and how the character behaved in relation to the event. Youth were asked questions about the character’s feelings, thoughts and behaviours. Following this, youth were asked what made them feel happy, sad, angry and scared (or anxious or nervous), what thoughts they might have with each feeling, and what they do when they experience these feelings.

Data analysis

Transcripts were uploaded to ATLAS.ti v8, qualitative software for thematic analysis. We analysed the data thematically according to the six steps as set out by Braun and Clarke (Reference Braun and Clarke2006). The data were coded deductively by S.H., who met regularly with H.G. and B.C. to check the consistency and accuracy of the codes developed. In line with deductive thematic analysis, we coded our data with a specific research question in mind (Braun and Clarke, Reference Braun and Clarke2006), in this case to explore young people’s understanding of key CBT concepts, such as defining feelings and thoughts, how feelings and thoughts are linked, and how feelings and thoughts relate to behaviours. Furthermore, in line with deductive thematic analysis, we grouped our codes together to form themes, with the themes identified matching the specific interview questions that were asked (Nowell et al., Reference Nowell, Norris, White and Moules2017). While data were coded with a specific research question in mind, namely, to explore young people’s understanding of key CBT concepts, the entire data set was coded so as to consider all possible responses from the participating young people. Themes were reviewed and refined by all authors. Finally, all authors were involved in producing, reviewing and revising the final report.

Results

We interviewed 22 young people, with a mean age of 11.6 years (ranging from 10 to 15 years; SD = 1.0); see Table 2. As this is a qualitative study, we anticipated that 20 interviews would be sufficient to meet the aim of our research.

Table 2. Demographics of participants (n=22)

In response to what we asked the youth, two main themes were identified, namely, ‘understanding feelings’ and ‘understanding thoughts’. Each has a number of sub-themes, which will be described with illustrative quotes (see Table 3). Pseudonyms have been used throughout.

Table 3. Themes and sub-themes identified following deductive thematic analysis

Understanding feelings

Relating to what we wanted to find out, this theme consists of the following three sub-themes: ‘Do young people understand what feelings are?’; ‘Do young people understand that feelings are related to events?’ and ‘Do young people understand that feelings effect behaviours?’.

Do young people understand what feelings are?

Thirteen young people (59%) were able to identity and define their feelings. Seven young people (32%) defined feelings as ‘how you feel’, ‘what you feel’, ‘how you feel about something’, ‘how you feel inside about someone’ or ‘what you feel on the inside’. Robin, an 11-year-old girl, said feelings are ‘how you feel about yourself’. Olivia, a 12-year-old girl, said feelings are ‘what’s going on inside you’ and Sophia, a 12-year-old girl, said ‘it’s what you feel inside but you can’t describe it’. Emma, an 11-year-old girl, described what happens in her body when she feels nervous: ‘when I feel so nervous and tremble with my reading’. Anne, a 12-year-old girl, explained ‘people that hurt your feelings’ and ‘hurt you with words’. Joy, an 11-year-old girl, said ‘if that person has feelings you musn’t make jokes or make fun of them’.

Do young people understand that feelings are related to events?

When asked what makes them feel happy, sad, angry or scared (anxious/nervous) all of the young people were able to relate feelings to specific situations and events. Examples that young people gave included feeling sad about the death of a family member, feeling sad or angry in response to the behaviours of others directed towards them, such as when others are rude to them, or when someone bullies, mocks or calls them names. Young people also said that other people’s behaviours make them feel sad or angry, even when those behaviours are not directed towards them, such as when older children bully younger children.

Interviewer: what makes you feel sad?

Emma, an 11-year-old girl: that is when someone dies, my family or anyone

Interviewer: what makes you feel sad?

Joy, an 11-year-old girl: it’s when people are rude to me, when they say nasty stuff about me, and, and if children gossip about me ma’am, if my mother scolds me sometimes then I feel sad

Interviewer: what makes you feel sad?

