Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-l7hp2 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-22T16:31:35.130Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

‘All eyes are on you’: Gender, race, and opinion writing on the US Courts of Appeals

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 January 2024

Laura P. Moyer*
Affiliation:
Department of Political Science, University of Louisville, Louisville, Kentucky, USA
John Szmer
Affiliation:
Department of Political Science and Public Administration, University of North Carolina at Charlotte, Charlotte, North Carolina, USA
Susan Haire
Affiliation:
Department of Political Science, University of Georgia, Athens, Georgia, USA
Robert K. Christensen
Affiliation:
Romney Institute of Public Service and Ethics, Brigham Young University, Provo, Utah, USA
*
Laura P. Moyer, University of Louisville, Louisville, Kentucky, USA., Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Because stereotyping affects individual assessments of ability and because of socializing experiences in the law, we argue that women and judges of color, while well-credentialed, feel pressure to work harder than their white male peers to demonstrate their competence. Using an original dataset of published appellate court opinions from 2008–2016, we find that majority opinions authored by female and non-white judges go farther to explain and justify their rulings, when compared to opinions written by white male peers. In comparison to other judges, opinions by white men are about 6% shorter, with 11% fewer citations, and 17% fewer extensively discussed citations. Our findings suggest that norms about crafting judicial opinions are gendered and racialized in ways that create higher workloads for women and judges of color.

Type
Articles
Copyright
© 2021 Law and Society Association.

Prestigious legal positions in the United States are filled, overwhelmingly, by white men. Women and people of color are underrepresented as tenured law school faculty and deans, as general counsel of Fortune 500 companies, and as partners in law firms (Reference Kellerman and RhodeKellerman & Rhode, 2017; Reference Nelson, Sendroiu, Dinovitzer and DaweNelson et al., 2019). In state judiciaries, a 2019 report noted that white men constituted 56% of state supreme court seats, in spite of making up less than one third of the US population (Reference Robbins and BannonRobbins & Bannon, 2019). The same report analyzed states with judicial elections and found that white male incumbents were more likely to run unopposed than white women, minority women, or minority men. As of January 2021, five of the nine sitting US Supreme Court justices were white men. Although Presidents Carter, Clinton, George W. Bush, and Obama all significantly increased the presence of women and individuals of color in the lower federal judiciary (Reference Haire and MoyerHaire & Moyer, 2015),Footnote 1 one observer recently noted, “If you are ever unfortunate enough to wind up in federal court, chances are that your fate will be decided by a white man” (Reference MillhiserMillhiser, 2019).

The experience of being one of a few “outsiders” in a high-profile, prestigious institution can be isolating and pressure-inducing for women and judges of color (Reference Haire and MoyerHaire & Moyer, 2015). As Justice Ruth Bader Ginsburg observed about her experiences as a woman in the legal profession, “when you are the only woman, all eyes are on you” (Associated Press, 1993). Biographers and historians have also documented how the first black and Latinx judges to be appointed to the federal appellate bench felt pressure to exceed expectations (Reference FischFisch, 1996; Reference WareWare, 1984).

How might racial minorities and women respond to their high-profile positions in the judiciary? From one line of inquiry, negative stereotypes about one's relative abilities will be activated for women and people of color as a result of their token presence in a largely white, male institution (Reference Hoyt, Johnson, Murphy and SkinnellHoyt et al., 2010; Reference KanterKanter, 1977; Reference Steele, Aronson and SpencerSteele et al., 2007; Reference Taylor, Fiske, Etcoff and RudermanTaylor et al., 1978). Although this type of stereotype threat has been observed to lead to diminished performance, counter-stereotypical, positive behaviors may also be induced as a result of this situation (Reference BrehmBrehm, 1966; Reference Hoyt and BlascovichHoyt & Blascovich, 2007; Reference Kray, Thompson and GalinskyKray et al., 2001). Related research on the impostor phenomenon finds that, in fields dominated by a particular group, individuals who do not fit into the profession's stereotype may adopt perfectionistic tendencies and set exceptionally high standards for their work, in order to demonstrate they have legitimately earned their position (Reference Clance and ImesClance & Imes, 1978; Reference Cokley, McClain, Enciso and MartinezCokley et al., 2013; Reference Sakulku and AlexanderSakulku & Alexander, 2011). As Reference Nelson, Sendroiu, Dinovitzer and DaweNelson et al. (2019, p. 1052) argue, “[g]ender and racial stereotypes afford individual members of privileged gender or ethno-racial groups the presumption of competence while women and racial minorities are held to a higher standard than their white male counterparts.” In the law, a profession that has historically been dominated by white men, Reference KirkpatrickKirkpatrick (2020) argues that white maleness is conflated with the neutral ideal of “judge as umpire,” leaving judges who are women and people of color open to charges of bias or incompatibility with the judicial role (see also Reference Means and UnahMeans & Unah, 2020).

Taken together, this literature raises the possibility that, whether consciously or subconsciously, judges who are people of color and/or women are likely to feel pressure to work harder than white men to explain and to justify their decisions to relevant audiences like co-panelists, litigants, and even the Supreme Court in order to be persuasive (Reference BaumBaum, 2006; Reference BoydBoyd, 2015). This is especially likely among the very few elite individuals appointed to the federal appellate courts, an institution comprised primarily of white, male judges. When deciding whether to pursue a seat, “outsider” judges may believe being overqualified is necessary to survive a multi-staged selection process characterized by intense public scrutiny.Footnote 2 This perception is reinforced by reality: women and non-white nominees receive lower ABA ratings, take longer to be confirmed, and receive more negative votes in the Judiciary Committee (Reference Nixon and GossNixon & Goss, 2001; Reference SenSen, 2014; Reference SteigerwaltSteigerwalt, 2010).

In this study, we explore whether female and non-white judges—judges who have survived the gauntlet of law school, a legal career, and Senate confirmation—process the weighty responsibility of opinion writing differently than their white male counterparts. Will these “outsider” judges be especially driven to legitimate the opinion of the court?

Judges who write for the majority have discretion when deciding how to explain the ruling of the court to key audiences (Reference BaumBaum, 2006; Reference Leonard and RossLeonard & Ross, 2016), including which, and how many, cases to cite (Reference Choi and GulatiChoi & Gulati, 2007; Reference HinkleHinkle, 2017; Reference KleinKlein, 2002). Indeed, a thorough, well-reasoned opinion helps to legitimize the judicial use of political power to shape public policy by demonstrating that the decision was reached by reasonably applying neutral legal principles (Reference CarterCarter, 1979, p. 221). Furthermore, Reference KimKim (2009, p. 1343) observes that it is “the content of the opinions rather than a simple declaration of a winner that shapes the development of the law.”

As we explain below, we expect that women and people of color will write longer, more thorough opinions that incorporate more extensive discussion of precedents cited than those authored by their white male colleagues. Using a large, representative sample of published federal appellate opinions from 2008 to 2016, we find evidence of differences between white male judges (“insiders”) and women and non-white judges (“outsiders”) that support our expectations.

GENDER, RACE, AND SELF-ASSESSMENT

A large body of research in the social sciences has documented that individuals' confidence in their abilities and intellect is heavily influenced by the structural hierarchies that assign meaning to race and gender. Social forces like family, mass media, teachers, and other cultural institutions shape the expectations that individuals have for themselves and for others (Reference Bian, Leslie and CimpianBian et al., 2017; Reference Furnham and RawlesFurnham & Rawles, 1995; Reference PajaresPajares, 2002).

With respect to gender, children internalize messages that relay different expectations for boys and girls, as well as assertions that particular domains are masculine (e.g., math and science) or feminine (e.g., language arts).Footnote 3 As a result of these and other processes, researchers consistently find that “boys are more likely to express confidence in skills they may not possess and to express overconfidence in skills they do possess” (Reference PajaresPajares, 2002, p. 118). Regardless of objective indicators of ability, research confirms that girls are more prone than boys to experience internal distress, evaluate themselves negatively, and worry about their academic performance (Reference Pomerantz, Altermatt and SaxonPomerantz et al., 2002; Reference Silverman, La Greca and WassersteinSilverman et al., 1995). These confidence differences persist into adulthood (Reference Kling, Hyde, Showers and BuswellKling et al., 1999) and have been documented even among highly accomplished individuals such as medical professionals (Reference Blanch, Hall, Roter and FrankelBlanch et al., 2008). When tasks are masculine-typed, women are more likely than men to display inaccurately low self-perceptions of their performance (Reference Beyer and BowdenBeyer & Bowden, 1997) and to underestimate their intelligence relative to men (Reference Furnham and RawlesFurnham & Rawles, 1995).

