Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-dsjbd Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-24T20:10:23.473Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Alcohol use polygenic risk score, social support, and alcohol use among European American and African American adults

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 October 2023

Jinni Su*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Arizona State University, Tempe, AZ, USA
Sally I-Chun Kuo
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Rutgers University, New Brunswick, NJ, USA
Fazil Aliev
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Rutgers University, New Brunswick, NJ, USA
Jill A. Rabinowitz
Affiliation:
Bloomberg School of Public Health, Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, MD, USA
Belal Jamil
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Arizona State University, Tempe, AZ, USA
Grace Chan
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Connecticut, Farmington, CT, USA Department of Psychiatry, University of Iowa, Iowa City, IA, USA
Howard J. Edenberg
Affiliation:
Department of Biochemistry and Molecular Biology, Indiana University, Indianapolis, IN, USA
Meredith Francis
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Washington University, St. Louis, MO, USA
Victor Hesselbrock
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Connecticut, Farmington, CT, USA
Chella Kamarajan
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, State University of New York Downstate Medical Center, Brooklyn, USA
Sivan Kinreich
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, State University of New York Downstate Medical Center, Brooklyn, USA
John Kramer
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Iowa, Iowa City, IA, USA
Donbing Lai
Affiliation:
Department of Biochemistry and Molecular Biology, Indiana University, Indianapolis, IN, USA
Vivia McCutcheon
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Washington University, St. Louis, MO, USA
Jacquelyn Meyers
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, State University of New York Downstate Medical Center, Brooklyn, USA
Ashwini Pandey
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, State University of New York Downstate Medical Center, Brooklyn, USA
Gayathri Pandey
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, State University of New York Downstate Medical Center, Brooklyn, USA
Martin H. Plawecki
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Indiana University, Bloomington, IN, USA
Marc Schuckit
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, CA, USA
Jay Tischfield
Affiliation:
Department of Genetics, Rutgers University, New Brunswick, NJ, USA
Danielle M. Dick
Affiliation:
Rutgers Addiction Research Center, Rutgers University, New Brunswick, NJ, USA
*
Corresponding author: J. Su; Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Alcohol use is influenced by genetic and environmental factors. We examined the interactive effects between genome-wide polygenic risk scores for alcohol use (alc-PRS) and social support in relation to alcohol use among European American (EA) and African American (AA) adults across sex and developmental stages (emerging adulthood, young adulthood, and middle adulthood). Data were drawn from 4,011 EA and 1,274 AA adults from the Collaborative Study on the Genetics of Alcoholism who were between ages 18–65 and had ever used alcohol. Participants completed the Semi-Structured Assessment for the Genetics of Alcoholism and provided saliva or blood samples for genotyping. Results indicated that social support from friends, but not family, moderated the association between alc-PRS and alcohol use among EAs and AAs (only in middle adulthood for AAs); alc-PRS was associated with higher levels of alcohol use when friend support was low, but not when friend support was high. Associations were similar across sex but differed across developmental stages. Findings support the important role of social support from friends in buffering genetic risk for alcohol use among EA and AA adults and highlight the need to consider developmental changes in the role of social support in relation to alcohol use.

Type
Regular Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press

In the U.S., alcohol use is common among adults and is associated with a number of negative social and health consequences, representing a significant public health concern (Grant et al., Reference Grant, Goldstein, Saha, Chou, Jung, Zhang, Pickering, Ruan, Smith, Huang and Hasin2015). Genetics play an important role in alcohol use outcomes, accounting for approximately 50% of the variance in alcohol use disorders (Verhulst et al., Reference Verhulst, Neale and Kendler2015). Genetic influences vary as a function of environmental experiences, a phenomenon referred to as gene-environment interaction (G×E) (Plomin et al., Reference Plomin, DeFries and Loehlin1977). That is, environmental factors can exacerbate or attenuate genetic influences on a given behavior. For example, environments that provide more social opportunity for or trigger alcohol use (e.g., affiliation with peers who use alcohol and substances, stressful life events) can exacerbate the effect of genetic risk for alcohol use, whereas environments that provide more social control against alcohol use (e.g., parental monitoring during adolescence) can attenuate genetic effects (Dick & Kendler, Reference Dick and Kendler2012). Understanding which environmental factors reduce alcohol use among those at greater genetic risk for alcohol use can inform prevention and intervention efforts aimed at mitigating risk for alcohol use and related negative sequelae.