Jessica, an 11-year-old girl: about my mom and my aunt and sister fight every day

In response to the question about what makes them feel scared, anxious or nervous, some young people used words such as ‘scared’, ‘nervous’, ‘anxious’, ‘worried’ or ‘shy’ interchangeably to describe the feeling that they have in response to certain situations. The situations, that young people referred to, when using these words interchangeably, included going to a party where you do not know all the people, not knowing what to talk about when someone else starts talking to you, having to do an oral (a speech) or having to read in front of their class, talking in front of a group of people, or being on stage in front of people.

is like oral I did my oral yesterday, but I didn’t feel nervous because I had cue cards with me, but without cue cards it makes me feel nervous and when I have to read then I also get a little bit nervous (Emma, an 11-year-old girl)

Contrary to the above, when youth used the word ‘scared’ to describe how they feel, without using words such as nervous, anxious, worried or shy interchangeably, they spoke about being scared that something bad might happen, such as there being something in the dark, or someone breaking into the house when you are home alone. Another young person recalled a traumatic incident from her past that made her feel scared, namely, feeling scared when a drunk family member called her to sit on his lap and then touched her in a way that made her feel uncomfortable. Yet another young person recalled traumatic incidents from her past that made her feel scared (having been sexually abused, and being hit by a car), and went on to explain that certain occurrences in the present (such as seeing men, and crossing a street by herself), make her feel scared, as these present occurrences make her think back to the past incidents.

like when my mom and dad are away and my brother they went somewhere I’m alone at home then uhm, I’m scared that something happens to me, that someone would break in or something, into the house (Sophia, a 12-year-old girl)

Do young people understand that feelings effect behaviours?

When asked what they do when they feel happy, sad, angry or scared (anxious/nervous) 20 young people (91%) were able to explain what they do in response to these feelings. Examples included crying when feeling sad, angry or scared, and preferring to be by themselves, as indicated by behaviours such as sitting somewhere on your own or shutting your eyes and pulling the blankets over your head.

I, sit on the ground and I cry and I don’t want anyone to be with me when I cry (Amy, a 13-year-old-girl)

Contrary to those who prefer to be by themselves when they experience unpleasant feelings, other young people said they deal with unpleasant feelings by speaking to someone who can help, such as their mother, grandmother, a caregiver or a family member, the school principal, or a teacher. Youth emphasized that one should talk to someone you trust, someone who will know what to do, and that they would encourage their friends to do the same.

I would encourage them [referring to her friends] if they want to talk about their feelings, then they have to talk to someone that they trust, or someone who they know, that uhm that one won’t tell anyone about how they feel, or they will have to talk to someone who is going to know what to do (Olivia, a 12-year-old girl)

Some young people mentioned that they pray when they experience unpleasant feelings. Youth also said they try to keep themselves calm by distracting themselves from thinking about what made them feel sad, angry or scared. They said they do this by keeping themselves busy, through writing, playing games with cousins, watching television, listening to music, or playing on their cell phone.

I like just rebuke the spirit of angriness out, I pray (Jamie, an 11-year-old girl)

I would sit in my room and listen to music or just do something to keep my thoughts off from it to feel better (Demi, a 12-year-old girl)

Two of the young people who said they are scared of the dark, said that they switch on a light when they feel scared, while other children said they would confront that which they are scared of doing by, for example, getting up to do the talk, continuing to read in front of the class, or being the first child to stand up to receive a certificate. Youth also said that how you feel has an influence on how you behave towards others. A number of young people said that they ignore the person who made them feel sad or angry, that they walk or run away, and that they do not confront the other person. Others spoke about confronting the person who made them feel sad, angry or scared, such as by saying something to the person or hitting the person.

Interviewer: what do you do when you feel angry?

Susan, an 11-year-old girl: then I fight.

Interviewer: okay, how do you fight?

Susan: I hit the children with the fist.

Understanding thoughts

Relating to what we wanted to find out, this theme consists of the following three sub-themes: ‘Do young people understand what thoughts are?’; ‘Do young people understand that thoughts are related to events?’ and ‘Do young people understand that thoughts effect feelings and behaviours?’.

Do young people understand what thoughts are?

When asked the open-ended question, ‘What does the word “thoughts” mean to you?’, 16 young people (73%) were able to respond and offer a definition. Ten children (45.5%) defined thoughts as ‘what you think’, ‘what you think about’, ‘things that you think’, ‘when you think something’ or ‘when you can think things’. Samantha, an 11-year-old girl, said thoughts are ‘something that you have in mind’, while Tommy, an 11-year-old boy, pointed to his head as a way of explaining what the word thoughts meant to him. Thoughts were also understood as referring to what you think of yourself or what others think of you.

Interviewer: and what does the word thoughts mean to you?

