Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-v9fdk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-04T21:11:43.766Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Hablando at home: Examining the interactional resources of a bilingual autistic child

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  30 October 2023

S. R. COHEN*
Affiliation:
Education Studies Department, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, USA
A. WISHARD GUERRA
Affiliation:
Education Studies Department, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, USA
J. MIGUEL
Affiliation:
Erikson Institute, Herr Research Center, Chicago, USA
K. BOTTEMA-BEUTEL
Affiliation:
Lynch School of Education and Human Development, Boston College, Boston, USA
G. OLIVEIRA
Affiliation:
Harvard Graduate School of Education, Cambridge, USA
*
Corresponding author: S. R. Cohen; Email: [email protected].
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Daily language interactions predict child outcomes. For multilingual families who rear neurodiverse children and who may be minoritized for their language use, a dearth of research examines families’ daily language interactions. Utilizing a language socialization framework and a case study methodology, 4,991 English and Spanish utterances from a 5-year old autistic child and his family were collected during naturally occurring interactions over 10 days. Utterances were analyzed for patterns of code-switching by speaker, activity setting, English or Spanish initial language, and code-switch function. Spanish was spoken in most activities. For reading, both languages were equally employed by the father. While participants used both languages across all activity settings, significant variations in code-switching type and function were observed by activity setting and speaker. We discuss implications for how home language resources can be integrated into autism interventions.

Type
Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Home language interactions are among the strongest predictors of young children’s language and cognitive development (Hoff et al., Reference Hoff, Core, Place, Rumiche, Señor and Parra2012). This is especially true for young children with developmental disabilities growing up in multilingual homes, a population on the rise (Castro & Artiles, Reference Castro and Artiles2021; Maenner et al., Reference Maenner, Shaw, Baio, Washington, Patrick, DiRienzo, Christensen, Wiggins, Pettygrove, Andrews, Lopez, Hudson, Baroud, Schwenk, White, Rosenberg, Lee, Harrington, Huston, Hewitt, Esler, Hall-Lande, Poynter, Hallas-Muchow, Constantino, Fitzgerald, Zahorodny, Shenouda, Daniels, Warren, Vehorn, Salinas, Durkin and Dietz2020). Strong evidence exists for bilingual families to use their multilingual interactional resources to support their children’s language development (Bialystok & Craik, Reference Bialystok and Craik2010; Marian & Shook, Reference Marian and Shook2012). Yet, children’s language background is not always reported, or investigated as an interacting factor, in developmental and intervention research (Byers-Heinlein et al., Reference Byers-Heinlein, Esposito, Winsler, Marian, Castro and Luk2019; Huerta et al., Reference Huerta, Cycyk, Sanford-Keller, Busch, Dolata, Moore, De Anda and Zuckerman2021), reinforcing a pervasive deficit perspective favoring middle-class, monolingual practices (Castro et al., Reference Castro, Gillanders, Prishker, Rodgriguez, Castro and Artiles2021; Kay-Raining Bird et al., Reference Kay-Raining Bird, Lamond and Holden2012). Cioé-Peña (Reference Cioé-Peña2020) found that Latinx parents internalize the deficit perspectives of providers, accepting intervention services in English only for their bilingual children despite initial resistance. Developmentally appropriate, linguistically diverse, and culturally responsive early interventions that support the language diversity, the educational expectations, and the cultural practices of bilingual families and their autistic childrenFootnote 1 are needed (Cohen & Miguel, Reference Cohen and Miguel2018; DuBay et al., Reference DuBay, Watson and Zhang2018; Stahmer et al., Reference Stahmer, Vejnoska, Iadarola, Straiton, Segovia, Luelmo, Morgan, Lee, Javed, Bronstein, Hochheimer, Cho, Aranbarri, Mandell, Hassrick, Smith and Kasari2019).

Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) is a neurocognitive disability linked to significant language and communication impairments (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). Research shows that early interventions hold promise for fostering autistic children’s development (Fuller & Kaiser, Reference Fuller and Kaiser2019; Landa, Reference Landa2018). Bilingual autistic children are significantly underrepresented in early intervention (EI) across the U.S. (Morrier & Gallagher, Reference Morrier and Gallagher2012). Few studies have utilized a culturally and linguistically responsive, asset-based approach to integrate autism interventions into the daily lives of culturally diverse families that build on their linguistic and cultural resources (Jegatheesan et al., Reference Jegatheesan, Miller and Fowler2010; Larson et al., Reference Larson, Cycyk, Carta, Hammer, Baralt, Uchikoshi, Gigi An and Wood2020). Some scholars have begun to investigate families’ daily routines and identify targeted opportunities for adapting culturally relevant interventions into immigrant families’ daily activities (Cohen, Miguel, & Wishard Guerra, Reference Cohen, Miguel and Wishard Guerra2020). Yet, these studies have not fully considered how language interactions may be critical, malleable factors that shape autism interventions and ultimately impact children’s language outcomes.

The following literature review will begin with a discussion of the theories of language use in multilingual communities. Then we describe how language has been studied and used in multilingual communities and how an analysis of code-switching can shed light on linguistic resources used by multilingual families. Finally, we review how autistic children develop language differently than non-autistic children, and the aims of the current study.

Language socialization theory in multilingual communities

Grounded in ethnographic methodology and informed by sociocultural theory, language socialization theory emphasizes the social, cultural, and cognitive processes of language development (Ochs & Schieffelin, Reference Ochs and Schieffelin2011). Routine interactions between children and their primary caregivers are the culturally specific contexts in which children are socialized (Garrett & Baquedano-López, Reference Garrett and Baquedano-López2002). Language socialization occurs through novices’ routine participation in semiotically mediated activities that integrate communicative environments (e.g., activities, artifacts, and technologies) with interlocutors’ beliefs about communication and language. Novice speakers are purposefully positioned in these communicative interactions to develop their semiotic repertoires and their language habits (Ochs & Schieffelin, Reference Ochs and Schieffelin2011).

Language socialization theory also allows for the integration of multiple languages to develop activity specific language habits (e.g., more frequent use of English in activities mediated by English print materials). Children who engage in translanguaging – integrating multiple languages into their language repertoire, and recognizing their hybrid identities and language ideologies – are agentic, creative, and cognitively flexible (McCabe et al., Reference McCabe, Bornstein, Wishard, Kuchirko, Páez, Tamis-LeMonda, Cates, Hirsh-Pasek, Melzi, Song, Golinkoff, Hoff and Mendelsohn2013; Song, Reference Song2019; Tönsing & Soto, Reference Tönsing and Soto2020; Wang, Reference Wang2019). Few studies have investigated bilingual autistic children’s experiences with code-switching practices nested within routine family activities (Yu, Reference Yu2016; Yu & Sterponi, Reference Yu and Sterponi2023). Yu’s (Reference Yu2016) work is one exception. She and her colleagues challenge assumptions that one language can be separated from another; parents deploy their full language repertoires when interacting with their autistic children. Her work recognizes that formulations of bilingualism as two distinct languages existing as separate cognitive entities is antiquated and does not align with families’ daily, bilingual experiences. For many bilingual families, language use is a dynamic experience, influenced both by macro socio-historical contexts and by the individuals with whom they speak within their micro contexts (Rogers et al., Reference Rogers, Niwa, Chung, Yip and Chae2021). Situated within a transnational context in which families have historically been oppressed by social policies driven by white supremacy and power, the current study builds on Yu’s work by exploring normative code-switching practices used within one family living on the border between the U.S. and Mexico, where English and Spanish are both commonly used. Home language practices will be analyzed to understand how code switching is used to communicate and to empower individuals (Rogers et al., Reference Rogers, Niwa, Chung, Yip and Chae2021)

Code-switching in multilingual communities

Code-switching is the act of alternating between two languages while concurrently using both languages in conversation (Reyes, Reference Reyes2004; Vu et al., Reference Vu, Bailey and Howes2010). From a language socialization perspective, code-switching serves as a contextual cue guiding the speaker’s complex understandings of local contexts (e.g., the language of the other interlocutors, the language setting – Ochs & Schieffelin, Reference Ochs and Schieffelin2011; Song, Reference Song2019; Zentella, Reference Zentella1997). Developmental age, language setting, interlocutors, and content of the discourse are known to shape the form and function of code-switching (Vu et al., Reference Vu, Bailey and Howes2010; Zentella, Reference Zentella1997). Younger children (under age 5) may use code-switching to bridge lexical gaps while older family members may use code-switching to achieve sociopragmatic functions (Vu et al., Reference Vu, Bailey and Howes2010). The way multilingual speakers switch between languages in the context of daily activities is a key component of implicit language socialization (Zentella, Reference Zentella1997).