Despite the fact that alcohol use is a health concern for all racial/ethnic groups in the United States, G×E research has primarily focused on populations of European ancestry. However, from a cultural genomics perspective (Causadias & Korous, Reference Causadias and Korous2018), genetically informed pathways of risk may vary across populations due to differences in genetic, environmental, and cultural factors and the complex interplay among them (Peterson et al., Reference Peterson, Kuchenbaecker, Walters, Chen, Popejoy, Periyasamy, Lam, Iyegbe, Strawbridge, Brick, Carey, Martin, Meyers, Su, Chen, Edwards, Kalungi, Koen, Majara, Schwarz and Duncan2019). G×E effects in European American samples may not be generalizable to non-European populations (Su et al., Reference Su, Kuo, Meyers, Guy and Dick2018). Compared to European Americans (EA), African Americans (AA) on average consume lower levels of alcohol, but experience similar or higher levels of negative social and health consequences related to alcohol use (Mulia et al., Reference Mulia, Ye, Greenfield and Zemore2009; Zapolski et al., Reference Zapolski, Pedersen, McCarthy and Smith2014). AAs are more likely to have a lower socioeconomic status and experience stressful life events, such as racial discrimination (Williams et al., Reference Williams, Mohammed, Leavell and Collins2010), that are associated with increased alcohol use (Gilbert & Zemore, Reference Gilbert and Zemore2016). There are also important differences in genetic diversity, allele frequencies, and linkage disequilibrium patterns between EAs and AAs (Campbell & Tishkoff, Reference Campbell and Tishkoff2008; Gabriel et al., Reference Gabriel, Schaffner, Nguyen, Moore, Roy, Blumenstiel, Higgins, DeFelice, Lochner, Faggart, Liu-Cordero, Rotimi, Adeyemo, Cooper, Ward, Lander, Daly and Altshuler2002; Peterson et al., Reference Peterson, Kuchenbaecker, Walters, Chen, Popejoy, Periyasamy, Lam, Iyegbe, Strawbridge, Brick, Carey, Martin, Meyers, Su, Chen, Edwards, Kalungi, Koen, Majara, Schwarz and Duncan2019; Rosenberg et al., Reference Rosenberg, Huang, Jewett, Szpiech, Jankovic and Boehnke2010), potentially contributing to differences in the effects of specific genetic variants on phenotypes such as alcohol use. Taken together, G×E effects on alcohol use outcomes may vary across racial/ethnic groups; thus, it is important to study G×E processes in diverse populations to better understand pathways of risk and resilience related to alcohol use outcomes (Peterson et al., Reference Peterson, Kuchenbaecker, Walters, Chen, Popejoy, Periyasamy, Lam, Iyegbe, Strawbridge, Brick, Carey, Martin, Meyers, Su, Chen, Edwards, Kalungi, Koen, Majara, Schwarz and Duncan2019).