Emma, an 11-year-old girl: it’s what people think of you or what you think of yourself

Some youth said that thoughts refer to memories of the past, and that a person can have good or sad memories. Others explained that you can have thoughts about the future, for example, thoughts about an upcoming school concert or about playing chess during the next break time at school, or thoughts about the fun things you will be doing once the school day is over.

thoughts for me means, thoughts for me is like what I did for the day and everything that I did, memory, so those are my thoughts and everything I have done with the people who have died and those are all my thoughts and my good things and my sad things (Lily, an 11-year-old girl)

it’s when you think about what you’re like going to do after school or something (Sophia, a 12-year-old girl)

Five young people (23%) were unable to define or explain thoughts. Anne, for example, a 12-year-old girl, spoke about behaviours instead of her thoughts.

Do young people understand that thoughts are related to events?

When asked what thoughts they have when they feel sad, angry or scared (anxious/nervous), eight young people (36%) mentioned behaviours instead of thoughts. Examples included going to grandma, holding onto a friend’s hand, confronting the person who made you angry, not talking to anyone, screaming, or crying.

Interviewer: and when you feel scared? what do you think then?

Jessica, an 11-year-old girl: then I go to the toilet and then I go cry I, and then I come play further

The remaining 14 young people (64%) were able to identify thoughts associated with feeling sad, angry or scared (anxious/nervous). Examples included expecting something bad to happen, such as children laughing at you, or thinking that you will never play with or talk to a certain person again.

my thoughts are like when I make a mistake children laugh and, and, and if I do something wrong they laugh (Joy, an 11-year-old girl)

One of the girls, who recalled a past traumatic experience, said that at the time of the incident, she did not have any thoughts: ‘when it happened I, thought, thought of nothing’. Others mentioned thoughts that they have about the behaviours of those people who make them feel sad, angry or scared, for example wondering why others do what they do or whether the bullies will cause pain to the other children.

I thought my grandpa is very rude to my mom (Kevin, a 12-year-old boy)

Do young people understand that thoughts effect feelings and behaviours?

Four young people (18%) expressed a link between thoughts and/or feelings and/or behaviours. Demi, a 12-year-old girl said:

thoughts are the things that we think about but not necessarily say, like if teacher maybe asks a question in class and you think you have the answer but you don’t want to say because you’re scared it’s wrong and then you keep quiet.

Joy, an 11-year-old girl, said: ‘you need to think before you do something and, and you need to think before you say something to someone else that’s rude’. When asked what she does when she feels sad, Jamie, an 11-year-old girl, said: ‘I’d rather just keep my mind off it keep me busy’. Lucy, an 11-year-old girl, who said that she feels nervous when she has to do a performance in front of a crowd of people, identified helpful thoughts: ‘I can do it I have to face my fear’. Lucy went on to explain:

Lucy: and then I do it, then I do that that I, that I feared and then afterwards.

Interviewer: how do you then feel afterwards?

Lucy: I feel proud of myself that I did it.

Discussion

Based on our qualitative findings, almost two-thirds of the youth that we interviewed understood the concept of feelings. All young people were able to speak about feelings more easily in context, when asked to describe situations or events that made them feel happy, sad, angry or scared (anxious/nervous). In terms of the concept of thoughts, three-quarters of youth were able to offer an understanding and highlighted how thoughts could be about themselves and what they do as well as how others perceived them. Youth understood that thoughts can relate to past and/or future events, and some identified that thoughts could be helpful or unhelpful. Two-thirds of young people were able to relate thoughts to their feelings, whilst the remaining third confused feelings and behaviours. In terms of behaviours, nearly all young people were able to explain what they do in response to feeling happy, sad, angry or scared (anxious/nervous). In terms of the interconnection, only one in five young people were able to express a link between feelings and/or thoughts and/or behaviours. Cognitive and emotional literacy is not a pre-requirement for CBT as this will be developed during the intervention. However, these results are encouraging and suggest that young people in these South African schools have sufficient cognitive and emotional development to engage in a CBT-based programme.

Young people were able to demonstrate that feelings vary and that different feelings are associated with different events. The situations that they referred to varied greatly, from feeling sad or angry when someone insulted or teased them, feeling nervous when having to do an oral presentation, or feeling scared when recalling past traumatic incidents such as being sexually abused or hit by a car. In terms of their emotional vocabulary, feeling ‘scared’ was used in relation to a range of events from talking in public through to past traumatic incidents. This suggests that the use of emotional language during school-based interventions needs to be carefully considered. Programme facilitators need to plan how disclosures will be dealt with in a caring and sensitive way, should disclosures of a traumatic nature happen in the context of a classroom.