Previous studies showed that children switched from Spanish to English when they engaged with researchers; they assumed that researchers were authority figures similar to teachers, and the expectation was to speak in English (Vu et al., Reference Vu, Bailey and Howes2010). When bilingual children from Puerto Rico encountered non-familiar adults, they were more likely to speak English until they were sure that the adult spoke Spanish (Zentella, Reference Zentella1997). In other studies, code-switching from Spanish to English was an acceptable practice in the United States given the assumption that Spanish speakers living in the U.S. understood some English (Ardila, Reference Ardila2005). When interlocutors integrate multiple languages in conversation using cross language strategies like code-switching, this language practice is a sign of linguistic control and cognitive flexibility for children with and without language disorders (Gonzalez-Barrero & Nadig, Reference Gonzalez-Barrero and Nadig2017; Kapantzoglou et al., Reference Kapantzoglou, Brown, Cycyk and Fergadiotis2021; Yow et al., Reference Yow, Tan and Flynn2018). Studies are beginning to show how bilingual autistic children who use both languages simultaneously have enhanced cognitive flexibility, strong executive function skills, and the ability to understand “false beliefs,” as compared to their monolingual autistic peers (Gonzalez-Barrero & Nadig, Reference Gonzalez-Barrero and Nadig2017; Peristeri et al., Reference Peristeri, Baldimtsi, Vogelzang, Tsimpli and Durrleman2021a). These cognitive skills have historically been seen as deficits among autistic individuals (Mostert-Kerckhoffs et al., Reference Mostert-Kerckhoffs, Staal, Houben and de Jonge2015). In fact, recent studies have shown that bilingual language exposure, in the form of enhanced language experiences, may compensate for the limited executive functioning skills that some autistic children exhibit (Peristeri et al., Reference Peristeri, Vogelzang and Tsimpli2021b).

Language development of autistic children

Echolalia, the repetition of words or sentences spoken by others, the absence or loss of language in the first two years of life, and the use of neologisms describe some of the language differences that characterize autistic children’s language (Tager-Flusberg, Reference Tager-Flusberg1999). Many autistic children also struggle with the sociopragmatic functions of language learning; the ability to understand nuanced and complex aspects of language that vary by context (Naigles & Tek, Reference Naigles and Tek2017). Like caregivers of non-autistic children, it is likely that caregivers of autistic children use code-switching as a cue to support their child in understanding language functions and social nuances within their cultural communities.

Qualitative studies have shown that autistic children, including multilingual autistic children, engage in a variety of sophisticated interactional processes across a range of situations (Angulo-Jimenez, Reference Angulo-Jimenez2020; Bottema-Beutel, et al., Reference Bottema-Beutel, Oliveira, Cohen and Miguel2020; Klein Reference Klein2021; Yu, Reference Yu2016). Some aspects of multilingual autistic children’s expressive language are similar to that of their monolingual peers (Gonzalez-Barrero & Nadig, Reference Gonzalez-Barrero and Nadig2017; Prévost & Tuller, Reference Prévost and Tuller2022; Siyambalapitiya et al., Reference Siyambalapitiya, Paynter, Nair, Reuterskiöld, Tucker and Trembath2021). Bilingual autistic children have stronger dominant language abilities in their native language as compared to monolingual autistic children (Hambly & Fombonne, Reference Hambly and Fombonne2012), suggesting that there is no advantage to speaking English only for children with communication challenges (Conner et al., Reference Conner, Baker and Allor2020; Prévost & Tuller, Reference Prévost and Tuller2022). In fact, recent studies have shown no negative impact of bilingual language environments on autistic children’s language development (Siyambalapitiya et al., Reference Siyambalapitiya, Paynter, Nair, Reuterskiöld, Tucker and Trembath2021; Zhou et al., Reference Zhou, Munson, Greenson, Hou, Rogers and Estes2019). Understanding how bilingual families strategically integrate their two languages with young autistic children -- for bridging lexical gaps, or for navigating the sociopragmatic functions of language in different activity settings -- can support more targeted language interventions to address children’s specific language needs.

Research aims

As new evidence shows the cognitive benefits of autistic children speaking multiple languages (Gonzalez-Barrero & Nadig, Reference Gonzalez-Barrero and Nadig2017; Prévost & Tuller, Reference Prévost and Tuller2022), it’s important to understand how children are engaged in bilingual language practices in their daily routines. This new knowledge can inform more precise and focused intervention approaches to capitalize on autistic bilingual children’s language strengths. In addition, new studies consistently call for ethnographic and sociolinguistic studies that examine the bilingual language development and code-switching practices of multilingual children (Wang, Reference Wang2019). This study addresses these significant research gaps by investigating the code-switching patterns and functions of a bilingual, immigrant family raising an autistic child. Using an exploratory, multivariate, case study design (not driven by a priori hypotheses) we examined the language, type, and function of code-switching. We addressed several research aims. The first was to describe the prevalence of code-switching within a bilingual family during multilingual interactions. Specifically, we examined the proportional presence and language of code-switching across activity settings and speakers. The second was to look within code-switching interactions to identify whether the language, the type and the function of the code-switch varied across activity settings and speakers. Informed by language socialization theory, we expected language, type, and function of code-switching to vary across activity settings and speakers.

Materials and methods

Researcher positionality

The lead author and the third author share a linguistic and ethnic background with study participants. They collected the data and developed a rapport with the family over the three-year study. The first author has clinical experience diagnosing bilingual autistic children. She has also worked as an Applied Behavior Analysis (ABA) therapist, a special education preschool teacher, and an advocate for bilingual families with neurodiverse children. The second author has lived in Central and South America, is fluent in Spanish, and has experience examining language as a cultural practice among immigrant families with young children. The third author has been a primary school teacher and an aide in bilingual and special education classrooms. The fourth author has worked with bilingual autistic children in a variety of educational settings, and the fifth author is a Latin American ethnographer who has worked with immigrant families for more than a decade. From these experiences we have learned that some bilingual families did not feel empowered to interact in their preferred language with their neurodiverse children. Existing literature upholds parent perceptions, noting practitioners and educators advise against the use of two languages (Hampton et al., Reference Hampton, Rabagliati, Sorace and Fletcher-Watson2017; Howard et al., Reference Howard, Gibson and Katsos2021; Trelles & Castro, Reference Trelles and Castro2019; Yu, Reference Yu2013). Parents shared that they were expected to use English even if they did not feel comfortable speaking it. Our analyses are informed by our experiences and are designed to link research to practice.

Research design

We used a Case Study Methodology to examine the daily language interactions of one bilingual family that included an autistic boy. Case studies are used to enhance our knowledge of complex human experiences within a specific context (Stake, Reference Stake2006). They provide detailed accounts of human behavior within the complex contexts in which participants live and learn (Merriam, Reference Merriam1998; Yin, Reference Yin2017). We implemented a single case study design to develop a more comprehensive understanding of how this family uses bilingual language practices to navigate daily routine activities, and support the child to be successful in school. This approach allows researchers to examine specific family language patterns, identify commonalities, and draw a single set of case conclusions (Eisenhardt, Reference Eisenhardt1991; Stake, Reference Stake2006; Yin, Reference Yin2017).

Participants

The family depicted in this study was part of a broader study of 38 Mexican heritage, Spanish speaking families raising at least one autistic child (Cohen & Miguel, Reference Cohen and Miguel2018). Families were recruited from two sources, a medical clinic near the U.S. Mexico border and a non-profit regional center. Recruitment packets were sent to Latino families rearing autistic children and interested parents were screened for eligibility requirements (i.e. availability, interest, age requirement). Once enrolled, participants could participate in individual interviews, focus group interviews, surveys, and daily video recordings of interactions. Prior to video recording, participants were informed of ethical considerations, including no denial of previous or future benefits, utilizing a secure online, password-protected, database to store data, applying pseudonyms to participants’ names, and ensuring researchers handling data received human research subjects training. The six families participating in video recording of daily activities were provided a video camera and they kept it for 10 days. Families completed IRB approved consent forms and were provided with camera training on the day we dropped off the video recorders. They were told they could halt filming and/or withdraw permission for videos and associated data at any time. This article examined one of the six families, specifically the code-switching language interactions between a bilingual child and his family.

The FernandezFootnote 2 family includes the target child, Herman, a five-year old autistic boy (diagnosed at 36 months), his 3-year-old sister Rocio, his 32-year-old mother Beatriz, a homemaker who was in the process of completing her Bachelor’s degree, and Herman’s father David, a 31-year-old merchant with a bachelor’s degree. All ages were reported at the time of data collection. The family’s annual household income was between $25,001-35,000. The family lived in a bicultural and bilingual border community along the U.S. - Mexico border and would regularly visit family in Mexico. The Fernandez children can be characterized as simultaneous bilinguals, having been exposed to both English and Spanish from birth. More specifically, Beatriz described their family’s home linguistic practices as Spanish dominant, with some English spoken by all family members throughout the day. Beatriz reported that she and Herman’s father, David, spoke more Spanish than English to Herman, and that Herman spoke equal amounts of English and Spanish at home. More recently, David has been reading to the children in English only.