With respect to race, research points to a similar mechanism whereby negative societal expectations about race contribute to low self-esteem and reduced performance among racial minorities. From this perspective, harmful stereotypes associated with racial identity (e.g., African Americans are not as intelligent as whites) can create an unconscious fear of confirming the negative stereotype (Reference SteeleSteele, 1997). Empirical evidence suggests that this stereotype threat emerges early on in life and undermines self-confidence, resulting in reduced academic performance among minority children and young adults (Reference Vecci and ZelinskyVecci & Zelinsky, 2019; Reference WasserbergWasserberg, 2014). Findings from multiple experimental studies of adults consistently document underperformance by African Americans who were primed by a negative stereotype; in the absence of the primed stereotype, performance was consistent with ability (Reference Steele, Aronson and SpencerSteele et al., 2007). Outside of the lab setting, research indicates that those who were the only people of color in their office experienced a greater degree of stereotype threat, which subsequently affected how those workers interpreted feedback from peers and supervisors (Reference Roberson, Deitch, Brief and BlockRoberson et al., 2003). Scholars have also observed the consequences of stereotype threat for professionals in higher education, including the legal academy (Reference ManningManning, 2018; Reference McClainMcClain, 2016; Reference WhaleyWhaley, 2018).Footnote 4

Differences in confidence may also manifest themselves in a psychological phenomenon known as impostor syndrome (Reference Clance and ImesClance & Imes, 1978). Impostors believe that they are unworthy of success and fear that they will be revealed as frauds, which drives them “to work even harder to keep up the charade” (Reference Ewing, Richardson, James-Myers and RussellEwing et al., 1996, p. 54). As such, impostorism is often characterized by extreme overpreparation, a desire to be special relative to peers, and perfectionistic tendencies (Reference Sakulku and AlexanderSakulku & Alexander, 2011) that drive self-perceived impostors to “hold themselves to exceptionally high standards” (Reference Cokley, McClain, Enciso and MartinezCokley et al., 2013, p. 85). Research has documented impostorism in a variety of male-dominated professional contexts for women (Reference Henning, Ey and ShawHenning et al., 1998; Reference Vaughn, Taasoobshirazi and JohnsonVaughn et al., 2019), as well as among racial and ethnic minorities in predominantly white settings (Reference Ewing, Richardson, James-Myers and RussellEwing et al., 1996; Reference Lige, Peteet and BrownLige et al., 2017; Reference Peteet, Montgomery and WeekesPeteet et al., 2015).

Next, we explore how these feelings of otherness may be fueled in law school and in subsequent experiences in the legal profession, ultimately affecting the behavior of the elite few who attain judicial office.

EXPERIENCES IN LEGAL EDUCATION AND THE PATH TO THE BENCH

Through most of the 20th century, law schools were almost exclusively white and male in their student composition (Reference TelesTeles, 2012), and the few women and racial minorities who pursued a legal education encountered outright hostility and explicit discrimination from professors and classmates alike (Reference Haire and MoyerHaire & Moyer, 2015). By the end of 2000, however, women had made significant inroads, making up just under half of the student population (Reference Moyer and HaireMoyer & Haire, 2015). With respect to race, one report noted that from 1993 to 2013, the percent of law school graduates who were racial minorities increased from 14% to 25% (NALP, 2014).

In spite of the trend toward a somewhat more diverse law student population, law schools have been the subject of critiques about the ways in which the climate and pedagogical approach undermines the self-confidence of women and people of color and fosters feelings of alienation (Reference Weiss and MellingWeiss & Melling, 1988). One study found that women and racial minorities were significantly more likely than white men to agree with the statement, “Before law school I thought of myself as intelligent and articulate, but I often don't feel that way about myself now” (Reference FischerFischer, 1996; Reference Guinier, Fine and BalinGuinier et al., 1994; Reference KrauskopfKrauskopf, 1994). Another account from first-year law students noted that,

“The white male students adjusted rapidly to this socialization process, and many outwardly seemed to enjoy it. Many women and people of color, on the other hand, felt themselves outside of this process and experienced a growing unhappiness and frustration with it…[I]t felt as if we had been admitted to [law school] under the condition that we speak, if at all, in a tongue that was entirely foreign to us.” (Reference Homer and SchwartzHomer & Schwartz, 1989, p. 3)

The homogeneity of law school faculty certainly plays a role in these critiques (Reference FischerFischer, 1996), particularly during the period of time that present-day judges attended law school. Indeed, half of US Courts of Appeals judges actively serving between 2008 and 2018 graduated from law school by 1972, and 75% earned law degrees by 1982 (FJC, 2020). In the mid-70s, faculty were almost entirely white (96%) and male (93%) (Reference FossumFossum, 1983). In contrast, by 2002, two-thirds of tenured law faculty were white men (Reference Mertz, Frances, Katherine, Wamucii, Molly and JoanneMertz et al., 2012). The numbers of female law faculty have risen, but they are still less likely to earn tenure (Reference NeumannNeumann, 2000) and are overrepresented in lower paid, nontenure track positions teaching legal writing. The authors of a 2001 report criticized this practice, noting that while “virtually all lawyers and judges acknowledge that legal writing is the single most important course in law school and agree that this course provides the fundamental underpinnings of law practice,… the overwhelmingly male power structure in law schools disdainfully treats teaching this course as ‘women's work’” (Reference Stanchi and LevineStanchi & Levine, 2001, p. 5). Women of color who teach legal writing describe how they are “presumed incompetent” and report the need to continually demonstrate their legitimacy to their fellow colleagues (Reference BannaiBannai, 2014). One recent book documents substantial, widespread professional and personal challenges faced by women of color law faculty that extend to all levels of the legal academy, reinforcing the idea that non-white women are “outsiders” or even “intruders” in the white, male legal profession (Reference DeoDeo, 2019, p. 41).

Beyond law school, women and racial minorities also deal with occupational stereotyping that undermines their sense of belonging in the legal profession. Surveys of attorneys reveal that lawyers who are not white males often deal with embarrassing and distressing instances of “mistaken identity” from peers, clients, and even judges (e.g., Reference EpnerEpner, 2006). For example, black male attorneys report being confused with criminal defendants, while Latinx attorneys are confused with interpreters and women of color assumed to be administrative staff (Reference RhodeRhode, 1994; Reference Garcia-LopezGarcia-Lopez, 2008). White women, women of color, and—to a lesser extent—non-white men are more likely than white male attorneys to have experienced a situation where a client requested a new attorney (Reference Nelson, Sendroiu, Dinovitzer and DaweNelson et al., 2019), and black judges are more likely to be asked to recuse themselves in cases involving race (Reference Means and UnahMeans & Unah, 2020). In the courtroom and in interactions with other attorneys, including their own co-workers, female attorneys report demeaning behavior, such as inappropriate comments about their appearance (Reference Collins, Dumas and MoyerCollins et al., 2017, Reference Collins, Dumas and Moyer2018). These experiences illustrate and reinforce perceptions that women and people of color do not belong in prestigious legal occupations, like that of attorney or judge.

In addition to occupational stereotyping that links white maleness with being a judge, women and people of color must also contend with negative stereotypes related to their gender and race (and the intersection of the two). As noted above, negative stereotypes about the competence and personality traits of particular groups can create anxiety for individuals in those groups. This anxiety is heightened with awareness that their behavior could be judged as “stereotypical” by outside groups. This, in turn, impacts the performance of achievement-oriented tasks (Reference Davis and SilverDavis & Silver, 2003). For example, Reference EpnerEpner (2006, p. 10) quotes a non-white, female attorney who described the demeaning comments she received because of her gender, race, and age: “When I first start practicing, it would make me incensed to the point where I would lose my concentration and focus and I would not be as good of an advocate as I would have been if it weren't an issue.” A black female attorney observed, “White associates are not expected to be perfect. Black associates. .. have one chance and if you mess up that chance, look out. There is no room for error” (Reference EpnerEpner, 2006, p. 25). Among attorneys, one study concluded that women of color “felt a need to continually establish their competence to professors, peers, and judges” (Reference EpnerEpner, 2006, p. 2).

Although negative stereotypes may ultimately fuel anxiety and result in under-performance by women and non-white lawyers, these negative stereotypes can also produce positive responses (Reference Hoyt and BlascovichHoyt & Blascovich, 2007). Described by Reference BrehmBrehm (1966) as psychological reactance theory, this perspective suggests that individuals respond to stereotype threat by engaging in behavior that is contrary to a negative stereotype about one's race or gender. For members of racial and ethnic minority groups, there is an additional motivation to counter negative expectations: linked fate. In his observations about race, and the legal profession, Wilkins noted that the “actions of individual blacks will affect the fate of the black community as a whole, and that ties the opportunities available to any individual black to the progress of the group” (Reference WilkinsWilkins, 1994, p. 1041).

Empirical support for reactance theory can also be found in research on legislative bodies where women's lower self-assessments of ability translate into more productive legislative behavior when they are elected to office. According to Reference Lazarus and SteigerwaltLazarus and Steigerwalt (2018), women in Congress overestimate their electoral vulnerability in part because of higher levels of self-doubt. As a result, they work harder than their male peers in a variety of ways, including increasing communication with constituents, garnering more federal spending for the district, and more effectively representing constituent preferences. Reference Thomsen and SandersThomsen and Sanders (2020) find similar results among state legislators. A recent study of judicial elections found that female lawyers who wish to become state supreme court justices are risk averse and underestimate their abilities; female prospective candidates wait until a seat is open before pursuing an office and delay running until they are certain they will have the “best” qualifications (Reference NguyenNguyen, 2019; but see Reference WilliamsWilliams, 2008). This literature suggests that female judges may be inclined to underestimate their abilities and believe they must overprepare in achievement-related tasks like writing majority opinions. For example, a female attorney, specifically commenting on gender stereotypes in the legal profession, said, “I show up extremely prepared, professional, articulate, knowledgeable about my case, friendly and authentic. I've found that it works in gaining the opposing side's respect and it helps educate them so they don't disrespect other women lawyers or me again” (Reference WeissWeiss, 2018).