At around 11–12 years of age, young people usually begin to develop the capacity for more abstract thinking (Louw and Louw, Reference Louw and Louw2014; Piaget, Reference Piaget1972), and as such it was expected that the young people we interviewed (with a mean age of 11.6 years) would have a reasonable understanding of the concept of thoughts. Our results are consistent with this, with three-quarters able to provide an understanding of thoughts. Similarly, emotional literacy was reasonably well developed amongst all young people, particularly when feelings were discussed within a context. However, in terms of developing and/or adapting school-based CBT interventions for this South African context, it will be important to build some preparatory work into such interventions. Particular difficulties we identified were the ability to differentiate amongst thoughts, feelings and behaviours, and to make connections between them. Whilst it was also encouraging to note that some young people mentioned helpful or unhelpful thoughts, it remains unclear whether they have the skills to test those thoughts and develop alternative thoughts, which is an important aspect of CBT (Albano and Kendall, Reference Albano and Kendall2002; Reynolds et al., Reference Reynolds, Girling, Coker and Eastwood2006; Stallard, Reference Stallard2019).

Strengths and limitations

A strength of this study is that we interviewed a range of young people from two public primary schools to inform the development and implementation of a CBT-based mental health early intervention in South African schools. The study did, however, have a number of limitations as well. Our sample is small, and it could be that the young people who volunteered to participate may not be representative of the wider group. As we did not screen participating youth for symptoms of anxiety and/or depression, we do not know if youth with existing mental health difficulties took part in the study or not, or whether the presence or absence of symptoms such as depression and/or anxiety might have impacted on their engagement with and/or understanding of CBT concepts. We did not screen participating youth for past experiences of trauma, and as such we also do not know how many of the participating young people experienced traumatic incidents in the past and if those experiences influenced their responses or not. Furthermore, we do not know whether any of the participating young people received any mental health support in the past, which may have helped them to become familiar with concepts such as feelings, thoughts and behaviours. Finally, our age range was wide (10–15 years) and there may be some important developmental differences between the younger and older youth which we did not detect.

Clinical and research implications

Our findings demonstrate that the sample of South African young people (aged 10–15 years), who took part in this study, are able to engage with certain key concepts of CBT. Their understanding and engagement can be enhanced through the use of concrete methods, including stories, and specific contextual questions which help to make abstract concepts accessible (Reynolds et al., Reference Reynolds, Girling, Coker and Eastwood2006). The importance of psychoeducation to increase young people’s mental health literacy and understanding of mental health difficulties has been highlighted (Georgakakou-Koutsonikou et al., Reference Georgakakou-Koutsonikou, Taylor and Williams2019; Georgakakou-Koutsonikou and Williams, Reference Georgakakou-Koutsonikou and Williams2017). In addition, our findings indicate that some initial psycho-educational work needs to be built into any school-based group programmes provided in this context to help youth understand the core concepts (such as thoughts, feelings and behaviours) that underpin the intervention. This appears particularly important in South Africa, where the education system involves mixed classes of children of different ages, development and ability. Once this shared understanding has been obtained, the relationship between the core parts of the CBT model can be strengthened and children introduced to the idea of helpful and unhelpful thoughts, an idea that was already familiar to some of the children we interviewed. In addition to building psycho-educational aspects into CBT-based universal or targeted interventions, it could also be important to develop and/or amend the content of such interventions so that it is suitable to young people, for example to focus on helping young people to understand and name feelings, and to focus on what they can do to make themselves feel better.

Future research needs to evaluate the content of early interventions and whether ideas from higher income countries are relevant within a South African context. How interventions are designed to fit within the school timetable needs to be considered too. In our participating schools, lessons were scheduled for 20–25 minutes and whether material can be covered in sufficient depth will need to be established. Similarly, resources in many South African schools are very limited with limited access to audio visual materials, the internet, and/or workbooks. This poses a challenge to ensure that interventions can be delivered in an engaging and understandable way. Finally, the efficacy of school-based mental health early interventions in South Africa is unknown and needs to be assessed.