Herman was receiving ABA intervention services in English approximately 15 hours per week at the time of data collection. He was also attending a state-funded preschool with an English dominant school-readiness, thematic curriculum. Given the area’s large Spanish-speaking community, bilingual preschool teacher aides were employed and helped to clarify curriculum content in Spanish as needed. No language designation was provided by the preschool. Prior to his autism diagnosis, Herman received four hours per week of Spanish behavioral intervention, at a child development center 15 minutes from their home, across the border, in Mexico. This intervention was parent initiated and implemented because of their proximity to the border and their connections to the clinic (Beatriz’s sister worked there). At 44-months, Herman received eight hours per week of ABA intervention in English at his home in the U.S. Beatriz described Herman as a hyperactive child who easily lost focus, and needed help to adapt to transitions (i.e., changes in his schedule) and curb maladaptive behaviors. He also required help with his fine and gross motor skills, he had difficulty maintaining and establishing gaze when spoken to, he spoke in the third person, and he had difficulty socializing and maintaining a conversation with peers. Beatriz noted that although Herman’s language production was clear and he was meeting or slightly exceeding his developmental milestones, he struggled to maintain conversations with peers. Beatriz was told that he did not require speech services, he had a good memory, and he was perceived as very affectionate with family and close relatives. Rocio, Herman’s younger sister, spoke mostly Spanish and regularly played with Herman. Beatriz cared for Rocio at home as she was not yet enrolled in school.

Procedures

To capture daily, routine interactions, the Fernandez family was given a digital video recorder and asked to film daily typical interactions over a 10-day period. Researchers described how to use the video recorder, and the types of interactions that should be recorded (e.g., play time, dinner time). At the end of 10 days a researcher collected the video recorder, the recorded footage, and invited Beatriz to share her experiences about data collection in a short audio-recorded interview. Beatriz reported confidence that the daily video recordings accurately captured their families’ naturally occurring routine activities. She explained that the recordings captured instances of teaching, conversations, and play. Reflecting on her teaching style she acknowledged learning how to teach Herman from the early intervention therapists, “cómo le refuerzan ciertas cosas o cómo le dirigen, cuando algo no está haciéndolo bien” (How they reinforce certain things or how they direct him, when he is not doing something right). Yet, Beatriz also encouraged Herman to play given most of his day included structured activities like school and therapy. When describing play time, she explained: “es como ese tiempo extra que él puede disfrutar ya jugar con su hermana” (It is like that extra time that he can enjoy and play with his sister). Beatriz reported an intentionality in Herman’s daily activities.

The Fernandez family collected 334 minutes across 20 digital video recordings. Video recordings were transcribed using a professional transcription service that specializes in transcribing English, Spanish, and bilingual interactions. Recorded interactions occurred between the mother, (Beatriz), the father (David), Herman (the autistic child), the younger sister (Rocio) and additional family members including Herman’s cousin, aunt, and uncle.

Data reduction

Transcripts were analyzed to identify code-switching excerpts, a phenomenon of interest noted during a previous conversational analysis study in which researchers were guided by a process of ‘unmotivated looking’ (Bottema-Beutel, et al., Reference Bottema-Beutel, Oliveira, Cohen and Miguel2020; ten Have, Reference ten Have, McHoul and Rapley2001). For this study we identified instances of code-switching – interlocutors switching between speaking Spanish and English and back to Spanish or English – in 14 of the 20 transcripts. These 14 transcripts were selected for further analysis to understand the characteristics of code-switching occurrences. The six transcripts that were excluded from the analysis included only Spanish, with no code-switching, and featured instances of pretend play, eating, and traveling between Beatriz, David, Herman and Rocio. Notably, all video recordings of reading or homework activities included code-switching.

Transcription

All transcripts featuring code-switching were segmented by speaker and by utterance, and were coded at the utterance level. An utterance was defined as a word, a phrase, or independent clause, or a 2 second pause or more in between vocalizations (Kanaya & Santiago, Reference Kanaya and Santiago2019; MacWhinney, Reference MacWhinney2000). This process was repeated for each of the 14 transcripts resulting in 4,991 utterances, with 9.5% (n = 476) featuring code-switching.

Coding for activity setting, speaker, and language

All codes were dichotomous, indicating the presence or absence of the phenomenon of interest. This allowed for proportional scores to be derived by dividing the coded utterances by total utterances (n= 4,991) or total code-switching utterances (n = 476).

Activity setting

To identify the activities in which code-switching occurred, authors read each transcript, watched the associated video recording and coded the overall activity based on what the child and parent were doing (e.g., pretend play, eating) separately.

Speaker

Each utterance was coded for the speaker. The target child Herman, his mother Beatriz, his father David, and his sister Rocio were the primary speakers; the other three family members who appeared infrequently were combined into one group for analytic purposes.

Coding for code-switching frequency, language, type, and functions

If an utterance included more than one language, the utterance was coded for the presence of code-switching (CS) (dichotomous code). All utterances coded for the presence of CS were further coded for the language of the CS, as either an English CS (from Spanish into English) or a Spanish CS (from English into Spanish). For example, an individual utterance: le estoy echando Ketchup ‘I am putting Ketchup on it’ would be coded for the presence of an English CS. A second example: Mother: De qué color vas a pintar el moño? ‘What color will you use for the bow?’ Child: Yellow.” In this instance the first utterance would be coded for presence of a Spanish CS, whereas the second would be coded for presence of an English CS.

Each occurrence of CS was categorized as one of two types: Code mix and code change. Code mix is a single word borrowing, primarily used to bridge lexical gaps (Vu et al., Reference Vu, Bailey and Howes2010). Types of code-mixing included descriptions of “evidence and context of word knowledge,” (Vu et al., Reference Vu, Bailey and Howes2010). Definitions of code mix types included noun or proper noun code mixing, where the speaker switches to English for one word: Water es en inglés en español es agua. ‘Water is in English, in Spanish it’s agua’. Code change functions included alternations between two languages, driven by underlying interactional and sociopragmatic purposes (Vu et al., Reference Vu, Bailey and Howes2010). Within code change, more specific codes were used to identify its function. For example, code change utterances were coded by topic shift (e.g., if the speaker shifted content topics), if speakers shifted roles (e.g., if the mother shifted from teaching a concept to nurturing the child), or if the CS was used to make requests or get someone’s attention. Clarification code changes were also further sub-categorized to better understand the code change function. For example, a restatement code was used if a speaker restated a concept in the new language. A subcategory of the clarification code included an emphasis code, used to emphasize a certain command or topic. The translation code was used when the speaker directly translated a statement (this usually occurred when the mother and the child were working on school work together). A routine code was used to code for poems, songs, or rhymes that were borrowed directly from English. In this code, the tune of the song was usually an indicator that the child borrowed the song from his classroom context.

To reach intercoder consensus (Cascio et al., Reference Cascio, Lee, Vauldrin and Freedman2019) on the CS language, type, and function, the first and third author coded each transcript together for language (e.g., English or Spanish) and for CS type (e.g., code mix or code change) and their subcategories. When there was ambiguity about a code, researchers reviewed the coding dictionary, discussed all of the possible alternative codes for that utterance, identified the most appropriate code based on the definition, and revised the coding dictionary to clarify coding definitions and add multiple examples from the transcripts for each code type and function. See Appendix A for detailed definitions and examples in the final coding dictionary.

Results

Video recordings yielded 4,991 utterances that were coded and analyzed for speaker, activity setting, language use, and code-switching features. Roughly half of the utterances were spoken by Beatriz (mother) (n = 2,332, 97% Spanish), a third by Herman (target child) (n = 1,529, 86% Spanish), and the remaining by Rocio (n = 460, 94% Spanish), David (father) (n = 233, 4% Spanish), and other relatives (e.g., cousin, aunt, and uncle) (n = 437, 99% Spanish).

Roughly half of the utterances were captured during pretend play (n = 2,560) in which speakers spoke mostly Spanish (n = 2496), followed by homework (n = 1,017) occurring mostly in Spanish (n = 936), reading (n = 740) where Spanish and English were equally used (Spanish = 367; English = 373), and traveling (n = 278) occurring primarily in Spanish (n = 265). Beatriz, Herman, and Rocio participated in all activities, while David participated in reading (97% of David’s utterances) and other relatives participated in pretend play (99.3% of all utterances spoken by other relatives in pretend play). David was the only speaker to use mostly English utterances during his interactions. Table 1 presents the patterns of code switching by speaker and within activity setting.