The pressure to overwork may also be fueled by contextual cues that signal to “outsiders” that they will be held to a higher standard of performance (e.g., Reference HengelHengel, 2017). Reference Lazarus and SteigerwaltLazarus and Steigerwalt (2018) note that the overworking behavior of women in Congress reflects the reality that female incumbents are more likely than their male counterparts to draw a challenger. In fact, women in Congress have to “introduce twice as much legislation as men to see the probability of challenger emergence decrease to a level that is indistinguishable from that of men” (Reference Atkinson and WindettAtkinson & Windett, 2019, p. 769). Reference Gill, Lazos and WatersGill et al. (2011) also note that external assessments of judicial performance are biased against minority and female judges.

Pressures toward overpreparation may also be shaped by the desire to persuade key audiences. As Reference Lazarus and SteigerwaltLazarus and Steigerwalt (2018) find, female legislators react to their gendered vulnerability by working harder to appeal to constituents, an audience that is central to legislators' ability to stay in office. Judges are also mindful of their audiences (Reference BaumBaum, 2006) and seek to persuade those audiences that their decisions are legally sound (Reference KleinKlein, 2002). For federal appellate judges, key audiences for a majority opinion author include their colleagues on the panel and circuit, the litigants, and the Supreme Court (Reference MoyerMoyer, 2008). Consistent with Reference ChaikenChaiken (1980) and Reference O'KeefeO'Keefe (1998), women and non-white judges may feel that they must work harder than their white male colleagues to persuade these audiences that their decisions are legally correct. Along these lines, Judge Patricia Wald of the DC Circuit observed that opinion authors write “to persuade their colleagues and the public they are moving law in the right direction” (Reference WaldWald, 1995, p. 1372).

Because white men have authored the vast majority of court opinions in the American legal system, we argue that this identity is implicitly linked with judicial opinion writing (see also Reference KirkpatrickKirkpatrick, 2020). Ifill (2011, p. 441) similarly argues that “society builds the values embodied in our laws, for example, upon a set of accepted and legitimized narratives … In general, the dominant community's narratives form the basis of our approach to legal doctrine, theory, and practice. When white men were the only group permitted to offer and legitimate narratives in the legal process, master narratives could function virtually undisturbed as unassailable truths.”

Taken together, these mechanisms all point in a similar direction: women and people of color will feel additional pressures to succeed in an environment that has long been the domain of white men. Both individual assessments of ability related to stereotypes and socializing experiences in the law will drive these judges, while well-credentialed, to feel that they must work harder than their white male peers to demonstrate their competence. As a result, we argue that “outsider” judges will strive for perfection in their work, including in the most visible and enduring aspect of their position as an appellate judge: authoring an opinion for the court.

On the federal appeals courts, rotating three-judge panels must quickly come to a collective resolution of a case. The opinion author plays a prominent role in shaping the output of the panel's deliberations (Reference HinkleHinkle, 2017). With help from clerks, the majority opinion assignee writes an initial opinion draft, which is then circulated to the other panelists. Commonly, the other judges on the panel will suggest changes to the draft, which are usually incorporated into the opinion (Reference Bowie, Songer and SzmerBowie et al., 2014). Majority opinion authors have strong incentives to accommodate their colleagues' preferences (Reference HinkleHinkle, 2017) because doing so can help insulate the opinion from en banc and Supreme Court review (Reference Beim, Hirsch and KastellecBeim et al., 2016).Footnote 5 Norms of consensus also motivate the opinion writer to incorporate her colleagues' perspectives (Reference Bowie, Songer and SzmerBowie et al., 2014, p. 97).

Extant research suggests that “outsider” opinion authors may more thoroughly explain the reasons behind their rulings than do white male opinion authors. Reference BoydAmong federal district court judges, Boyd (2015) finds that female and non-white judges tend to write lengthier opinions, perhaps to better justify the outcome to litigants. Judges who are racial minorities are more thorough in drawing upon due process jurisprudence when adjudicating sentencing guidelines (Reference Sisk, Heise and MorrissSisk et al., 1998) and have been found to write longer opinions in other appellate contexts (Reference Budziak, Hitt and LempertBudziak et al., 2019). This leads us to our first hypothesis:

H1 Female and non-white judges will write longer opinions than white, male judges.

In addition, over-preparation may also manifest itself through a greater identification of, reliance on, and incorporation of legal authorities in the majority opinion. While citing legal authorities in a majority opinion is linked to self-presentation motivations among all judges (Reference BaumBaum, 2006; Reference Choi and GulatiChoi & Gulati, 2007; Reference HumeHume, 2009), we argue that female judges and judges of color will be especially driven to justify their rulings by incorporating more legal authorities compared to their white male colleagues. This leads to the following two hypotheses:

H2 Female and non-white judges will write opinions that cite more legal authorities than opinions authored by white, male judges.

H3 Female and non-white judges will write opinions that devote more attention to discussing cited legal authorities than opinions authored by white, male judges.

DATA AND MEASURES

Using an original sample of signed, published opinions from the US Courts of Appeals (2008–2016), we merged case data with information on judicial backgrounds acquired from the Federal Judicial Center.Footnote 6 The analysis contains three dependent variables, each of which captures an aspect of opinion thoroughness: page length (Opinion Length), the total number of precedents cited in the opinion (Total Authorities), and the number of cited cases discussed in depth (Deep Cites).Footnote 7 Because the variables are counts, and because concrete evidence shows overdispersion,Footnote 8 we use negative binomial regression to estimate the model parameters. Additionally, given potentially correlated errors within circuits and over time, as well as across opinions authored by the same judge, we include unconditional circuit and year fixed effects along with White-Huber robust standard errors clustered by opinion author.Footnote 9

Our primary variables of interest reflect the race and gender of the judge who authored the opinion, Female Judge (1 if female, 0 if male) and Non-white Judge (1 if non-white, 0 if white).Footnote 10 We expect that the coefficients for these variables will be positive in all models.

We also include controls for characteristics of the opinion author that could influence the opinion length and sourcing: Law Professor (coded 1 if the judge was previously a tenure-track law school professor and 0 otherwise), Elite Law School (coded 1 if the judge attended an elite law school and 0 if not),Footnote 11 and Judge Tenure (the number of years since the judge was commissioned to serve as a federal appeals court judge, logged due to skewness and potential diminishing marginal returns). Because tenure-track law school professors are socialized to write lengthy, well-cited texts due to research expectations within the field, we expect to find a positive Law Professor coefficient. Elite Law School should be positive to the extent that it provides a rough measure of the ability of the judge. Conversely, we posit a negative coefficient for Judge Tenure, as early-career judges may feel the need to write longer opinions with arguments justified by more sources.

Judges should generally pen longer, more thoroughly reasoned opinions in salient cases (Reference Bowie, Songer and SzmerBowie et al., 2014). Here, we incorporate three indirect measures of case salience: whether the opinion involved constitutional interpretation (Constitutional Issue), whether an amicus was filed (Amicus Curiae), and whether a prior published decision was associated with the case (Prior Publication). We follow the coding protocols developed by Reference SongerSonger (1997); information on coding procedures related to measures for these case characteristics can be found in the codebook that accompanies the Multi-User Database. We expect the estimated coefficients for all three to be positive in all models.

Closely related to these indicators of case salience is a dummy variable, Affirmed, coded 1 if the court affirmed the lower court or agency decision and 0 if the court completely or partially reversed the prior decision. Courts are more likely to affirm decisions of the lower court or agency when the issues are “easy.” In these routine cases, judges are more likely to write shorter opinions with fewer quoted citations. Consistent with this causal mechanism, one prior study concluded that Courts of Appeals judges tended to write longer opinions when the panel reversed the lower-court decision (Reference Epstein, Landes and PosnerEpstein et al., 2013), while another found that federal appellate judges typically wrote shorter opinions when the panel affirmed the decision below (Reference Haire and MoyerHaire & Moyer, 2015). Based on the theory and prior evidence, we expect the coefficients will be negative.

Opinions also may be longer and devote greater attention to precedents as a result of the number of lawyers in an appeal. Evidence suggests that Courts of Appeals judges are less likely to suppress issues raised in briefs by larger litigation teams; moreover, judges tend to side with litigants represented by more attorneys (Reference Haire and MoyerHaire & Moyer, 2008). Together, this suggests that larger litigation teams will present more persuasive and informative briefs—which can facilitate the work of the opinion author. Because the variable is positively skewed, and existing work finds diminishing marginal returns as the number of lawyers increases (Reference Haire and MoyerHaire & Moyer, 2008), we calculate the natural log of the total number of attorneys who are listed as counsel representing all appellant(s) and respondent(s) in the case (Number of Attorneys). We expect the estimated coefficient will be positive in all models.

We also include controls that account for the role of the majority opinion author's policy preferences and potential ideological differences among panel members. Outcome Congruence is coded 1 if the judges' presumed ideology (based on the party of the appointing president) is congruent with the ideological direction of the court's decision (i.e., a Democrat [Republican] supporting a liberal [conservative] outcome); otherwise, it is coded 0. We expect that judges will be inclined to write longer, more thoroughly cited opinions when the outcome is consistent with their policy predisposition and shorter, less thoroughly cited opinions when their policy views are inconsistent with the outcome. Split Panel is coded 1 if the panel included judges who were appointed by presidents from different political parties. Circuit court norms strongly encourage the judges to incorporate colleagues' suggestions when writing the majority opinion (Reference Bowie, Songer and SzmerBowie et al., 2014). Judges who are like-minded are more likely to converge quickly on the reasoning, resulting in an opinion that is shorter and less thorough in its reasoning. Based on these expectations, the coefficient for Split Panel should be positive.