Conclusions

There is increased focus on the importance of universal mental health interventions for primary school-aged children (Yap and Jorm, Reference Yap and Jorm2015). There is a need for school-based early interventions to be improved and refined, and for working towards embedding such interventions in a sustainable way within schools (Werner-Seidler et al., Reference Werner-Seidler, Spanos, Calear, Perry, Torok, O’Dea, Christensen and Newby2021). The applicability of interventions developed in higher income countries to LMICs such as South Africa cannot be assumed. This qualitative study highlights the importance of assessing youth’s ability to understand and engage with the core CBT concepts (such as feelings, thoughts and behaviours), in order to ensure that interventions (whether universal or targeted) are developmentally appropriate and accessible to all young people (Quakley et al., Reference Quakley, Reynolds and Coker2004; Reynolds et al., Reference Reynolds, Girling, Coker and Eastwood2006; Withers, Reference Withers, Dryden and Branch2012). Attending to cognitive development and emotional literacy ensures that intervention content is appropriately tailored towards the needs and development of young people.

Data availability statement

Data for this study are available from the authors on reasonable request.

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank Helene Loxton, Naomi Myburgh, Gerrit Laning and the NGO staff members for their continuous guidance and support. We would also like to thank the children who participated in this study; we learned so much from you.

Author contributions

Suzanne Human: Conceptualization (equal), Data curation (equal), Formal analysis (equal), Funding acquisition (supporting), Investigation (equal), Methodology (equal), Project administration (lead), Resources (supporting), Software (equal), Supervision (supporting), Validation (equal), Visualization (equal), Writing – original draft (lead), Writing – review & editing (lead); Hermine Gericke: Conceptualization (supporting), Data curation (equal), Formal analysis (equal), Funding acquisition (supporting), Investigation (equal), Methodology (supporting), Project administration (supporting), Resources (supporting), Software (equal), Supervision (supporting), Validation (supporting), Visualization (supporting), Writing – original draft (supporting), Writing – review & editing (supporting); Maria Loades: Conceptualization (lead), Data curation (equal), Formal analysis (equal), Funding acquisition (lead), Investigation (equal), Methodology (equal), Project administration (supporting), Resources (equal), Software (supporting), Supervision (lead), Validation (equal), Visualization (equal), Writing – original draft (supporting), Writing – review & editing (supporting); Paul Stallard: Conceptualization (lead), Data curation (supporting), Formal analysis (equal), Funding acquisition (lead), Investigation (equal), Methodology (equal), Project administration (supporting), Resources (equal), Software (supporting), Supervision (lead), Validation (equal), Visualization (equal), Writing – original draft (supporting), Writing – review & editing (supporting); Bronwynè Coetzee: Conceptualization (lead), Data curation (equal), Formal analysis (equal), Funding acquisition (lead), Investigation (equal), Methodology (equal), Project administration (equal), Resources (equal), Software (equal), Supervision (lead), Validation (lead), Visualization (equal), Writing – original draft (supporting), Writing – review & editing (supporting).

Financial support

This research was funded in whole by the Wellcome Trust (213987/Z/18/Z). For the purpose of Open Access, the author has applied a Creative Commons Attribution (CC-BY) public copyright licence to any Author Accepted Manuscript version arising from this submission. Dr Maria Loades is funded by the National Institute for Health Research (NIHR Doctoral Research Fellowship, DRF-2016-09-021). This is independent research. The views expressed in this publication are those of the authors(s) and not necessarily those of the NHS, NIHR or the Department of Health and Social Care.

Conflicts of interest

The authors declare none.

Ethical standards

The study was approved by Stellenbosch University’s Research Ethics Committee: Human Research (Humanities) (SU project number: 9183) and the Western Cape Education Department (reference number: 20190213-1562). Reciprocity was received from Bath University. Authors have abided by the Ethical Principles of Psychologists and Code of Conduct as set out by the BABCP and BPS.