Table 1. Code-Switch (CS) presence and language

To further characterize the language of the Fernandez family, below are some examples of how Beatriz often prompted Herman and Rocio with open-ended questions related to their daily activities (i.e. “A que mas jugaron ayer platiquen me / What else did you play yesterday tell me,” “Que buscas? / What are you looking for?”). Herman would respond with one-word answers “B: Quieres un hermanito? / Do you want a little brother? H: No” or “B: Do you fight with your sister? H: Si / Yes.” There were other instances where Beatriz had to prompt Herman multiple times with a question prior to responding, as he would look away from his mother or focus on objects in his hands or around him. In other instances, Herman responded using multiple word sentences. For example, in the following instance his mother asked Herman about his best friend in school,

  1. (1) Beatriz: Melissa es tu mejor amiga? ‘Melissa is your best friend?’

  2. (2) Herman: Si y tu no! ‘Yes and not you!’

  3. (3) Beatriz: [Dramatic gasp] Y que me dijiste ayer de Melissa? ‘And what did you tell me yesterday about Melissa?’

  4. (4) Herman: Melissa me va (voy a) casar con ella ‘Melissa, I am going to marry her’

  5. (5) Beatriz: Te vas a casar con ella? ‘You’re going to marry her?’

  6. (6) Herman: Si ‘Yes’

  7. (7) Beatriz: Porque te vas a casar con Melissa, Herman? ‘Why are you going to marry Melissa, Herman?’

  8. (8) Herman: Porque es que se te va romper el corazon! ‘Because it is going to break your heart!’

In the scenario above, the whole family is driving. Beatriz and her kids have been engaging in conversation, reciting the months of the year in Spanish and English, and talking about their friendships. This conversation demonstrates Herman’s ability to produce expressive vocabulary and respond to cognitively demanding questioning from Beatriz in Spanish.

Aim 1: Nature and prevalence of code-switching

Presence and language of code-switching

Code-switching was analyzed for presence and language of the CS (into Spanish or into English). Overall, 9.5% of the utterances included some form of code-switching, mostly made up of English CS – a switch to English from a Spanish utterance (95% of all codeswitching). Code-switching was used at least once by all speakers and in all activity settings. Notably, David and other relatives did not use any Spanish CS. See Table 1.

Presence of code-switching by language, speaker, and activity settings

A univariate analysis of variance (ANOVA) was used to examine whether the presence of code-switching varied significantly by speaker, by activity setting, or whether there was an interaction between the two. One dependent variable, presence of code-switching, was included. Independent variables included speaker (Herman, Beatriz, Rocio, David, other relatives) and activity setting (pretend play, eating, reading, homework, traveling). IBM SPSS was used for the analysis, with N = 4,991 utterances included. While there were no main effects of presence of CS by speaker, F(4,4990) = 2.17, p = .07, or activity setting, F(4, 4990) = 1.24, p = .29, the results reflected a significant but weak interaction between speaker and activity setting, F(11, 4990) = 6.64, p < .001, η2= .01. See Table 2 for code-switching distributions. Beatriz, David and Rocio used code-switching most often during reading activities while Herman used code-switching least often during reading activities. Herman used code-switching most often during homework and traveling activities while Beatriz, Rocio, and David used code-switching least often during homework and traveling activities. For example, in a typical homework interaction between Beatriz and Herman, Beatriz spoke in Spanish to prompt Herman to answer the questions and Herman responded in English:

  1. (1) Beatriz: No, te estoy preguntando ¿de qué color son las bananas? ‘No, I am asking you, what color are the bananas?’

  2. (2) Herman: Yellow

  3. (3) Beatriz: Ah, ¿de qué color son las manzanas? ‘Ah, what color are the apples?’

  4. (4) Herman: Red

Table 2. Distribution of CS presence, language, and type by Activity Setting and Speaker

Note. CS Spanish and CS Change represent the proportion of time the CS was either Spanish or Change. The difference is attributed to either CS English or CS Mix.

Aim 2: Patterns of code-switching by type, function, language, speaker, and activity setting

Type and function of code-switching

Although all speakers across all activity settings used code mix and code change types of code-switching, code mix was the most common (78.4%) type. Code mix and code change were both most frequently observed with English CS (Spanish to English) (97.9% and 84.2% respectively). When Spanish CS (English to Spanish) was observed, it occurred most frequently during code change (66.7%). See Table 1. Code mix types included seven categories (See Appendix A). Noun code mixing (42.1%) and proper noun code mixing (36.9%) occurred frequently, while evaluation (1.6%), context specific (3.5%), proper article use (3.5%), and improper article use (1.1%) occurred less frequently. There were also uncategorizable code mix utterances (11.3%). Code change functions included 12 categories. Topic shift (23.3%) and routinized code changes (17.5%) occurred frequently. The remaining code change functions occurred less frequently: Narrative frame break (5.8%), mitigating request (1.9%), mitigating response (10.7%), attention attraction (8.7%), reading in English (1.9%), parallel speaker turn (5.8%), restatement, (10.7%), emphasis/command (3.9%), translation (7.8%), and uncategorizable (1.9%). English was the dominant language of the CS with the exception of attention attraction which occurred most frequently (67%) in Spanish.

Language, speaker, and activity settings of code-switching

Multivariate Analysis of Variance (MANOVA) was used to investigate variations in the language and type of CS by variations in speaker or activity setting. The largest categories of type of CS (code mix) and language of CS (English CS) were used as the reference group. Two dependent variables were included: proportion of CS Spanish and proportion of code change. Independent variables were speaker (Herman, Beatriz, Rocio, David, Other relative) and activity setting (pretend play, eating, reading, homework, traveling). Pillai’s criterion was used to assess main effects due to unequal sample sizes across speaker and activity setting IVs (Tabachnick & Fidell, Reference Tabachnick and Fidell2013). The combined DVs were significantly related to both speaker, F(8, 918) = 37.11, p < .01, and activity setting, F(8, 918) = 39.09, p <.01. The interaction term was also significant, F(16, 918) = 18.23, p < .01, with large effect sizes (partial η2 = .16, η2 = .25, and η2= .24, respectively). CS Spanish was almost exclusively used during reading and primarily by David and Rocio. For code change, Herman was least likely to use this type of CS during pretend play and reading, while David and Rocio used code change most frequently during reading. Results of this analysis are summarized in Table 3. For example, Herman is asking his father for help translating a word:

  1. (1) Herman: And they have - como se dice disfraz en Inglés? ‘And they have - how do you say costume in English?’

  2. (2) David: Costume

Table 3. Test of Activity Setting, Speaker, and Their Interaction

Note.

* p < .05,

** p < .01

Code mix by language, speaker, and activity settings

To examine the distribution of language by code mix type and code mix function, two crosstab analyses were conducted. A chi-square test of independence demonstrated no statistically significant differences between speaker and code mix types, X 2 (20, N = 331) = 16.95, p = .66, nor between CS language and code mix types, X 2 (5, N = 331) = 10.00, p = .08.

There were significant differences across activity setting and code mix types, X 2 (20, N = 331) = 155.33, p < .01. Code mixing utterances in pretend play were either noun code mixing (44.9%) or proper noun code mixing (44.9%). For example, Herman stated “Mamá a Rocío le dejaron la letter B de bear. [Mom they left Rocio the letter B of bear]” or “Me das más Play-Doh? [Can I have more Play-Doh?].” Noun code mixing was the dominant type of code mixing for eating (71%), homework (77%) and traveling (78.6%), while reading included proper noun code mixing (96.6%).

Code change function by language, speaker, and activity settings

There were significant differences across CS language and code change, X 2 (10, n = 101) = 32.24, p <.01. The most frequent functions of English code change were topic shift (22.4%), routinized (21.3%), mitigating response (12.9%), and restatement (12.9%). The most common Spanish code change functions were attention attraction (37.5%), topic shift (31.3%), translation (18.8%), and parallel speaker Turn (12.6%).

There were significant differences between speaker and code change function, X 2 (40, N = 101) = 128.52, p < .01. Herman’s utterances were coded as routinized (33%), topic shift (22.2%), mitigating response (16.7%) and narrative frame break (11.1%). Beatriz’s utterances included restatements (31.3%), topic shifts (31.3%), translations (15.6%) or parallel speaker turns (9.4%). Rocio’s utterances were attention attraction (60%), topic shift (20%), mitigating response (10%) and parallel speaker Turn (10%). David used code change for translation (75%).