Many G×E studies on alcohol use have used twin and family designs, and G×E studies with measured genotypes have traditionally focused on candidate genes or a single genetic polymorphism (Dick & Kendler, Reference Dick and Kendler2012). However, candidate G×E studies have been criticized for inconsistent results, which is likely due to the existence of publication bias, low statistical power, and a high false discovery rate (Dick et al., Reference Dick, Agrawal, Keller, Adkins, Aliev, Monroe, Hewitt, Kendler and Sher2015; Duncan & Keller, Reference Duncan and Keller2011). Complex behaviors like alcohol use are polygenic, influenced by many genes of small effect sizes (Liu et al., Reference Liu, Jiang, Wedow, Li, Brazel, Chen, Datta, Davila-Velderrain, McGuire, Tian, Zhan, Choquet, Docherty, Faul, Foerster, Fritsche, Gabrielsen, Gordon and Vrieze2019; Plomin et al., Reference Plomin, Haworth and Davis2009). In recent years, there have been rapid advances in large scale genome-wide association studies (GWAS) to understand the complex genetic architecture underlying alcohol-related phenotypes (Gelernter et al., Reference Gelernter, Sun, Polimanti, Pietrzak, Levey, Lu, Hu, Li, Radhakrishnan, Aslan, Cheung, Li, Rajeevan, Sayward, Harrington, Chen, Cho, Honerlaw and Lencz2019; Kranzler et al., Reference Kranzler, Zhou, Kember, Vickers Smith, Justice, Damrauer, Tsao, Klarin, Baras, Reid, Overton, Rader, Cheng, Tate, Becker, Concato, Xu, Polimanti, Zhao and Gelernter2019; Liu et al., Reference Liu, Jiang, Wedow, Li, Brazel, Chen, Datta, Davila-Velderrain, McGuire, Tian, Zhan, Choquet, Docherty, Faul, Foerster, Fritsche, Gabrielsen, Gordon and Vrieze2019; Walters et al., Reference Walters, Polimanti, Johnson, McClintick, Adams, Adkins, Aliev, Bacanu, Batzler, Bertelsen, Biernacka, Bigdeli, Chen, Clarke, Chou, Degenhardt, Docherty, Edwards, Fontanillas, Foo and Agrawal2018; Zhou et al., Reference Zhou, Sealock, Sanchez-Roige, Clarke, Levey, Cheng, Li, Polimanti, Kember, Smith, Thygesen, Morgan, Atkinson, Thursz, Nyegaard, Mattheisen, Børglum, Johnson, Justice, Palmer and Gelernter2020). To capitalize on the knowledge gained from GWAS, genome-wide polygenic scores have emerged as a powerful approach that aggregates information across individuals’ genome to characterize individuals’ genetic predispositions. The genome-wide polygenic score approach has been applied to understand associations between genetic predispositions and a variety of complex traits and behaviors (Bogdan et al., Reference Bogdan, Baranger and Agrawal2018), and can be leveraged to understand how genetic risk interacts with environmental factors to influence alcohol use problems (Dick et al., Reference Dick, Barr, Cho, Cooke, Kuo, Lewis, Neale, Salvatore, Savage and Su2018). Prior research using the polygenic score approach has demonstrated significant polygenic effects on alcohol use outcomes among adolescents and adults in European American samples (Barr et al., Reference Barr, Ksinan, Su, Johnson, Meyers, Wetherill, Latvala, Aliev, Chan, Kuperman, Nurnberger, Kamarajan, Anokhin, Agrawal, Rose, Edenberg, Schuckit, Kaprio and Dick2020; Johnson et al., Reference Johnson, Sanchez-Roige, Acion, Adams, Bucholz, Chan, Chao, Chorlian, Dick, Edenberg, Foroud, Hayward, Heron, Hesselbrock, Hickman, Kendler, Kinreich, Kramer, Kuo, Kuperman and Edwards2021; Schaefer et al., Reference Schaefer, Jang, Clark, Deak, Hicks, Iacono, Liu, McGue, Vrieze and Wilson2023). A few studies examined polygenic effects on alcohol use among African American adolescents and young adults but did not find significant associations (Elam et al., Reference Elam, Bountress, Ha, Shaw, Wilson, Aliev, Dick and Lemery-Chalfant2022; Su et al., Reference Su, Trevino, Kuo, Aliev, Williams, Guy and Dick2022). One study examined polygenic effects on alcohol use from age 16 to 30, and found that polygenic risk for alcohol use was associated with alcohol use from age 22 to 27 among AAs and from age 24.5 to 29 among EAs, suggesting that polygenic effects on alcohol use may vary across age among EAs and AAs (Elam et al., Reference Elam, Ha, Neale, Aliev, Dick and Lemery-Chalfant2021). A major limitation in the field is that GWAS and genetic research in general has been primarily focused on European Americans and other populations of European ancestry, but polygenic scores created based on a discovery GWAS in one racial/ethnic group have limited portability to and reduced predictive power in other racial/ethnic groups (Martin et al., Reference Martin, Kanai, Kamatani, Okada, Neale and Daly2019). This highlights the need for studies that develop alcohol use polygenic scores based on discovery GWAS in racial/ethnic samples aligned with those in the target sample, in order to improve accuracy and predictive power of polygenic scores in racial/ethnic minority groups.