References

Albano, A. M., & Kendall, P. C. (2002). Cognitive behavioural therapy for children and adolescents with anxiety disorders: clinical research advances. International Review of Psychiatry, 14, 129134. doi: 10.1080/09540260220132644 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (5th edn). Arlington, VA, USA: American Psychiatric Publishing Google Scholar
Belfer, M. L. (2008). Editorial. International Review of Psychiatry, 20, 215216. doi: 10.1080/09540260802028967 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bradshaw, M., Gericke, H., Coetzee, B. J., Stallard, P., Human, S., & Loades, M. (2021). Universal school-based mental health programmes in low- and middle-income countries: a systematic review and narrative synthesis. Preventive Medicine, 143, 106317. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2020.106317 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Braun, V., & Clarke, V. (2006). Using thematic analysis in psychology. Qualitative Research in Psychology, 3, 77101. doi: 10.1191/1478088706qp063oa CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cortina, M. A., Fazel, M., Hlungwani, T. M., Kahn, K., Tollman, S., Cortina-Borja, M., & Stein, A. (2013) Childhood psychological problems in school settings in rural Southern Africa. PLoS One, 8, e65041. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0065041CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Creswell, C., Waite, P., & Cooper, P. J. (2014). Assessment and management of anxiety disorders in children and adolescents. Archives of Disease in Childhood, 99, 674678. doi: 10.1136/archdischild-2013-303768 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dalsgaard, S., McGrath, J., Østergaard, S. D., Wray, N. R., Pedersen, C.B., Mortensen, P. B., & Petersen, L. (2020). Association of mental disorder in childhood and adolescence with subsequent educational achievement. Journal of the American Medical Association Psychiatry, 77, 797805. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2020.0217 Google ScholarPubMed
Das-Munshi, J., Lund, C., Mathews, C., Clark, C., Rothon, C., & Stansfeld, S. (2016) Mental health inequalities in adolescents growing up in post-apartheid South Africa: cross-sectional survey, SHaW Study. PLoS One, 11, e0154478. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0154478 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
De Girolamo, G., Dagani, J., Purcell, R., Cocchi, A., & McGorry, P. D. (2012). Age of onset of mental disorders and use of mental health services: needs, opportunities and obstacles. Epidemiology and Psychiatric Sciences, 21, 4757. doi: 10.1017/s2045796011000746 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Devereux, S., Hochfeld, T., Karriem, A., Mensah, C., Morahanye, M., Msimango, T., Mukubonda, A., Naicker, S., Nkomo, G., Sanders, D., & Sanousi, M. (2018). School feeding in South Africa: what we know, what we don’t know, what we need to know, what we need to do. Food Security SA Working Paper Series No. 004. DST-NRF Centre of Excellence in Food Security, South Africa. https://foodsecurity.ac.za/wp-content/uploads/2018/06/CoE-FSWP4-School-Feeding-in-South-Africa-11-jun-18.pdf Google Scholar
Finning, K., Waite, P., Harvey, K., Moore, D., Davis, B., & Ford, T. (2020) Secondary school practitioners’ beliefs about risk factors for school attendance problems: a qualitative study. Emotional and Behavioural Difficulties, 25, 1528. doi: 10.1080/13632752.2019.1647684 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Flisher, A. J., Dawes, A., Kafaar, Z., Lund, C., Sorsdahl, K., Myers, B., Thom, R., & Seedat, S. (2012) Child and adolescent mental health in South Africa. Journal of Child & Adolescent Mental Health, 24, 149161, doi: 10.2989/17280583.2012.735505 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fombonne, E., Simmons, H., Ford, T., Meltzer, H., & Goodman, R. (2001). Prevalence of pervasive developmental disorders in the British nationwide survey of child mental health. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 40, 820827. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200107000-00017 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gee, B., Reynolds, S., Carroll, B., Orchard, F., Clarke, T., Martin, D., Wilson, J., & Pass, L. (2020). Practitioner Review: Effectiveness of indicated school-based interventions for adolescent depression and anxiety: a meta-analytic review. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 61, 739756. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.1320 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Georgakakou-Koutsonikou, N., Taylor, E. P., & Williams, J. M. (2019). Children’s concepts of childhood and adolescent depression. Child and Adolescent Mental Health, 24, 1928. doi: 10.1111/camh.12266 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Georgakakou-Koutsonikou, N., & Williams, J. M. (2017). Children and young people’s conceptualizations of depression: a systematic review and narrative meta-synthesis. Child: Care, Health and Development, 43, 161181. doi: 10.1111/cch.12439 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Giel, R., De Arango, M. V., Climent, C. E., Harding, T. W., Ibrahim, H. H., Ladrido-Ignacio, L., Murthy, R. S., Salazar, M. C., Wig, N. N., & Younis, Y. O. (1981). Childhood mental disorders in primary health care: results of observations in four developing countries. A report from the WHO collaborative Study on Strategies for Extending Mental Health Care. Pediatrics, 68, 677683. Retrieved from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/7312471 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Human, S. (2018). Child-friendly activities to assist young children in the identification of feelings: a core component of CBT-based anxiety intervention programmes. Master’s thesis, Stellenbosch University, Stellenbosch, South Africa.Google Scholar
Kessler, R. C., Angermeyer, M., Anthony, J. C., De Graaf, R., Demyttenaere, K., Gasquet, I., De Girolamo, G., Gluzman, S., Gureje, O., Haro, J. M., Kawakami, N., Karam, A., Levinson, D., Medina Mora, M. E., Oakley Browne, M. A., Posada-Villa, J., Stein, D. J., Adley Tsang, C. H., Aguilar-Gaxiola, S., Alonso, J., … & Ustün, T. B. (2007). Lifetime prevalence and age-of-onset distributions of mental disorders in the World Health Organization’s World Mental Health Survey Initiative. World Psychiatry, 6, 168176.Google ScholarPubMed
Kieling, C., Baker-Henningham, H., Belfer, M., Conti, G., Ertem, I., Omigbodun, O., Rohde, L. A., Srinath, S., Ulkuer, N., & Rahman, A. (2011). Child and adolescent mental health worldwide: evidence for action. Lancet, 378, 15151525. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(11)60827-1 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Klein, J. B., Jacobs, R. H., & Reinecke, M. A. (2007). Cognitive-behavioral therapy for adolescent depression: a meta-analytic investigation of changes in effect-size estimates. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 46, 14031413. doi: 10.1097/chi.0b013e3180592aaa CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Louw, D., & Louw, A. (2014). Middle childhood. In D. Louw and A. Louw (eds), Child and Adolescent Development (2nd edn, pp. 223300). Bloemfontein, South Africa: Psychology Publications.Google Scholar
Loxton, H., & Human, S. (2017). Exploring child-friendly activities to assist young South African children in the identification of feelings. 12th International Conference on Child and Adolescent Psychopathology, 17–19 July 2017.Google Scholar
Loxton, H., & Webber, I. (2015). Viability of a CBT-based activity to improve psychosocial development in a group of poverty-stricken South African children. Presentation at the 14th European Congress of Psychology, 7–10 July 2015.Google Scholar
Lu, C., Li, Z., & Patel, V. (2018). Global child and adolescent mental health: the orphan of development assistance for health. PLOS Medicine, 15, e1002524. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.1002524 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mackenzie, K., & Williams, C. (2018). Universal, school-based interventions to promote mental and emotional well-being: what is being done in the UK and does it work? A systematic review. BMJ Open, (8): e022560. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2018-022560 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mahoney-Davies, G., Roberts-Collins, C., Russell, A., & Loades, M. (2017). Socialization to the model in adolescent cognitive behavioural therapy: measurement and insights. the Cognitive Behaviour Therapist, 10. doi: 10.1017/S1754470X17000186 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mrazek, P. J., & Haggerty, R. J. (1994). Reducing Risks for Mental Disorders: Frontiers for Preventive Intervention Research. Washington, DC, USA: National Academy Press.Google Scholar
Nowell, L. S., Norris, J. M., White, D. E., & Moules, N. J. (2017). Thematic analysis: striving to meet the trustworthiness criteria. International Journal of Qualitative Methods, 16, 113. doi: 10.1177/1609406917733847 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Patel, V., Flisher, A. J., Hetrick, S., & McGorry, P. (2007). Mental health of young people: a global public-health challenge. Lancet, 369, 13021313. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(07)60368-7 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Patel, V., Flisher, A. J., Nikapota, A., & Malhotra, S. (2008). Promoting child and adolescent mental health in low and middle income countries. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 49, 313334. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2007.01824.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Piaget, J. (1972). The Child and Reality. Frederick Muller Ltd.Google Scholar
Polanczyk, G. V., Salum, G. A., Sugaya, L. S., Caye, A., & Rohde, L. A. (2015). Annual research review: a meta-analysis of the worldwide prevalence of mental disorders in children and adolescents. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 56, 345365. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12381 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Quakley, S. M. (2001). An investigation into children’s developing ability to identify and link thoughts, feelings and behaviours: Implications for cognitive behaviour therapy for children. Doctoral dissertation, University of East Anglia, Norwich, UK. Retrieved from: http://ethos.bl.uk/OrderDetails.do?uin=uk.bl.ethos.247209.Google Scholar