There were also significant differences between activity setting and code change functions, X 2 (40, N = 101 = 113.98, p < .01. Within reading, the primary functions of code change were attention attraction (33.3%), topic shift (27.8%) and translation (22.2%), while within homework the code change function had more variance including topic shift (24%), restatements (26%), narrative frame break (12%), and mitigating response (12%).

Discussion

This study examined bilingual language practices, specifically how code-switching is used within daily activities among the Fernandez family, a Spanish dominant family living in a southern U.S bicultural and bilingual border community. Little is known about how bilingual families who raise autistic children utilize their linguistic and cultural resources to accomplish daily tasks including academic and therapy activities. Our study addresses this gap by documenting the language, types, and functions of code-switching used by a bilingual family to engage in daily activities with their autistic child. Framed within language socialization theory, findings suggest that the Fernandez family’s bilingual language practices were shaped by the context of the activity setting and the people participating in the activity.

Aim 1: Nature and prevalence of code-switching

Spanish was the dominant language used by all family members and the majority of code-switching across all speakers and activity settings was from Spanish into English. Reading and homework activities, both activities mediated by English print material, had the highest proportion of code-switching across all activity settings. It is possible that the English print materials signaled to the caregivers that English should be used at home to promote children’s literacy development. Immigrant caregivers have used homework routines to support children’s academic skill development and to reinforce socialization norms that align with families’ cultural backgrounds and immigration experiences (Alvarez, Reference Alvarez2014; Mangual Figueroa, Reference Mangual Figueroa2011). In our study, we identified interaction effects of speakers by activity setting pointing to important patterns of code-switching and language use in both reading and homework activities (see Table 1). During reading, David and Rocio, who were most frequently observed code-switching, were adhering to the language of the print material, mostly using a Spanish code mix for clarification. Additionally, Herman was least likely to code-switch during homework activities, suggesting his familiarity and preference to use English in this English mediated activity. The prevalence of English during reading and homework demonstrates how monolingual norms in academic settings can impose deficit language identities, even when speakers hold positive attitudes towards heritage language (Tseng, Reference Tseng2021). Other adult-directed activities like Herman’s behavioral interventions were also in English, further evidence of the influence of monolingual norms directing learning and therapy activities. The Fernandez family were likely internalizing monolingual expectations in academic contexts and they used reading and homework activities to socialize their child into expected academic language interactions to learn new content and to build proficiency in the language of school, English. Previous studies have shown how families strategically used bilingual youth as language brokers in supporting homework activities and in preparation for taking standardized tests in Spanish dominant homes (Alvarez, Reference Alvarez2014; Dorner et al., Reference Dorner, Orellana and Li-Grining2007). For family activities that were more child-centered and less likely to be mediated by English materials, like pretend play or eating, Spanish was the dominant language with fewer instances of code-switching (See Table 1).

The socio historical context of the U.S.-Mexico border community where the Fernandez family resided further shapes the daily language practices of people living within this community. The Fernandez family described seeking services and interacting with family and service providers in both Mexico and in the U.S. This required consistent use of both English and Spanish to enhance collaborative meaning making within their border community. Previous studies have shown how multilingual speakers, or community translanguagers, contextually modify their specific language practices to optimize shared meaning making and communication (Kim et al., Reference Kim, Dorner and Song2021; Worthy et al., Reference Worthy, Nuñez and Espinoza2016).

Aim 2: Patterns of code-switching by type, function, language, speaker, and activity setting

The majority of the code-switching across activities and speakers was defined as an English code mix (i.e., English noun or pronoun inserted into a Spanish utterance). Table 2 describes the presence of the non-dominant code-switching language (CS Spanish) and type (CS Change). Children would often insert an English word into a Spanish phrase: “Ah, podemos hacer un hot dog [oh, we can make a hot dog],” “Mami, ¿me prestas poquito blue? [Mom, can you give me some blue],” “Me das poquito white, mami, para que pueda tener un muñeco de nieve. [Can you give me some white, mom, so I can have a snowman],” “Tienes que escuchar bien lo que diga la teacher que hay que hacer okay [You have to listen well to what the teacher says, what to do, ok],” “Mami la cookie también es de muchos colores. [Mom the cookie is also of many colors].” Speakers were more likely to use these types of noun word insertions rather than completely shifting languages and continue speaking in the new language (in this case, English).

When activity settings were mediated by English print material (e.g., reading or worksheet activities), family members drew from a diverse range of code change functions to clarify language pragmatics, or to change speech direction (e.g., change topics, change speaker roles, respond to a question, to get the attention of the other interlocutor). For example, during one reading interaction the father stated, “They look at the clues and ask questions. Do you remember the-” Herman interrupts to ask, “Papi quién is this? [Daddy who is this?]”

These findings suggest the societally dominant language was an important linguistic resource for this family. Although the Fernandez family lived in a U.S.-Mexico border community where Spanish was frequently used in the broader community, Herman was attending an English-speaking preschool and he received early intervention services at home with English-speaking interventionists. Previous studies have used a Bakhtinian approach to identify English as the discourse of power and social capital (Bakhtin, Reference Bakhtin1983; Palmer, Reference Palmer2007). In our study it is clear that English influenced how the Fernandez family spoke during daily routine activities. We see family members engaging in bilingual interactions that support the home language but also thoughtfully integrating English to reinforce language learned outside the home.

David and Beatriz (Mom and Dad) used code change utterances (in English) during academic activities (See Table 2). For example, during reading, Spanish and English were equally used by David who also had the highest proportion of code change utterances. English was not his dominant language but David read books in English and code-switched into Spanish to clarify the meaning of English words or to directly translate from English to Spanish. In the following example David read the passage in English and then clarified as follows: “se quemó mira con el fuego; there’s fires [it burned, look with the fire; there’s fires]”.

Herman, Rocio, and their mother used Code Changing more frequently during homework and worksheet activities as the worksheets were all in English. Herman used Code Changing for routinized utterances that he likely learned in school (e.g., singing the “Clean up” song while cleaning up his Play Doh at home). Rocio used Code Changing for Attention Attraction, to get her mother’s attention when Beatriz was teaching Herman. Beatriz used Code Changing to restate ideas, to shift topics, and to clarify concepts to address her academic goals for Herman. During worksheet activities, a typical interaction facilitated by Beatriz follows:

  1. (1) Beatriz: Circle and color the smallest object in each box. ¿Qué tienes que hacer? ‘What do you have to do?’

  2. (2) Herman: Mira Mama ‘Look Mom’

  3. (3) Beatriz: ¿Qué tienes que hacer? ‘What do you have to do?’ The smallest (pause). What is the smallest?

  4. (4) Herman: Right here.

  5. (5) Beatriz: ¿Cuál? ‘Which one?’

  6. (6) Herman: Ese ‘This one’

These code change utterances appeared frequently and strategically during adult directed activities in which Beatriz and David were teaching or scaffolding learning activities for academic skill development; Herman and Rocio used Code Changing to showcase newly learned songs and language or to get a parent’s attention.

Herman and Rocio most frequently used code mix utterances during child centered activities like pretend play or eating activities when adults were not present (See Table 2). Code mixing included inserting an English word (e.g., hot dog, blue, dinosaur, pirate) into a Spanish utterance. Many times, one child would say the English word within a Spanish utterance and the other child would repeat the English word in a follow up Spanish utterance. It is possible that Herman and Rocio learned many vocabulary words at school or while watching TV in English, and they used these comforting home activities to share their new knowledge.

Research implications

Findings from this study point to the need for more research that investigates how multilingual families raising an autistic child strategically utilize their linguistic resources to navigate academic and therapeutic contexts constrained by monolingual norms. Future research should explore how autism interventionists’ language ideologies may prevent the implementation of best practices specifically suited for young multilingual autistic children. Researchers might also consider how interventionists gather information about their neurodiverse students’ home language contexts and how they understand the research, pedagogy and policies related to multilingual language learning. Therapists and teachers should recognize the diverse language resources that their multilingual autistic students bring to school to build key cognitive and social skills. Recent studies have identified that bilingual autistic children have more advanced cognitive flexibility than monolingual autistic children (Peristeri et al., Reference Peristeri, Baldimtsi, Vogelzang, Tsimpli and Durrleman2021a, Reference Peristeri, Vogelzang and Tsimpli2021b), yet more research is needed to further understand how teachers and therapists can build on these strengths. Additionally, some researchers may consider integrating a translanguaging framework within academic contexts and research methodologies to promote an understanding of language as a singular dynamic system (O’Connor et al., Reference O’Connor, Mortimer, Bartlett, de la Piedra, Rabelo Gomes, Mangual Figueroa, Novaro, Faulstich Orellana and Ullman2019), although some scholars have identified translanguaging as a threat to sustaining minoritized languages (Cenoz & Gorter, Reference Cenoz and Gorter2017).