We control for those cases in which conflict on the panel resulted in publishing a dissenting (Dissent) or concurring opinion (Concurrence). Separate opinions are more likely to occur in salient and legally ambiguous cases (Reference Hettinger, Lindquist and MartinekHettinger et al., 2006). In addition, majority opinion authors should respond to the dissenter's counterarguments. Supreme Court justices, Courts of Appeals judges, and judges on state courts of last resort tend to write longer majority opinions in cases with separate opinions (Reference Bowie, Songer and SzmerBowie et al., 2014; Reference Epstein, Landes and PosnerEpstein et al., 2013; Reference Leonard and RossLeonard & Ross, 2016). Therefore, the coefficients should be positive, indicating a tendency to write longer opinions with more directly quoted, and thoroughly cited legal precedents.

Finally, we estimate circuit caseload (Caseload) using the Judicial Caseload Profile measure of the US Courts' Federal Court Management Statistics. This profile measures the number of terminated cases per active judge during the year the case was decided.Footnote 12 Reference Epstein, Landes and PosnerEpstein et al. (2013) posit that judicial behavior balances costs and benefits. When judges have higher workloads, the opportunity costs of writing longer and more diligently sourced opinions increases. Consistent with this expectation, researchers found that higher circuit workload (Reference Epstein, Landes and PosnerEpstein et al., 2013) and opinion-writer workload (Reference Bowie, Songer and SzmerBowie et al., 2014) tended to reduce the length of US Courts of Appeals majority opinions. With that in mind, the coefficient should be negative.

ANALYSIS

Based on multiple mechanisms, including stereotyping, impostorism, and socialization forces, we expect women and people of color, as “outsiders” on the prestigious federal appellate bench, to experience higher pressures to be successful in the status quo environment dominated by “insider” white male judges. Since our hypotheses generally focus on the effects of outsider status, we begin by comparing white male judges to all other judges.

Figure 1 displays evidence from the negative binomial regression model that confirms our general expectations: minority and female judges write longer opinions with more sources, cites, and deep cites. The plots show expected counts of the dependent variables (number of pages, total citations, and deep citations) for judges who are white men (“insiders”) and those who are not white men (“outsiders”), averaged across observed combinations of the other covariates.Footnote 13 The estimates are circles, and the horizontal lines are confidence interval ranges. On average, white men write opinions that are roughly two-thirds of a page shorter, with almost three fewer cites, and one fewer deep cite. We also generated percent changes to get a better sense of the relative effects of “outsider” performance pressures on each of the dependent variables. We observed the largest gaps in citation patterns. Opinions written by white males, on average, contain 10.7% fewer citations to precedent and 16.6% fewer citations discussed with more than a paragraph of text. Conversely, their opinions are 5.26% shorter than those written by women and non-white judges.

Figure 1. Marginal effects of white male versus other judges, US Courts of Appeals cases, 2008–2016. Note: N = 2906. Confidence intervals shown are for two-tailed tests (p < 0.05). Full results for these models appear in Table A1

The following analyses unpack the previous findings with respect to each of our hypotheses. Hypothesis 1 predicts that women and non-white judges would author lengthier opinions than their white, male counterparts. We include a control for Total Authorities, which allows us to evaluate the effect of our variable of interest after taking into account the number of citations on opinion length.Footnote 14 Figure 2 displays the results of the negative binomial regression on the number of pages in the majority opinion using average marginal effectsFootnote 15 plots, with the independent variables on the Y axis.Footnote 16 As the figure shows, there is a positive and statistically significant effect for the Female Judge variable. On average, women write opinions that are approximately two-thirds of a page longer than their male peers. Put another way, women write opinions that are roughly 5.6% longer, on average. Although the relationship is in the predicted direction, non-white judges do not author significantly longer opinions than their white colleagues to a statistically significant degree.

Figure 2. Marginal effects of expected count of opinion length, US Courts of Appeals cases, 2008–2016. Note: N = 2906. Unconditional circuit and year fixed effects not shown. Confidence intervals shown are for two-tailed tests (p < 0.05). Full results shown in Table A2

Moving to control variables, the plot also shows that former law professors author longer opinions than judges who lack experience in legal academia. Case characteristics such as the presence of a constitutional issue, amicus curiae, or a dissenting opinion tend to increase the number of pages in the majority opinion, as expected.

Figure 3 shows the average marginal effects generated from the negative binomial regression on the total number of authorities cited in the majority opinion, testing Hypothesis 2. Consistent with our hypothesis, female and minority opinion writers include more citations in majority opinions than their white male colleagues. Opinions of the court written by women contain, on average, 6.7% more citations than those authored by male peers. The results for Non-white Judge indicate that non-white judges write opinions that have 11.2% more citations than those written by white jurists, all else equal.

Figure 3. Marginal effects of expected count of the total number of authorities cited in majority opinions, US Courts of Appeals cases, 2008–2016. Note: N = 2906. Unconditional circuit and year fixed effects not shown. Confidence intervals shown are for two-tailed tests (p < 0.05). Full results shown in Table A2

Among the other covariates, Judge Tenure, Amicus Curiae, Number of Attorneys, Affirmed, Prior Publication, Dissent, and Constitutional Issue are statistically significant—and all in the expected direction.Footnote 17 The effects for cases that raise a constitutional issue are the most striking. On average, judges include more than eight additional citations in constitutional cases than in cases that merely interpret statutes or administrative regulations.

To evaluate our third hypothesis, Figure 4 shows the average marginal effects generated from the negative binomial regression on the number of cases discussed in depth (Deep Cites) in the majority opinion. Again, the marginal effects of Female Judge and Non-white Judge are positive and statistically significant. Consistent with expectations, on average, female judges extensively discuss about one-half more cited cases than do their male counterparts. When converted to percent changes, we see that, on average, women incorporate 11.3% more deep cites compared to male opinion authors. Also, as with the previous model, Non-white Judge is positive and significant; opinions authored by nonwhite judges discuss about one more cited authority in depth compared to opinions authored by white judges. Expressed using percent changes, appellate judges of color write majority opinions that contain, on average, 19.5% more deep citations than their white peers.

Figure 4. Marginal effects of expected count of the deep cites in majority opinions, US Courts of Appeals cases, 2008–2016. Note: N = 2906. Unconditional circuit and year fixed effects not shown. Confidence intervals shown are for two-tailed tests (p < 0.05). Full results shown in Table A2

The findings for most control variables support our expectations about the role of case-level factors in shaping the depth of discussion surrounding a cited precedent. Constitutional Issue, Amicus Curiae, Affirmed, Prior Publication, Number of Attorneys, and Concurrence are all statistically significant in the posited direction. In particular, judges seem to examine precedents more deeply when their majority opinion is challenged by a dissent. On average, the majority opinion includes 3.2 more deep cites when there is a dissent than in unanimously decided cases.

Of course, women of color in the legal profession experience multiple facets of discrimination that reflect the intersecting nature of their identities (Reference Collins, Dumas and MoyerCollins et al., 2017; Reference Collins and MoyerCollins & Moyer, 2008; Reference DeoDeo, 2019) and that could drive impostorism fears that affect opinion writing. We estimated a series of additional models that looked at the effect of race and gender together. In each model, the results failed to identify intersectional differences (see Supporting Information for full models).Footnote 18

DISCUSSION

As the ranks of the federal judiciary continue to include judges from a broader range of backgrounds, it is increasingly important to understand how social forces condition the ways that individuals assess their abilities and determine requirements for career success in male-dominated professions like politics and law. Even those who have reached the elite ranks of their profession are not immune to self-doubt and feelings of impostorism (Reference Blanch, Hall, Roter and FrankelBlanch et al., 2008; Reference Lazarus and SteigerwaltLazarus & Steigerwalt, 2018). While much of the extant work on gender and race in the judiciary focuses on the voting behavior of judges, another profitable avenue for inquiry focuses on the micro-foundations of racialized and gendered institutions, identifying ways in which actors and institutional rules or norms interact to shape expectations and how such interactions shape the distribution of power within an institution (Reference LowndesLowndes, 2020, p. 544).

Building from this body of work, our central argument posits that high-achieving women and people of color in the legal profession have adopted strategies for dealing with pressure and societal expectations and that they carry these strategies with them onto the bench. Consistent with our expectations, the evidence suggests that female and minority appellate judges are more likely to overprepare in achievement-related tasks like writing majority opinions, spending more effort than their male colleagues to justify to key audiences (litigants, other judges, and the Supreme Court) that their decision is legally correct. Majority opinions written by women and minorities include more citations to prior cases, and they are more likely to devote more attention to discussing specific precedents. Even after controlling for the tendency to include more citations, our study shows that women still write longer opinions than men do, consistent with findings from other, earlier studies of trial courts (Reference BoydBoyd, 2015). Moreover, these differences in opinion writing do not appear to vary based on the length of time a judge has served on the bench.Footnote 19

Our results have important implications for scholars of courts and other legal and political institutions. First, our findings suggest that standard measures of workload, like cases per judge, may obscure systematic differences in effort. To put this into context, the median number of written opinions per year for a judge in our sample is 44, consistent with what circuit judges themselves describe (Reference WaldWald, 1995). If women write about two-thirds of a page more in each opinion, this would translate to an estimated additional 26 pages a year for each woman. Over a female judge's career, this would total hundreds of additional pages of writing from her majority opinions alone. Of course, this is likely a conservative estimate, given studies that find that female judges are disproportionately assigned to write more opinions than are men on other courts with discretionary opinion-assignment practices (Reference Christensen, Szmer and StritchChristensen et al., 2012). In addition, the cumulative effect of additional work, particularly over time, could hasten work-related burnout and stress, which some studies have found to affect more female judges than male judges (Reference Lustig, Delucchi, Tennakoon, Kaul, Marks and SlavinLustig et al., 2008; Reference Flores, Miller, Chamberlain, Richardson and BornsteinFlores et al., 2008, Flores et al., 2009; but see Reference Miller, Reichert, Bornstein and ShulmanMiller et al., 2018). Future research should continue to explore whether and in what ways this gendered pattern of additional effort extends to other high-achieving female political elites, such as members of Congress and state legislators (Reference Dolan and FordDolan & Ford, 1995), as well how it may extend to the experiences of racial minorities in political institutions.