Quakley, S., Coker, S., Palmer, K., & Reynolds, S. (2003). Can children distinguish between thoughts and behaviours? Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 31, 159168. doi: 10.1017/S1352465803002030.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Quakley, S., Reynolds, S., & Coker, S. (2004). The effect of cues on young children’s abilities to discriminate among thoughts, feelings and behaviours. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 42, 343356. doi: 10.1016/S0005-7967(03)00145-1.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rescorla, L., Achenbach, T., Ivanova, M. Y., Dumenci, L., Almqvist, F., Bilenberg, N., Bird, H., Chen, W., Dobrean, A., Döpfner, M., Erol, N., Fombonne, E., Fonseca, A., Frigerio, A., Grietens, H., Hannesdottir, H., Kanbayashi, Y., Lambert, M., Larsson, B., … & Verhulst, F. (2007). Behavioral and emotional problems reported by parents of children ages 6 to 16 in 31 societies. Journal of Emotional and Behavioral Disorders, 15, 130142. doi: 10.1177/10634266070150030101 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reynolds, S., Girling, E., Coker, S., & Eastwood, L. (2006). The effect of mental health problems on children’s ability to discriminate amongst thoughts, feelings and behaviours. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 30, 599607. doi: 10.1007/s10608-006-9037-6 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reynolds, S., Wilson, C., Austin, J., & Hooper, L. (2012). Effects of psychotherapy for anxiety in children and adolescents: a meta-analytic review. Clinical Psychology Review, 32, 251262. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2012.01.005 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stallard, P. (2019). Think Good, Feel Good: A Cognitive Behavioural Therapy Workbook for Children and Young People (2nd edn). Chichester, UK: John Wiley & Sons Ltd.Google Scholar
Visagie, L., Loxton, H., Stallard, P., & Silverman, W. K. (2017). Insights into the feelings, thoughts, and behaviors of children with visual impairments: a focus group study prior to adapting a cognitive behavior therapy–based anxiety intervention. Journal of Visual Impairment and Blindness, 111, 231246.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Webber, L. P. (2016). Exploring the viability of a cognitive behavioural therapy-based activity for usage in a future anxiety intervention programme within the South African context. Master’s thesis, Stellenbosch University, Stellenbosch, South Africa.Google Scholar
Weisz, J. R., McCarty, C. A., & Valeri, S. M. (2006). Effects of psychotherapy for depression in children and adolescents: a meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 132149. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.132.1.132 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Werner-Seidler, A., Spanos, S., Calear, A. L., Perry, Y., Torok, M., O’Dea, B., Christensen, H., & Newby, J. M. (2021). School-based depression and anxiety prevention programs for young people: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Clinical Psychology Review, 89, 102079. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2021.102079 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wickersham, A., Dickson, H., Jones, R., Pritchard, M., Stewart, R., Ford, T., & Downs, J. (2020). Educational attainment trajectories among children and adolescents with depression, and the role of sociodemographic characteristics: longitudinal data-linkage study. British Journal of Psychiatry, 218, 151157. doi: 10.1192/bjp.2020.160 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Withers, T. (2012). CBT with children and adolescents. In Dryden, W. and Branch, R. (eds), The CBT Handbook (pp. 289307). London, UK: SAGE Publications Ltd.Google Scholar
Yap, M. B. H., & Jorm, A. F. (2015). Parental factors associated with childhood anxiety, depression, and internalizing problems: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Journal of Affective Disorders, 175, 424440. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2015.01.050 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yatham, S., Sivathasan, S., Yoon, R., da Silva, T. L., & Ravindran, A. V. (2018). Depression, anxiety, and post-traumatic stress disorder among youth in low and middle income countries: a review of prevalence and treatment interventions. Asian Journal of Psychiatry, 38, 7891. doi: 10.1016/j.ajp.2017.10.029 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Demographic information (2011 national census) on the areas where the two schools are situated, compared with provincial (Western Cape) and national averages

Figure 1

Table 2. Demographics of participants (n=22)

Figure 2

Table 3. Themes and sub-themes identified following deductive thematic analysis

Submit a response

Comments

No Comments have been published for this article.