Research Practice Partnerships built on long-term collaboration between researchers and practitioners have proven effective in early childhood interventions, especially for children in low-income families or who have been historically marginalized (Bassok et al., Reference Bassok, Markowitz and Morris2021; Brotman et al., Reference Brotman, Dawson-McClure, Rhule, Rosenblatt, Hamer, Kamboukos, Boyd, Mondesir, Chau, Lashua-Shriftman, Rodriguez, Barajas-Congzalez and Huang2021). Future research must collaborate with regional centers and school districts designing and implementing interventions for autistic children growing up in multilingual families.

Practical implications

As recent studies suggest, bilingual language practices may serve a range of interactive and relational functions. For example, bilingual practices have been shown to enhance autistic children’s executive functioning skills (Peristeri et al., Reference Peristeri, Baldimtsi, Vogelzang, Tsimpli and Durrleman2021a, Reference Peristeri, Vogelzang and Tsimpli2021b) and they may buffer against axes of oppression such as immigration status, socioeconomic status, and educational opportunities (García Coll et al., Reference García Coll, Lamberty, Jenkins, McAdoo, Crnic, Wasik and Vázquez García1996).

Capacity building interventions that promote early bilingual language development and balance opportunity gaps have been obscured by inequitable U.S. education policies that promote deficit oriented and subtractive language practices (e.g., reclassification policies for English Language learners with disabilities – Cabrera, Reference Cabrera2013; Valenzuela, Reference Valenzuela1999; Walker & Carta, Reference Walker and Carta2020). Our study shows that families use their multiple language systems purposefully to enact their daily routines and achieve their educational goals for their children. Strong evidence suggests that educators can mitigate inequities early by building upon family’s heritage language in academic settings and empowering students to actively negotiate their linguistic repertoires in their environments, negotiating, interpreting, and responding to systems of power (García et al., Reference García, Flores and Chu2011; Goodman & Tastanbek, Reference Goodman and Tastanbek2021; Menken et al., Reference Menken, Kleyn and Chae2012; Rogers et al., Reference Rogers, Niwa, Chung, Yip and Chae2021; Spencer, Reference Spencer, Budwig, Turiel and Zelazo2017).

Therapists and educators are encouraged to build upon families’ language choices and move beyond simplistic perspectives about language development (i.e., English only is best – Siyambalapitiya et al., Reference Siyambalapitiya, Paynter, Nair, Reuterskiöld, Tucker and Trembath2021). Educators should build their own capacities by listening and learning about how students use language at home and elevate children’s native language in school and other intervention settings. For example, young autistic children often have difficulty transitioning between routine activities or into new routines. Educators may incorporate words from the child’s native language into their classroom with songs and rhymes to scaffold smooth transitions. Studies have shown the language learning benefits of using songs and rhymes for English language learners to improve vocabulary and pronunciation (Saricoban & Metin, Reference Saricoban and Metin2000).

Given current study findings showing that interlocutors were influenced by their environments and by the language of print materials that mediated interactions, educators should intentionally include materials and experiences that build on home language resources. Educators who do not speak the students’ home language may label materials in the child’s classroom environment in multiple languages represented by the students to signal the value of children’s home language; they may integrate home language formulaic phrases into daily routines, use extra-linguistic materials (e.g., video, pictures) to support meaning making in the new language, and layer new language vocabulary over the native language to reinforce vocabulary development (Lucas et al., Reference Lucas, Villegas and Freedson-Gonzalez2008). Studies also call for educators to talk with students about the importance of valuing and honoring their home language, and empowering them to speak it (Young, Reference Young2014). It is devastating to see how children are placed in learning environments that strategically devalue their home language especially when families are engaging in purposeful, playful, language-rich bilingual interactions at home. These types of bilingual interactions have been shown to promote cognitive flexibility and long-term academic outcomes (Craig et al., Reference Craig, Zhang, Hensel and Quinn2009; McCabe et al., Reference McCabe, Bornstein, Wishard, Kuchirko, Páez, Tamis-LeMonda, Cates, Hirsh-Pasek, Melzi, Song, Golinkoff, Hoff and Mendelsohn2013; Peristeri et al., Reference Peristeri, Vogelzang and Tsimpli2021b).

Study limitations

There are several study limitations. First, these case study findings should not be generalized to broader populations of Mexican-heritage families. A strength of this case study design was to develop a more nuanced understanding of how one bilingual family engaged in language interactions with their autistic child. This in-depth analysis contributes to the scant research in this area but will not apply to all contexts. This study also did not utilize school documents or seek a separate autism diagnosis. The programs from which we recruited participants confirmed that children in our study were properly diagnosed. Authors also relied on parent reports of autism diagnosis, language use, intervention use, and school choice.

The video recording data collection was participant-led by design. Participants were responsible for filming instances of daily activities where language was present, the findings of this study are limited to what they captured on video. There may be language practices and language contexts or activity settings that researchers would have liked to capture (e.g., ABA intervention), but were not captured due to the study design. A strength of this design is how study participants felt empowered to capture what they felt was important to illustrate family language interactions. In an exit interview Beatriz confirmed having captured routine activities in which the target child engages daily, and in which he consistently behaves as he does when there is no video recording. Beatriz mentioned that Herman: “se portó como siempre se porta.” (he behaved how he typically behaves). Future research might consider following up participant-led video recordings with more in-depth interviews asking caregivers to reflect upon their language use (including why they used code-switching).

Conclusion

Drawing on language socialization theory, current study findings, and from interviews with Beatriz describing how she observed and learned from the English-speaking therapists engaging with her son during his early intervention program, the Fernandez family members were making purposeful and thoughtful language choices to support their autistic child’s language learning. Much of the adult-led code-switching interactions were aimed at teaching new language or complex concepts to Herman and Rocio. Much of the child-led code-switching interactions were responding to those adult teaching strategies with routinized language, attention seeking language, or to test out new English vocabulary. Current study findings show that code-switching is an adaptive language competence that promotes rich language interactions. For families rearing bilingual autistic children, seeing these bilingual home language practices as linguistic assets and understanding how, when, and with whom code switching is used, can support families and therapists to develop focused learning goals and activities that capitalize on children’s extra linguistic resources and address individual learning needs.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at http://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000923000600.

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to acknowledge the Yankelovich Center at UC San Diego for funding this research. We would also like to thank the study participants who took time out of their busy schedules to describe their experiences.

Competing interest

The authors declare none.

Footnotes

1 To honor the language preferences of disabled individuals, researchers used identity-first language throughout the manuscript. For further reference see Bottema-Beutel et al. (Reference Bottema-Beutel, Kapp, Lester, Sasson and Hand2021) and American Psychological Association (2020) publication manual guidelines.