This study also contributes to the burgeoning judicial-politics literature on opinion content and the use of precedent in the federal judiciary (Reference Black, Owens, Wedeking and WohlfarthBlack et al., 2016; Reference Corley, Collins and CalvinCorley et al., 2011; Reference Nelson and HinkleNelson & Hinkle, 2018). Opinion content is an important focus for inquiry because it is “the opinions themselves, not who won or lost … which provide the constraining directions to the public and private decision makers who determine the 99 percent of conduct that never reaches the courts” (Reference ShapiroShapiro, 1968, p. 39). The US Courts of Appeals, in particular, are crucial institutions to examine because they establish the bulk of federal precedent due to the relatively small docket of the Supreme Court, which effectively renders most federal appellate decisions the final word (Reference HinkleHinkle, 2015).

There are, of course, limitations to our study that point to areas where more research is needed. Law clerks undoubtedly play a central role in the work of federal appellate judges; a 2013 survey of federal appellate judges found that more than 90 percent used their law clerks to do legal research and help them draft opinions (Reference Peppers, Giles and Tainer-ParkinsPeppers et al., 2014). Unfortunately, law-clerk information for each judge's chambers is not publicly available, so we are unable to control for the demographic makeup of each judge's chambers and cannot systematically evaluate how law clerks affected the content of particular opinions.Footnote 20 It is quite likely, however, that men are overrepresented as law clerks for the judges in our sample; national data from the law school class of 2016 reveals that more than half of all federal clerkships went to men, a number that has stayed consistent since 2006 (NALP, 2017; see also Reference Szmer, Kaheny and ChristensenSzmer et al., 2014).Footnote 21 Future surveys will be an important vehicle to investigate whether the addition of more female and non-white judges has had the effect of socializing future judges into different norms of opinion writing than those more commonly adopted by white male judges. More generally, while we note that gender and race-based differences are suggestive of labor disparities—in terms of both observable work product and the psychological toll that may be associated with dynamics such as impostorism and stereotype threat—it is also possible that the contributions of “outsider” judges could raise expectations for all judges, in the long run, about what a majority opinion should be.

With respect to intersectional identities, studies of the legal profession have identified particular ways in which women of color (Reference Collins, Dumas and MoyerCollins et al., 2017; Reference DeoDeo, 2019; Reference EpnerEpner, 2006; Reference Haire and MoyerHaire & Moyer, 2015) experience distinct kinds of discrimination compared to white women and minority men. We fail to find evidence of different opinion-writing approaches by women of color, though our analysis is hampered by the relatively small number of majority opinions authored by this group in our sample. For this reason, quantitative approaches may not always be ideal for untangling the ways in which socialization and stereotypes play out in opinion writing for particular intersectional cohorts. Future work should use interviews and archival materials to explore how these judges negotiate the pressures and expectations associated with the prestigious posts they occupy. Moreover, researchers might profitably explore deeper narratives that might, for example, identify whether female judges and judges of color feel impostorism and stereotype threat pressures differently and under what circumstances.

Taken together, our findings suggest that norms about a central activity of judging, crafting opinions, are gendered and racialized in ways that create higher workloads for “outsider” judges. As scholars explore other facets of diversifying legal institutions, it will be important to assess whether other norms operate in the same fashion and, if so, what changes in organizational practice can create more equitable outcomes.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

The authors gratefully acknowledge that data for this project were collected with support from the National Science Foundation (NSF SES #1655159, 1654614, 1654559, 1654697). Any opinions, findings, and conclusions or recommendations expressed are those of the authors and do not necessarily reflect the views of the National Science Foundation. The authors also would like to thank Barry Edwards, Ethan Boldt, Alyson Hendricks-Benton, Madison Eckels, and Samuel LaVergne. Please direct all correspondence to Laura Moyer, 205 Ford Hall, Louisville, KY 40292, .

SUPPORTING INFORMATION

Additional supporting information may be found online in the Supporting Information section at the end of this article.

How to cite this article: Moyer, Laura P., John Szmer, Susan Haire, Robert K. Christensen. 2021. “‘All eyes are on you’: Gender, race, and opinion writing on the US Courts of Appeals.” Law & Society Review 55(3): 452-472. https://doi.org/10.1111/lasr.12559

APPENDIX

Table A1 Models comparing white males to all other judges (negative binomial models with unconditional fixed effects)

Note: Standard errors clustered by judge. Output for circuit and year dummies not shown.

*p < 0.10 (two-tailed); **p < 0.05 (two-tailed).

Table A2 Full results for Figures 24 (negative binomial models with unconditional fixed effects)

Note: Standard errors clustered by judge. Output for circuit and year dummies not shown.

*p < 0.10 (two-tailed); **p < 0.05 (two-tailed).

Footnotes

How to cite this article: Moyer, Laura P., John Szmer, Susan Haire, Robert K. Christensen. 2021. “All eyes are on you”: Gender, race, and opinion writing on the US Courts of Appeals. Law & Society Review 55(3): 452-472. https://doi.org/10.1111/lasr.12559

Funding information National Science Foundation, Grant/Award Numbers: 1654614, 1654559, 1654697, 1655159

Note: Standard errors clustered by judge. Output for circuit and year dummies not shown.

*p < 0.10 (two-tailed); **p < 0.05 (two-tailed).

Note: Standard errors clustered by judge. Output for circuit and year dummies not shown.

*p < 0.10 (two-tailed); **p < 0.05 (two-tailed).

1 Under President Trump, this trend has reversed, with the appointment of fewer non-white judges to the federal bench (FJC 2020). Trump did appoint slightly more women than George W. Bush but far fewer than recent Democratic presidents (Reference GramlichGramlich, 2021).

2 We use the term “insider” to refer to white male judges because they are the largest race-gender cohort in the federal judiciary and legal profession, both historically and during the period of study, and, as such, shape perceptions of what a judge should look like and who should be a judge. We use the term “outsider” to refer to judges who are women and/or people of color because individuals from these groups have faced (and continue to face) overt discrimination in the legal profession and represent a change from the status quo of the white male judge. However, we recognize that on other dimensions, like connections to powerful interest groups or professional networks, outsider and insider status might break down differently.

3 Even in feminine-typed subjects, girls do not evaluate themselves more positively than boys after academic performance is taken into account (Reference Pomerantz, Altermatt and SaxonPomerantz et al., 2002). This raises the possibility that institutional factors reinforce and compound gender gaps in self-assessment.

4 In the Supreme Court context, Reference Onwuachi-WilligOnwuachi-Willig (2004) argues that both Clarence Thomas and the late Thurgood Marshall have frequently been described in ways that rely on stereotypes about African Americans' supposed intellectual inferiority and laziness.

5 Other members of the panel also have incentives to be persuaded by the majority-opinion author rather than to write a separate opinion, including the extra time and effort associated with writing separate opinions (Reference Epstein, Landes and PosnerEpstein et al., 2011).

6 Following the sampling design of Reference SongerSonger (1997), we drew a stratified, random sample of 30 published cases per circuit year for the 12 geographic circuits. En banc decisions were excluded. The Federal Circuit is omitted due to the breadth of its geographic jurisdiction and its limited subject-matter jurisdiction.

7 The Deep Cites variable is derived from Westlaw's “Depth of Treatment Codes” and represents the number of precedents that were labeled either “discussed” or “examined” in the text of the majority opinion. In its documentation, Westlaw states that “discussed” indicates that the author devotes “more than a paragraph but less than a printed page” and that “explained” reflects more than a printed page devoted to discussing the cited case. (More on Westlaw's coding scheme can be found here: https://lscontent.westlaw.com/images/content/KeyCite%20on%20Westlaw.pdf. Accessed February 20, 2020).

8 The likelihood-ratio test of the alphas for all three models resulted in observed probability levels below 0.000. We can reject the null hypothesis that alpha is equal to zero, which is a clear indication of overdispersion (Reference Long and FreeseLong & Freese, 2014).

9 We did not use conditional fixed effects because they lead to unstable, biased estimates of the fixed covariates in negative binomial models (Reference AllisonAllison, 2009). Following Reference Allison and WatermanAllison and Waterman (2002), we also estimated outer product of gradient standard errors as a potential correction for the bias and present these substantively similar results in the Supporting Information file.