One important environmental factor that may be particularly relevant in influencing alcohol use behaviors is social support. Social support is associated with decreases in a variety of risky behaviors, including decreasing levels of alcohol consumption and fewer alcohol-related problems among racially and ethnically diverse populations (Boateng-Poku et al., Reference Boateng-Poku, Benca-Bachman, Najera, Whitfield, Taylor, Thorpe and Palmer2020; Cano et al., Reference Cano, Sánchez, Rojas, Ramírez-Ortiz, Polo, Romano and De La Rosa2018). Social support may be protective by providing greater social control and/or buffering effects of stressors, thus reducing individuals’ likelihood of drinking to cope (Peirce et al., Reference Peirce, Frone, Russell and Cooper1996). There is also evidence that social support can moderate genetic influences on substance use and that these associations may vary by sex and the source of social support (Barr et al., Reference Barr, Salvatore, Maes, Korhonen, Latvala, Aliev, Viken, Rose, Kaprio and Dick2017; Jarnecke & South, Reference Jarnecke and South2014). For example, using a sample of young adults in the Finnish Twin Study, Barr and colleagues (2017) found that higher social support was associated with a decrease in genetic influence on alcohol use for men but an increase in genetic influences on alcohol use for women. In a sample of adult twins participating in the Midlife Development in the United States study, social support from romantic partners (but not from family or friends) moderated the genetic influence on alcohol problems, such that greater perceived support from romantic partners was associated with increased genetic influences (Jarnecke & South, Reference Jarnecke and South2014). Molecular genetic studies have also showed that social support buffered genetic risk in relation to alcohol use and related outcomes. For example, support from parents mitigated the genetic risk effect of the serotonin transporter gene (5-HTTLPR) on developmental trajectories of alcohol use from adolescence to young adulthood in a nationally representative sample (Su et al., Reference Su, Supple, Leerkes and Kuo2019). Support from parents was found to attenuate the association between 5-HTTLPR and depressive symptoms (Reinelt et al., Reference Reinelt, Barnow, Stopsack, Aldinger, Schmidt, John and Grabe2015) and social anxiety disorders (Reinelt et al., Reference Reinelt, Aldinger, Stopsack, Schwahn, John, Baumeister, Grabe and Barnow2014) among adults. A recent study using data from the Collaborative Study on the Genetics of Alcoholism (COGA) found that social support from friends buffered the effect of alcohol use polygenic scores on alcohol use among European American adults (Su et al., Reference Su, Kuo, Aliev, Chan, Edenberg, Kamarajan, McCutcheon, Meyers, Schuckit, Tischfield and Dick2021); however, this study did not examine interactions between polygenic risk and social support among African Americans.

Alcohol use is a developmental phenomenon, where initiation typically occurs in adolescence and the risk for developing alcohol use disorder peaks in early adulthood (Grant et al., Reference Grant, Goldstein, Saha, Chou, Jung, Zhang, Pickering, Ruan, Smith, Huang and Hasin2015). Twin studies suggest that genetic influences on alcohol use become increasingly important from adolescence to adulthood (Dick et al., Reference Dick, Pagan, Viken, Purcell, Kaprio, Pulkkinen and Rose2007; Kendler et al., Reference Kendler, Schmitt, Aggen and Prescott2008). Molecular genetic studies have also shown that the effects of specific genes on alcohol use can vary across stages of development. For example, variation in the GABRA2 genotype was associated with an increase in drunkenness that occurred at the transition between adolescence and adulthood (18–19 years), but not during adolescence (Dick et al., Reference Dick, Cho, Latendresse, Aliev, Nurnberger, Edenberg, Schuckit, Hesselbrock, Porjesz, Bucholz, Wang, Goate, Kramer and Kuperman2014). The importance of different environmental influences can also vary across development. For example, prior research suggests that peers and friends exert their most robust influence during adolescence and emerging adulthood (Harris, Reference Harris1995; Hill et al., Reference Hill, Bromell, Tyson and Flint2007). The nature and functioning of social support change across the life course (Ertel et al., Reference Ertel, Glymour and Berkman2009), with some evidence suggesting that support from friends may impact younger adults more and support from family may benefit older adults more (Walen & Lachman, Reference Walen and Lachman2000). Collectively, these findings suggest that genetic and environmental (e.g., social support) influences on alcohol use may vary across development. However, we note that in general there is a limited understanding of how genetic and G×E effects vary across developmental stages.