2 Pseudonyms.

References

Alvarez, S. (2014). Translanguaging Tareas: Emergent bilingual youth as language brokers for homework in immigrant families. Language Arts, (91), 326339. https://www.jstor.org/stable/24575544Google Scholar
American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Washington, DC.Google Scholar
American Psychological Association. (2020). Publication Manual of the American Psychological Association (7th ed.).Google Scholar
Angulo-Jimenez, H. G. (2020). I was like ‘oh my God, los dos’”: Code-switching in conversations with a bilingual adult on the autism spectrum [Doctoral Dissertation]. University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign. http://hdl.handle.net/2142/107884Google Scholar
Ardila, A. (2005). Spanglish: An anglicized Spanish dialect. Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sciences, 27(1), 6081.10.1177/0739986304272358CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bakhtin, M. M. (1983). The dialogic imagination: Four essays (Vol. 1). University of Texas Press.Google Scholar
Bassok, D., Markowitz, A., & Morris, P. (2021). Introducing the Issue: Research Practice Partnerships to Strengthen Early Education. The Future of Children, 31(1), 319.10.1353/foc.2021.0001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E., & Craik, F. I. (2010). Cognitive and linguistic processing in the bilingual mind. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 19(1), 1923.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bottema-Beutel, K., Oliveira, G., Cohen, S. R., & Miguel, J. (2020) Question-response-evaluation sequences in the home interactions of a bilingual child with autism spectrum disorder. Int J Lang Commun Disord. 55(2), 216230. https://doi.org/10.1111/1460-6984.12513. Epub 2019 Nov 6. PMID: 31696610.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bottema-Beutel, K., Kapp, S. K., Lester, J. N., Sasson, N. J., & Hand, B. N. (2021). Avoiding ableist language: Suggestions for autism researchers. Autism in adulthood, 3(1), 1829. http://doi.org/10.1089/aut.2020.0014CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brotman, L., Dawson-McClure, S., Rhule, D., Rosenblatt, K., Hamer, K., Kamboukos, D., Boyd, M., Mondesir, M., Chau, I., Lashua-Shriftman, E., Rodriguez, V., Barajas-Congzalez, R. G., & Huang, K. (2021). Scaling early childhood evidence-based interventions through RPPs. The Future of Children, 31(1), 5774.10.1353/foc.2021.0002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Byers-Heinlein, K., Esposito, A. G., Winsler, A., Marian, V., Castro, D. C., & Luk, G. (2019). The case for measuring and reporting bilingualism in developmental research. Collabra: Psychology, 5 (1): 37. https://doi.org/10.1525/collabra.233CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cabrera, N. J. (2013). Positive development of minority children and commentaries. Social Policy Report, 27(2), 130.10.1002/j.2379-3988.2013.tb00075.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cascio, M. A., Lee, E., Vauldrin, N., & Freedman, D. A. (2019). A team-based approach to open-coding: Considerations for creating intercoder consensus. Field Methods, 31, I16130.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Castro, D., & Artiles, A. J. (Eds.). (2021). Language, Learning, and Disability in the Education of Young Bilingual Children. Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Castro, D., Gillanders, C., Prishker, N., & Rodgriguez, R. (2021). A sociocultural, integrative, and interdisciplinary perspective on the development and education of young bilingual children with disabilities. In Castro, D. & Artiles, A. (Eds) Language, Learning, and Disability in the Education of Young Bilingual Children. Multilingual Matters.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cenoz, J., & Gorter, D. (2017). Minority languages and sustainable translanguaging: Threat or opportunity? Journal of Multilingual and Multicultural Development, 38(10), 901912. DOI: 10.1080/01434632.2017.1284855CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cioé-Peña, M. (2020). Planning inclusion: The need to formalize parental participation in individual education plans (and meetings), The Educational Forum, 84(4), 377390. DOI: 10.1080/00131725.2020.1812970CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cohen, S. R., Miguel, J., & Wishard Guerra, A. (2020). Child-rearing routines among Mexican-heritage children with autism spectrum disorder. Autism, 24(1), 8094. https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361319849244CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohen, S., & Miguel, J. (2018). Child-rearing routines among Mexican-heritage children with autism spectrum disorder. Autism, 24(1), 8094. https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361319849244CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Conner, C., Baker, D. L., & Allor, J. H. (2020). Multiple language exposure for children with autism spectrum disorder from culturally and linguistically diverse communities. Bilingual Research Journal, 118.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Craig, H. K., Zhang, L., Hensel, S. L., & Quinn, E. J. (2009). African American English–speaking students: An examination of the relationship between dialect shifting and reading outcomes. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dorner, L. M., Orellana, M. F., & Li-Grining, C. P. (2007). “I helped my mom,” and it helped me: Translating the skills of language brokers into improved standardized test scores. American Journal of Education, 113(3), 451478. https://doi.org/10.1086/512740CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DuBay, M., Watson, L. R., & Zhang, W. (2018). In search of culturally appropriate autism interventions: Perspectives of Latino caregivers. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 48(5), 16231639.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eisenhardt, K. M. (1991). Better stories and better constructs: The case for rigor and comparative logic. Academy of Management Review, 16(3), 620627. doi: 10.2307/258921.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fuller, E. A., & Kaiser, A. P. (2019). The effects of early intervention on social communication outcomes for children with autism spectrum disorder: A meta-analysis. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 1-18. 10.1007/s10803-019-03927-zGoogle Scholar
García, O., Flores, N., & Chu, H. (2011). Extending bilingualism in U.S. secondary education: New variations. International Multilingual Research Journal, 5(1), 118. DOI: 10.1080/19313152.2011.539486CrossRefGoogle Scholar
García Coll, C., Lamberty, G., Jenkins, R., McAdoo, H. P., Crnic, K., Wasik, B. H., & Vázquez García, H. (1996). An integrative model for the study of developmental competencies in minority children. Child Development, 67(5), 18911914.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Garrett, P. B., & Baquedano-López, P. (2002). Language socialization: Reproduction and continuity, transformation and change. Annual review of anthropology, 31(1), 339361.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gonzalez-Barrero, A. M., & Nadig, A. (2017). Verbal fluency in bilingual children with Autism Spectrum Disorders. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 7 ( 3-4), 460475.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goodman, B., & Tastanbek, S. (2021). Making the shift from a code-switching to a translanguaging lens in English language teacher education. TESOL Journal, 55, 2953. https://doi.org/10.1002/tesq.571CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hambly, C., & Fombonne, E. (2012). The impact of bilingual environments on language development in children with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 42(7), 13421352.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hampton, S., Rabagliati, H., Sorace, A., & Fletcher-Watson, S. (2017). Autism and bilingualism: A qualitative interview study of parents’ perspectives and experiences. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 60(2), 435446.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoff, E., Core, C., Place, S., Rumiche, R., Señor, M., & Parra, M. (2012). Dual language exposure and early bilingual development. Journal of child language, 39(1), 127.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Howard, K., Gibson, J., & Katsos, N. (2021). Parental perceptions and decisions regarding maintaining bilingualism in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 51(1), 179192. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-020-04528-xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huerta, L., Cycyk, L. M., Sanford-Keller, H., Busch, A., Dolata, J., Moore, H., De Anda, S., & Zuckerman, K. A. (2021). Retrospective review of communication evaluation of young Latinx children. Journal of Early Intervention, 119.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jegatheesan, B., Miller, P. J., & Fowler, S. A. (2010). Autism from a religious perspective: A study of parental beliefs in South Asian Muslim immigrant families. Focus on Autism and Other Developmental Disabilities, 25(2), 98109.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kanaya, T., & Santiago, M. (2019). Mother-child storytelling patterns among first-generation Latino dyads: Implications for improving home-school engagement in early literacy skills. Journal of Latinos and Education, 19.Google Scholar
Kapantzoglou, M., Brown, J. E., Cycyk, L. M., & Fergadiotis, G. (2021). Code-switching and language proficiency in bilingual children with and without developmental language disorder. Journal of Speech, Language & Hearing Research, 64(5), 16051620. https://doi.org/10.1044/2020_JSLHR-20-00182CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kay-Raining Bird, E., Lamond, E., & Holden, J. (2012). Survey of bilingualism in autism spectrum disorders. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 47(1), 5264.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kim, S., Dorner, L. M., & Song, K. H. (2021). Conceptualizing community translanguaging through a family literacy project. International Multilingual Research Journal. DOI: 10.1080/19313152.2021.1889112CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klein, W. (2021). Managing trouble spots in conversation: Other-initiated repair elicitations produced by a bilingual youth with autism. Pragmatics, 31(2), 225249. https://doi.org/10.1075/prag.19042.kleCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Landa, R. J. (2018). Efficacy of early interventions for infants and young children with, and at risk for, autism spectrum disorders. International Review of Psychiatry, 30(1), 2539. https://doi.org/10.1080/09540261.2018.1432574CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Larson, A. L., Cycyk, L. M., Carta, J. J., Hammer, C. S., Baralt, M., Uchikoshi, Y., Gigi An, Z., & Wood, C. (2020). A systematic review of language-focused interventions for young children from culturally and linguistically diverse backgrounds. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 50, 157178.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lucas, T., Villegas, A. M., & Freedson-Gonzalez, M. (2008). Linguistically responsive teacher education: Preparing classroom teachers to teach English language learners. Journal of Teacher Education, 59(4), 361373.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MacWhinney, B. (2000). The CHILDES project: The database (Vol. 2). Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Maenner, M. J., Shaw, K. A., Baio, J., EdS1, Washington, A., Patrick, M., DiRienzo, M., Christensen, D. L., Wiggins, L. D., Pettygrove, S., Andrews, J. G., Lopez, M., Hudson, A., Baroud, T., Schwenk, Y., White, T., Rosenberg, C. R., Lee, L.C., Harrington, R. A., Huston, M., Hewitt, A., PhD-7, Esler, A., Hall-Lande, J., Poynter, J. N., Hallas-Muchow, L., Constantino, J. N., Fitzgerald, R. T., Zahorodny, W., Shenouda, J., Daniels, J. L., Warren, Z., Vehorn, A., Salinas, A., Durkin, M. S., & Dietz, P. M. (2020). Prevalence of Autism Spectrum Disorder Among Children Aged 8 Years - Autism and Developmental Disabilities Monitoring Network, 11 Sites, United States, 2016. Morbidity and mortality weekly report. Surveillance summaries (Washington, D.C.: 2002), 69(4), 112. https://doi.org/10.15585/mmwr.ss6904a1Google Scholar