10 We recognize that the dummy-variable approach lumps together distinct races and ethnicities. In our sample, most of the opinion authors were either white (82%), African American (10%), or Latinx (6%). Because Asian American judges authored opinions only in less than 2% of the cases (and there was only one Asian American woman), we also estimated the models excluding those observations (see Supporting Information). Regardless of specification, the results are unchanged.

11 We used Reference SlotnickSlotnick's (1983) measure, which identified 15 elite law schools: UC Berkeley, Columbia, Pennsylvania, Harvard, Yale, Michigan, Stanford, Chicago, Cornell, NYU, Duke, Cornell, Texas, UCLA, and Virginia. See also Reference Szmer and GinnSzmer and Ginn (2014).

12 We accessed the reports here: http://www.uscourts.gov/statistics-reports/analysis-reports/federal-court-management-statistics (accessed August 19, 2018). Prior to 2010, the US Courts estimated statistics only for fiscal years, not calendar years. Therefore, for 2009 we used the federal fiscal-year estimates, which ran from October 1, 2008 through September 30, 2009.

13 Please see Table A1 for the estimates of the full model with control variables.

14 The effects of Female Judge and Non-white Judge are even larger when we exclude Total Authorities (see Supporting Information).

15 The average marginal effects are differences in the expected counts averaged across all values of the covariates.

16 The point estimates of the average marginal effects are black circles, and the gray lines running through the circles illustrate the 95 percent confidence intervals. Positive point estimates are to the right of the solid vertical zero line. When the confidence intervals do not cross the vertical zero line, the estimate is statistically significant. (The table of results appears in the Appendix.)

17 To assess whether there were writing-related gender differences in adjusting to the appellate bench, we also estimated supplemental models that investigated whether the effect of Female Judge was conditional on Judge Tenure (see Supporting Information), but the interaction terms and marginal effects graphs provided no evidence of a statistically significant interaction effect.

18 We caution against generalizing from these results in light of the small number of women of color in our sample (13 circuit judges and four district judges sitting by designation). Collectively, these judges authored less than 5% of the majority opinions in the analysis, and only 10 non-white women in our sample authored more than one majority opinion.

19 It is also possible that, if a judge's life experiences related to race and/or gender lead them to have a different basis for a decision, it may take a longer opinion with more citations to justify and contextualize those reasons, given that these perspectives are underrepresented within the federal judiciary. We thank an anonymous reviewer for raising these points.

20 Most of what is known about law clerk contributions to appellate opinion writing comes from interviews with judges or former clerks (e.g., Reference Bowie, Songer and SzmerBowie et al., 2014; Reference Swanson and WasbySwanson & Wasby, 2008); the Administrative Office of the Courts does not collect demographic information on law clerks (Reference MauroMauro, 2019). Recent news accounts and Congressional testimony have cataloged concerning instances of abuse and harassment by male circuit judges toward their female law clerks (Reference MatthewsMatthews, 2020; Reference PurvisPurvis, 2017).

21 The NALP report also reveals substantial racial disparities in clerkships; 40% of all federal law clerks are white men, followed by white women at 30%, minority women at 9.2%, and minority men at 5.9% (NALP, 2017).

References

REFERENCES

Allison, Paul D. 2009. Fixed Effects Regression Models. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Allison, Paul D., and Waterman, Richard P. 2002. “Fixed-Effects Negative Binomial Regression Models.” Sociological Methodology 32(1): 247-65.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Associated Press. 1993. “Science Will Minimize Dispute over Abortion, Ginsburg Says.” Deseret News, accessed November 16, 2020, https://www.deseret.com/1993/8/9/19060163/science-will-minimize-dispute-over-abortion-ginsburg-says.Google Scholar
Atkinson, Mary Layton, and Windett, Jason Harold. 2019. “Gender Stereotypes and the Policy Priorities of Women in Congress.” Political Behavior 41: 769-89.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bannai, Lorraine K. 2014. “Challenged x 3: The Stories of Women of Color Who Teach Legal Writing.” Berkeley Journal of Gender, Law, and Justice 29: 275-94.Google Scholar
Baum, Lawrence. 2006. Judges and their Audiences. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beim, Deborah, Hirsch, Alexander V., and Kastellec, Jonathan P. 2016. “Signaling and Counter-Signaling in the Judicial Hierarchy: An Empirical Analysis of En Banc Review.” American Journal of Political Science 60(2): 490508.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beyer, Sylvia, and Bowden, Edward M. 1997. “Gender Differences in Self-Perceptions: Convergent Evidence from Three Measures of Accuracy and Bias.” Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 23(2): 157-72.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bian, Lin, Leslie, Sarah-Jane, and Cimpian, Andrei. 2017. “Gender Stereotypes about Intellectual Ability Emerge Early and Influence Children's Interests.” Science 355(6323): 389-91.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Black, Ryan C., Owens, Ryan J., Wedeking, Justin, and Wohlfarth, Patrick C. 2016. US Supreme Court Opinions and their Audiences. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blanch, Danielle C., Hall, Judith A., Roter, Debra L., and Frankel, Richard M. 2008. “Medical Student Gender and Issues of Confidence.” Patient Education and Counseling 72(3): 374-81.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bowie, Jennifer B., Songer, Donald R., and Szmer, John. 2014. The View from the Bench and Chambers: Examining Judicial Process and Decision Making on the U.S. Courts of Appeals. Charlottesville, VA: University of Virginia Press.Google Scholar
Boyd, Christina L. 2015. “Opinion Writing in the Federal District Courts.” Justice System Journal 36(3): 254-73.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brehm, Jack W. 1966. A Theory of Psychological Reactance. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Budziak, Jeffrey, Hitt, Matthew P., and Lempert, Daniel. 2019. “Determinants of Writing Style on the United States Circuit Courts of Appeals.” Journal of Law and Courts 7(1): 128.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carter, Lief. 1979. Reason in Law. Boston: Little, Brown.Google Scholar
Chaiken, Shelly. 1980. “Heuristic Versus Systematic Information Processing and the Use of Source Versus Message Cues in Persuasion.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 39(5): 752-66.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Choi, Stephen J., and Gulati, Mitu. 2007. “Trading Votes for Reasoning: Covering in Judicial Opinions.” Southern California Law Review 81: 735-79.Google Scholar
Christensen, Robert K., Szmer, John, and Stritch, Justin M. 2012. “Race and Gender Bias in Three Administrative Contexts: Impact on Work Assignments in State Supreme Courts.” Journal of Public Administration Research and Theory 22(4): 625-48.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clance, Pauline R., and Imes, Suzanne A. 1978. “The Imposter Phenomenon in High Achieving Women: Dynamics and Therapeutic Intervention.” Psychotherapy: Theory, Research and Practice 15(3): 241-7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cokley, Kevin, McClain, Shannon, Enciso, Alicia, and Martinez, Mercedes. 2013. “An Examination of the Impact of Minority Status Stress and Impostor Feelings on the Mental Health of Diverse Ethnic Minority College Students.” Journal of Multicultural Counseling and Development 41(2): 8295.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Collins, Todd A., Dumas, Tao L., and Moyer, Laura P. 2017. “Intersecting Disadvantages: Race, Gender, and Age Discrimination among Attorneys.” Social Science Quarterly 98(5): 1642-58.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Collins, Todd A., Dumas, Tao L., and Moyer, Laura P. 2018. “Tipping the Scales of Justice: Perceptions of Unfair Treatment in the Courtroom.” Justice System Journal 39(4): 303-21.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Collins, Todd A., and Moyer, Laura P. 2008. “Gender, Race, and Intersectionality on the Federal Appellate Bench.” Political Research Quarterly 61(2): 219-27.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Corley, Pamela, Collins, Paul M. Jr., and Calvin, Bryan. 2011. “Lower Court Influence on U.S. Supreme Court Opinion Content.” Journal of Politics 73(1): 3144.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davis, Darren W., and Silver, Brian D. 2003. “Stereotype Threat and Race of Interviewer Effects in a Survey on Political Knowledge.” American Journal of Political Science 47(1): 3345.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deo, Meera E. 2019. Unequal Profession: Race and Gender in Legal Academia. Palo Alto, CA: Stanford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dolan, Kathleen, and Ford, Lynne E. 1995. “Women in the State Legislatures: Feminist Identity and Legislative Behaviors.” American Politics Quarterly 23(1): 96108.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Epner, Janet E. 2006. Visible Invisibility: Women of Color in Law Firms. Chicago: ABA Commission on Women in the Profession.Google Scholar
Epstein, Lee, Landes, William M., and Posner, Richard A. 2011. “Why (and When) Judges Dissent: A Theoretical and Empirical Analysis.” Journal of Legal Analysis 3(1): 101-37.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Epstein, Lee, Landes, William M., and Posner, Richard A. 2013. The Behavior of Federal Judges: A Theoretical and Empirical Study of Rational Choice. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Ewing, Kimberly M., Richardson, Tina Q., James-Myers, Linda, and Russell, Richard K. 1996. “The Relationship between Racial Identity Attitudes, Worldview, and African American Graduate Students' Experience of the Imposter Phenomenon.” Journal of Black Psychology 22(1): 5366.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Federal Judicial Center. 2020 “Biographical Directory of Article III Federal Judges, 1789-present.” Accessed November 16, 2020. https://www.fjc.gov/history/judges.Google Scholar
Fisch, Louise. 1996. All Rise: Reynaldo G. Garza, the First Mexican American Federal Judge. College Station: University Press.Google Scholar
Fischer, Judith D. 1996. “Portia Unbound: The Effects of a Supportive Law School Environment on Women and Minority Students.” UCLA Women's Law Journal 7(1): 81130.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Flores, David M., Miller, Monica K., Chamberlain, Jared, Richardson, James T., and Bornstein, Brian H. 2009. “Judges' Perspectives on Stress and Safety in the Courtroom: An Exploratory Study.” Court Review 45(3): 7689.Google Scholar
Fossum, Donna. 1983. “Law and the Sexual Integration of Institutions: The Case of American Law Schools.” ALSA Forum 7(2–3): 222-50.Google Scholar
Furnham, Adrian, and Rawles, Richard. 1995. “Sex Differences in the Estimation of Intelligence.” Journal of Social Behavior and Personality 10(3): 741-5.Google Scholar
Garcia-Lopez, Gladys. 2008. “Nunca Te Toman En Cuenta (They Never Take You Into Account).” Gender and Society 22: 590612.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gill, R.D., Lazos, S.R., and Waters, M.M. 2011. “Are Judicial Performance Evaluations Fair to Women and Minorities? A Cautionary Tale from Clark County, Nevada.” Law and Society Rev. 45(3): 731-59.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gramlich, John. 2021. How Trump Compares with Other Recent Presidents in Appointing Federal Judges. Pew Research Center. Accessed January 13, 2021. https://www.pewresearch.org/fact-tank/2021/01/13/how-trump-compares-with-other-recent-presidents-in-appointing-federal-judges/Google Scholar
Guinier, Lani, Fine, Michelle, and Balin, Jane. 1994. “Becoming Gentlemen: Women's Experiences at One Ivy League Law School.” University of Pennsylvania Law Review 143(1): 1110.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haire, Susan B., and Moyer, Laura P. 2008. “Advocacy Through Briefs in the US Courts of Appeals.” Southern Illinois Law Journal 32: 593609.Google Scholar
Haire, Susan B., and Moyer, Laura P. 2015. Diversity Matters: Judicial Policy Making in the U.S. Courts of Appeals. Charlottesville, VA: University of Virginia Press.Google Scholar
Hengel, Erin. 2017. “Publishing While Female: Are Women Held to Higher Standards? Evidence from Peer Review.” Cambridge Working Paper Economics: 1753.Google Scholar
Henning, Kris, Ey, Sydney, and Shaw, Darlene. 1998. “Perfectionism, the Impostor Phenomenon and Psychological Adjustment in Medical, Dental, Nursing and Pharmacy Students.” Medical Education 32(5): 456-64.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hettinger, Virginia A., Lindquist, Stefanie A., and Martinek, Wendy L. 2006. Judging on a Collegial Court: Influences on Federal Appellate Decision Making. Charlottesville, VA: University of Virginia Press.Google Scholar
Hinkle, Rachael K. 2015. “Legal Constraint in the US Courts of Appeals.” Journal of Politics 77(3): 721-35.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hinkle, Rachael K. 2017. “Panel Effects and Opinion Crafting in the US Courts of Appeals.” Journal of Law and Courts 5(2): 313-36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Homer, Suzanne, and Schwartz, Lois. 1989. “Admitted but Not Accepted: Outsiders Take an inside Look at Law School.” Berkeley Women's Law Journal 5(1): 174.Google Scholar
Hoyt, Crystal L., and Blascovich, Jim. 2007. “Leadership Efficacy and Women Leaders’ Responses to Stereotype Activation.” Group Processes & Intergroup Relations 10(4): 595616.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hoyt, Crystal L., Johnson, Stefanie K., Murphy, Susan Elaine, and Skinnell, Kerri Hogue. 2010. “The Impact of Blatant Stereotype Activation and Group Sex-composition on Female Leaders.” The Leadership Quarterly 21(5): 716-32.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hume, Robert J. 2009. “The Impact of Judicial Opinion Language on the Transmission of Federal Circuit Court Precedents.” Law and Society Review 43(1): 127-50.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ifill, Sherrilyn A. 2011. “Racial Diversity on the Bench: Beyond Role Models and Public Confidence.” Washington and Lee Law Review 57: 405496.Google Scholar
Kanter, Rosabeth Moss. 1977. Men and Women of the Corporation. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Kellerman, Barbara, and Rhode, Deborah. 2017. “Women at the Top: The Pipeline as Pipe Dream.” About Campus 21(6): 11-8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kim, Pauline. 2009. “Deliberation and Strategy on the United States Courts of Appeals: An Empirical Exploration of Panel Effects.” University of Pennsylvania Law Review 157(5): 1319-81.Google Scholar
Kirkpatrick, Jennet. 2020. “Fairness has a Face: Neutrality and Descriptive Representation on Courts.” Politics, Groups, and Identities 8(4): 803-11.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klein, David E. 2002. Making Law in the United States Courts of Appeals. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kling, Kristen C., Hyde, Janet Shibley, Showers, Carolin J., and Buswell, Brenda N. 1999. “Gender Differences in Self-Esteem: A Meta-Analysis.” Psychological Bulletin 125(4): 470500.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Krauskopf, Joan M. 1994. “Touching the Elephant: Perceptions of Gender Issues in Nine Law Schools.” Journal of Legal Education 44(3): 311-40.Google Scholar
Kray, Laura J., Thompson, Leigh, and Galinsky, Adam. 2001. “Battle of the Sexes: Gender Stereotype Confirmation and Reactance in Negotiations.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 80(6): 942-58.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lazarus, Jeffrey, and Steigerwalt, Amy. 2018. Gendered Vulnerability: How Women Work Harder to Stay in Office. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leonard, Meghan E., and Ross, Joseph V. 2016. “Understanding the Length of State Supreme Court Opinions.” American Politics Research 44(4): 710-33.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lige, Quiera M., Peteet, Bridgette J., and Brown, Carrie M. 2017. “Racial Identity, Self-Esteem, and the Impostor Phenomenon Among African American College Students.” Journal of Black Psychology 43(4): 345-57.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Long, J. Scott, and Freese, Jeremy. 2014. Regression Models for Categorical Dependent Variables Using Stata. College Station, TX: Stata Press.Google Scholar