Mangual Figueroa, A. (2011). Citizenship and education in the homework completion routine. Anthropology & Education Quarterly, 42(3), 263280. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1548-1492.2011.01131.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marian, V., & Shook, A. (2012). The cognitive benefits of being bilingual. Cerebrum: The Dana Forum on Brain Science, 13.Google ScholarPubMed
McCabe, A., Bornstein, M. H., Wishard, A., Kuchirko, Y., Páez, M., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Cates, C., Hirsh-Pasek, K., Melzi, G., Song, L., Golinkoff, R., Hoff, E., & Mendelsohn, A. (2013). Multilingual Children: Beyond Myths and Toward Best Practices and commentaries. Social Policy Report, 27(4), 137.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Menken, K., Kleyn, T., & Chae, N. (2012). Spotlight on “long-term English language learners”: Characteristics and prior schooling experiences of an invisible population. International Multilingual Research Journal, 6(2), 121142, DOI: 10.1080/19313152.2012.665822CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Merriam, S. B. (1998) Qualitative Research and Case Study Applications in Education. Jossey-Bass Publishers, San Francisco.Google Scholar
Morrier, M. J., & Gallagher, P. A. (2012). Racial disparities in preschool special education eligibility for five southern states. The Journal of Special Education, 46(3), 152169.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mostert-Kerckhoffs, M. A. L., Staal, W. G., Houben, R. H., & de Jonge, M. (2015). Stop and change: Inhibition and flexibility skills are related to repetitive behavior in children and young adults with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 45(10), 31483158. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-015-2473-yCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Naigles, L. R., & Tek, S. (2017). ‘Form is easy, meaning is hard’ revisited:(re) characterizing the strengths and weaknesses of language in children with autism spectrum disorder. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 8(4), e1438.Google ScholarPubMed
Ochs, E., & Schieffelin, B. B. (2011). The theory of language socialization. The handbook of language socialization, 71(1), 111.Google Scholar
O’Connor, B., Mortimer, K., Bartlett, L., de la Piedra, M., Rabelo Gomes, A., Mangual Figueroa, A., Novaro, G., Faulstich Orellana, M., & Ullman, C. (2019). Cruzar fronteras em espaços acadêmicos: Transgressing “the limits of translanguaging.” Applied Linguistics Review. https://doi.org/10.1515/applirev-2019-0003CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Palmer, D. (2007). A dual immersion strand programme in California: Carrying out the promise of dual language education in an English-dominant context. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 10(6), 752768.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peristeri, E., Baldimtsi, E., Vogelzang, M., Tsimpli, I., & Durrleman, S. (2021a). The cognitive benefits of bilingualism in autism spectrum disorder: Is theory of mind boosted and by which underlying factors? Autism. DOI: 10.1002/aur.2542CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peristeri, E., Vogelzang, M., & Tsimpli, I. (2021b). Bilingualism effects on the cognitive flexibility of autistic children: Evidence from verbal dual-task paradigms. Neurobiology of Language, (2), 558585. https://doi.org/10.1162/nol_a_00055CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Prévost, P., & Tuller, L. (2022). Bilingual language development in autism. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 12(1), 132. https://doi.org/10.1075/lab.21018.preCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reyes, I. (2004). Functions of code switching in schoolchildren’s conversations. Bilingual Research Journal, 28(1), 7798.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rogers, L. O., Niwa, E. Y., Chung, K., Yip, T., & Chae, D. (2021). M(ai)cro: Centering the macrosystem in human development. Human Development, 65, 270292. DOI: 10.1159/000519630CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Saricoban, A., & Metin, E. (2000). Songs, verse, and games for teaching grammar. The Internet TESL Journal, 6(10), 17.Google Scholar
Siyambalapitiya, S., Paynter, J., Nair, V. K., Reuterskiöld, C., Tucker, M., & Trembath, D. (2021). Longitudinal Social and Communication Outcomes in Children with Autism Raised in Bi/Multilingual Environments. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 110. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-021-04940-xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Song, J. (2019). Language socialization and code-switching: A case study of a Korean–English bilingual child in a Korean transnational family. International journal of bilingual education and bilingualism, 22(2), 91106. https://doi.org/10.1080/13670050.2016.1231165CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spencer, M. B. (2017). Privilege and critical race perspectives’ intersectional contributions to a systems theory of human development. In Budwig, N., Turiel, E., & Zelazo, P. D. (Eds.), New perspectives on human development (pp. 287312). Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stahmer, A. C., Vejnoska, S., Iadarola, S., Straiton, D., Segovia, F. R., Luelmo, P., Morgan, E. H., Lee, H. S., Javed, A., Bronstein, B., Hochheimer, S., Cho, E., Aranbarri, A., Mandell, D., Hassrick, E. M., Smith, T., & Kasari, C. (2019). Caregiver Voices: Cross-Cultural Input on Improving Access to Autism ServicesJournal of Racial and Ethnic Health Disparities6(4), 752773. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40615-019-00575-yCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stake, R. E. (2006). Multiple case study analysis. Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2013). Using multivariate statistics: International edition. Pearson.Google Scholar
Tager-Flusberg, H. (1999). The challenge of studying language development in children with autism. In Methods for studying language production (pp. 317336). Psychology Press.Google Scholar
ten Have, P. (2001). Applied conversation analysis. In McHoul, A. & Rapley, M. (Eds.), How to analyze talk in institutional settings: A casebook of methods (p. 311). Continuum.Google Scholar
Tönsing, K. M., & Soto, G. (2020). Multilingualism and augmentative and alternative communication: Examining language ideology and resulting practices. Augmentative and Alternative Communication, 36(3), 190201. https://doi.org/10.1080/07434618.2020.1811761CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Trelles, M. P., & Castro, K. (2019). Bilingualism in autism spectrum disorder: Finding meaning in translation. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 58(11), 10351037. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2019.05.027CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tseng, A. (2021). ‘Qué barbaridad, son latinos y deberían saber español primero’: Language Ideology, Agency, and Heritage Language Insecurity across Immigrant Generations. Applied Linguistics, 42(1), 113135.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Valenzuela, A. (1999). Subtractive schooling: Issues of caring in education of US-Mexican youth. State University of New York Press.Google Scholar
Vu, J. A., Bailey, A. L., & Howes, C. (2010). Early cases of code-switching in Mexican-heritage children: Linguistic and sociopragmatic considerations. Bilingual Research Journal, 33(2), 200219.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Walker, D., & Carta, J. J. (2020). Intervention research to improve language-learning opportunities and address the inequities of the word gap.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wang, W. (2019). Code-switching and its role in language socialization, International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 22:7, 787800, DOI: 10.1080/13670050.2017.1313809.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Worthy, J., Nuñez, I., & Espinoza, K. (2016). “Wow, I get to choose now!” Bilingualism and biliteracy development from childhood to young adulthood. Bilingual Research Journal, 39(1), 2034. DOI: 10.1080/15235882.2016.1139518CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yin, R. K. (2017). Case study research and applications: Design and methods. Sage Publications.Google Scholar
Young, A. S. (2014). Unpacking teachers’ language ideologies: Attitudes, beliefs, and practiced language policies in schools in Alsace, France. Language Awareness, 23(1-2), 157171. DOI: 10.1080/09658416.2013.863902CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yow, W., Tan, J., & Flynn, S. (2018). Code-switching as a marker of linguistic competence in bilingual children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 21(5), 10751090. DOI: 10.1017/S1366728917000335CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yu, B. (2013). Issues in bilingualism and heritage language maintenance: Perspectives of minority-language mothers of children with autism spectrum disorders. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 22, 1024.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yu, B. (2016). Code-switching as a communicative resource within routine, bilingual family interactions for a child on the autism spectrum. Perspectives of the ASHA Special Interest Groups, 1(14), 1728. https://doi.org/10.1044/persp1.SIG14.17CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yu, B., & Sterponi, L. (2023). Toward neurodiversity: How conversation analysis can contribute to a new approach to social communication assessment, Language, Speech and Hearing Services in Schools, (54) 2741. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1044/2022_LSHSS-22-00041CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zentella, A. C. (1997). Growing up bilingual: Puerto Rican children in New York. Wiley-Blackwell.Google Scholar
Zhou, V., Munson, J. A., Greenson, J., Hou, Y., Rogers, S., & Estes, A. (2019). An exploratory longitudinal study of social language outcomes in children with autism in bilingual home environments. Autism, 23, (2), 394404. https://journals.sagepub.com/doi/10.1177/1362361317743251CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Code-Switch (CS) presence and language

Figure 1

Table 2. Distribution of CS presence, language, and type by Activity Setting and Speaker

Figure 2

Table 3. Test of Activity Setting, Speaker, and Their Interaction

Supplementary material: File

Cohen et al. supplementary material
Download undefined(File)
File 17.3 KB