Lowndes, Vivien. 2020. “How Are Political Institutions Gendered?Political Studies 68(3): 543-64.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lustig, Stuart L., Delucchi, Kevin, Tennakoon, Lakshika, Kaul, Brent, Marks, Dana Leigh, and Slavin, Denise. 2008. “Burnout and Stress among United States Immigration Judges.” Bender's Immigration Bulletin: 2230.Google Scholar
Manning, P.J. 2018. “Word to the Wise: Feedback Intervention to Moderate the Effects of Stereotype Threat and Attributional Ambiguity on Law Students.” University of Maryland Law Journal of Race, Religion, Gender and Class 18: 99138.Google Scholar
Matthews, Susan. 2020. “When a ‘Liberal Lion’ Is Also Your Harasser.” Slate, accessed November 16, 2020, https://slate.com/news-and-politics/2020/02/sexual-harassment-stephen-reinhardt-clerkships-judiciary-congress.html.Google Scholar
Mauro, Tony. 2019. “Law Clerk Diversity: ‘It's Complicated.’” Law.com, accessed November 16, 2020, https://www.law.com/nationallawjournal/2019/09/26/law-clerk-diversity-its-complicated/.Google Scholar
McClain, Russell A. 2016. “Helping our Students Reach their Full Potential: The Insidious Consequences of Ignoring Stereotype Threat.” Rutgers Race and Law Review 17: 156.Google Scholar
Means, Taneisha and Unah, Isaac. 2020. “Judging Black Judges in Racialized Conflicts.” Presented at the Moore Undergraduate Research Apprentice Program (MURAP) Faculty Research Presentation Session, University of North Carolina-Chapel Hill, May.Google Scholar
Mertz, E., Frances, T., Katherine, B., Wamucii, N., Molly, H., and Joanne, M. 2012. “After Tenure: Post-Tenure Law Professors in the United States.” American Bar Foundation, accessed November 16, 2020, http://www.americanbarfoundation.org/uploads/cms/documents/after_tenure-_addendum_to_lsac_report_-_july_30.pdfGoogle Scholar
Miller, Monica K., Reichert, Jenny, Bornstein, Brian H., and Shulman, Grant. 2018. “Judicial Stress: The Roles of Gender and Social Support.” Psychiatry, Psychology and Law 25(4): 602-18.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Millhiser, Ian. 2019. “The Absurd Whiteness of America's Court System, in 2 Charts.” Vox, accessed November 16, 2020, https://www.vox.com/policy-and-politics/2019/10/3/20893643/whiteness-federal-judiciary-diversity-obama-trump.Google Scholar
Moyer, Laura P. 2008. “Organizations, Complexity, and Decision Making in the U.S. Courts of Appeals.” Ph.D. diss., Department of Political Science, University of Georgia.Google Scholar
Moyer, Laura P., and Haire, Susan B. 2015. “Trailblazers and those that Followed: Personal Experiences, Gender, and Judicial Empathy.” Law and Society Review 49(3): 665-89.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
NALP. 2014. “Increasing Diversity of Law School Graduates Not Reflected Among Judicial Clerks.” NALP Bulletin, accessed November 16, 2020, https://www.nalp.org/0914research.Google Scholar
NALP. 2017. “A Demographic Profile of Judicial Clerks—2006 to 2016.” NALP Bulletin, Accessed November 16, 2020. https://www.nalp.org/1017research.Google Scholar
Nelson, Michael J., and Hinkle, Rachael K. 2018. “Crafting the Law: How Opinion Content Influences Legal Development.” Justice System Journal 39(2): 97122.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nelson, Robert L., Sendroiu, Ioana, Dinovitzer, Ronit, and Dawe, Meghan. 2019. “Perceiving Discrimination: Race, Gender, and Sexual Orientation in the Legal Workplace.” Law and Social Inquiry 44(4): 1051-82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Neumann, Richard K. 2000. “Women in Legal Education: What the Statistics Show.” Journal of Legal Education 50: 313-57.Google Scholar
Nguyen, Tony. 2019. “Why Women Win: Gender and Success in State Supreme Court Elections.” American Politics Research 47(3): 582600.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nixon, David C., and Goss, David L. 2001. “Confirmation Delay for Vacancies on the Circuit Courts of Appeals.” American Politics Research 29(3): 246-74.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
O'Keefe, Daniel J. 1998. “Justification Explicitness and Persuasive Effect: A Meta-Analytic Review of the Effects of Varying Support Articulation in Persuasive Messages.” Argumentation and Advocacy 35(2): 6175.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Onwuachi-Willig, Angela. 2004. “Just another Brother on the SCT: What Justice Clarence Thomas Teaches us about the Influence of Racial Identity.” Iowa Law Review 90: 9311009.Google Scholar
Pajares, Frank. 2002. “Gender and Perceived Self-Efficacy in Self-Regulated Learning.” Theory Into Practice 41(2): 116-25.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peppers, Todd C., Giles, Micheal W., and Tainer-Parkins, Bridget. 2014. “Surgeons or Scribes? The Role of United States Court of Appeals Law Clerks in ‘Appellate Triage’.” Marquette Law Review 98: 313-32.Google Scholar
Peteet, Bridgette J., Montgomery, LaTrice, and Weekes, Jerren C. 2015. “Predictors of Imposter Phenomenon Among Talented Ethnic Minority Undergraduate Students.” Journal of Negro Education 84(2): 175-86.Google Scholar
Pomerantz, Eva M., Altermatt, Ellen R., and Saxon, Jill L. 2002. “Making the Grade but Feeling Distressed: Gender Differences in Academic Performance and Internal Distress.” Journal of Educational Psychology 94(2): 396404.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Purvis, Dara. 2017. “When Judges Prey on Clerks.” New York Times, Accessed November 16, 2020. https://www.nytimes.com/2017/12/12/opinion/law-schools-alex-kozinski.html.Google Scholar
Rhode, Deborah L. 1994. Professional Responsibility: Ethics by the Pervasive Method. Boston: Little and Brown.Google Scholar
Robbins, Laila and Bannon, Alice. 2019. “State Supreme Court Diversity.” Brennan Center for Justice, accessed November 16, 2020, https://www.brennancenter.org/our-work/research-reports/state-supreme-court-diversity.Google Scholar
Roberson, Loriann, Deitch, Elizabeth A., Brief, Arthur P., and Block, Caryn J. 2003. “Stereotype Threat and Feedback Seeking in the Workplace.” Journal of Vocational Behavior 62(1): 176-88.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sakulku, Jaruwan, and Alexander, James. 2011. “The Impostor Phenomenon.” Journal of Behavioral Science 6(1): 7597.Google Scholar
Sen, Maya. 2014. “How Judicial Qualification Ratings May Disadvantage Minority and Female Candidates.” Journal of Law and Courts 22(1): 3365.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shapiro, Martin. 1968. The Supreme Court and Administrative Agencies. New York: Free Press.Google Scholar
Silverman, Wendy K., La Greca, Annette M., and Wasserstein, Shari. 1995. “What Do Children Worry about? Worries and Their Relation to Anxiety.” Child Development 66(3): 671-86.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sisk, Gregory C., Heise, Michael, and Morriss, Andrew P. 1998. “Charting the Influences on the Judicial Mind: An Empirical Study of Judicial Reasoning.” NYU Law Review 73: 1377–500.Google Scholar
Slotnick, Elliot E. 1983. “Federal Trial and Appellate Judges: How Do They Differ?Western Political Quarterly 36(4): 570-78.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Songer, Donald A. 1997. “Multi-User Database on the U.S. Courts of Appeals, 1925–1996.” Accessed May 1, 2020. http://artsandsciences.sc.edu/poli/juri/KH_update_codebook.pdfGoogle Scholar
Stanchi, Kathryn M., and Levine, Jan M. 2001. “Gender and Legal Writing: Law School's Dirty Little Secrets.” Berkeley Women's Law Journal 16(3): 327.Google Scholar
Steele, C.M., Aronson, J., and Spencer, S.J. 2007. “Stereotype Threat.” In Encyclopedia Social Psychology. Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE Publications.Google Scholar
Steele, Claude M. 1997. “A Threat in the Air. How Stereotypes Shape Intellectual Identity and Performance.” American Psychologist 52(6): 613-29.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Steigerwalt, Amy. 2010. Battle Over the Bench: Senators, Interest Groups, and Lower Court Confirmations. Charlottesville, VA: University of Virginia Press.Google Scholar
Swanson, Rick A., and Wasby, Stephen L. 2008. “Good Stewards: Law Clerk Influence in State High Courts.” Justice System Journal 29(1): 2446.Google Scholar
Szmer, John, and Ginn, Martha H. 2014. “Examining the Effects of Information, Attorney Capability, and Amicus Participation on U.S. Supreme Court Decision Making.” American Politics Research 42(3): 441-71.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Szmer, John J., Kaheny, Erin B., and Christensen, Robert K. 2014. “Taking a Dip in the Supreme Court Clerk Pool: Gender-Based Discrepancies in Clerk Selection.” Marquette Law Review 98: 261-88.Google Scholar
Taylor, Shelley E., Fiske, Susan T., Etcoff, Nancy L., and Ruderman, Audrey J. 1978. “Categorical and Contextual Bases of Person Memory and Stereotyping.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 36(7): 778.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Teles, Steven M. 2012. The Rise of the Conservative Legal Movement. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Thomsen, Danielle M., and Sanders, Bailey K. 2020. “Gender Differences in Legislator Responsiveness.” Perspectives on Politics 18(4): 1017-30.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vaughn, Ashley R., Taasoobshirazi, Gita, and Johnson, Marcus L. 2020. “Impostor Phenomenon and Motivation: Women in Higher Education.” Studies in Higher Education 45(4): 780-95.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vecci, Joseph, and Zelinsky, Tomas. 2019. “Behavioural Challenges of Minorities: Social Identity and Role Models.” PLoS One 14(7): e0220010.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wald, Patricia M. 1995. “The Rhetoric of Results and the Results of Rhetoric: Judicial Writings.” University of Chicago Law Review 62: 1371–419.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ware, Gilbert. 1984. William Hastie: grace under pressure. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Wasserberg, M.J. 2014. “Stereotype Threat Effects on African American Children in an Urban Elementary School.” Journal of Experimental Education 82(4): 502-17.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weiss, Catherine, and Melling, Louise. 1988. “The Legal Education of Twenty Women.” Stanford Law Review: 40(5): 1299–369.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weiss, Debra C. 2018. “Showing Anger Can Backfire for Female Lawyers, Studies Say; Law Prof Suggests ‘Gender Judo’ Response.” ABA, accessed November 16, 2020, https://www.abajournal.com/news/article/showing_anger_in_the_courtroom_can_backfire_for_women_lawyers_study_suggest.Google Scholar
Whaley, A.L. 2018. “Advances in Stereotype Threat Research on African Americans: Continuing Challenges to the Validity of its Role in the Achievement Gap.” Social Psychology of Education 21: 111-37. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11218-017-9415-9CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wilkins, David B. 1994. “Race, Ethics, and the First Amendment: Should a Black Lawyer Represent the Ku Klux Klan.” George Washington Law Review 63(6): 1030-70.Google Scholar
Williams, Margaret S. 2008. “Ambition, Gender, and the Judiciary.” Political Research Quarterly 61(1): 6878.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1. Marginal effects of white male versus other judges, US Courts of Appeals cases, 2008–2016. Note: N = 2906. Confidence intervals shown are for two-tailed tests (p < 0.05). Full results for these models appear in Table A1

Figure 1

Figure 2. Marginal effects of expected count of opinion length, US Courts of Appeals cases, 2008–2016. Note: N = 2906. Unconditional circuit and year fixed effects not shown. Confidence intervals shown are for two-tailed tests (p < 0.05). Full results shown in Table A2

Figure 2

Figure 3. Marginal effects of expected count of the total number of authorities cited in majority opinions, US Courts of Appeals cases, 2008–2016. Note: N = 2906. Unconditional circuit and year fixed effects not shown. Confidence intervals shown are for two-tailed tests (p < 0.05). Full results shown in Table A2

Figure 3

Figure 4. Marginal effects of expected count of the deep cites in majority opinions, US Courts of Appeals cases, 2008–2016. Note: N = 2906. Unconditional circuit and year fixed effects not shown. Confidence intervals shown are for two-tailed tests (p < 0.05). Full results shown in Table A2

Figure 4

Table A1 Models comparing white males to all other judges (negative binomial models with unconditional fixed effects)

Figure 5

Table A2 Full results for Figures 2–4 (negative binomial models with unconditional fixed effects)

Supplementary material: File

Moyer et al. supplementary material
Download undefined(File)
File 145 KB