The overarching goal of the present study was to examine whether social support buffers genetic risk in relation to alcohol use among EA and AA adults and across men and women using a genome-wide polygenic score approach. We considered the effects of social support from both family and friends on alcohol use, allowing us to examine whether the effects differed by sources of social support. Building on prior research, we hypothesized that (1) higher alcohol use polygenic scores (alc-PRS) would be associated with higher levels of alcohol use in both EA and AA adults, and (2) higher social support would be associated with lower alcohol use in both EA and AA adults. Although prior twin studies yielded mixed findings regarding the moderating effect of social support on genetic influences (Barr et al., Reference Barr, Salvatore, Maes, Korhonen, Latvala, Aliev, Viken, Rose, Kaprio and Dick2017; Jarnecke & South, Reference Jarnecke and South2014), molecular genetic studies have consistently demonstrated that social support may buffer genetic risk in relation to alcohol use and related outcomes (Reinelt et al., Reference Reinelt, Barnow, Stopsack, Aldinger, Schmidt, John and Grabe2015; Su et al., Reference Su, Supple, Leerkes and Kuo2019, Reference Su, Kuo, Aliev, Chan, Edenberg, Kamarajan, McCutcheon, Meyers, Schuckit, Tischfield and Dick2021). Thus, we hypothesized that social support would moderate the association between alc-PRS and alcohol use such that this association would be weaker when social support is higher in EA and AA adults. In addition, we tested potential sex differences in the associations between alc-PRS, social support, and alcohol use, given evidence of sex differences in rates of alcohol use and levels of social support (Grant et al., Reference Grant, Goldstein, Saha, Chou, Jung, Zhang, Pickering, Ruan, Smith, Huang and Hasin2015; Turner, Reference Turner1994) and in view of recommendations for examining sex differences in G×E processes in relation to alcohol use outcomes and other traits (Martin et al., Reference Martin, Khramtsova, Goleva, Blokland, Traglia, Walters, Hübel, Coleman, Breen, Børglum, Demontis, Grove, Werge, Bralten, Bulik, Lee, Mathews, Peterson, Winham and Wray2021; Salvatore et al., Reference Salvatore, Cho and Dick2017). Finally, given prior evidence suggesting that genetic and environmental influences on alcohol use vary across development (Dick et al., Reference Dick, Pagan, Viken, Purcell, Kaprio, Pulkkinen and Rose2007; Kendler et al., Reference Kendler, Schmitt, Aggen and Prescott2008), we capitalized on the rich dataset from the Collaborative Study on the Genetics of Alcoholism (COGA), which includes participants of a wide age range, to explore whether the associations between alc-PRS, social support, and alcohol use vary across developmental stages (i.e., emerging adulthoods [age 18–29] vs young adulthood [age 30–44] vs middle adulthood [age 45–65]). However, given that prior literature has not systematically examined developmental differences in effects of genetic factors, social support, and their interactions across adulthood, we did not have strong-enough evidence to derive specific hypotheses about how the associations between alc-PRS, social support, and alcohol use vary across developmental stages. We note that these developmental analyses are exploratory. Hypotheses of the current study were preregistered in the Open Science Framework (https://osf.io/hz7gb).

Method

Sample

Data for this study were drawn from the Collaborative Study on the Genetics of Alcoholism (COGA), a multi-site, large, multigenerational family study that aims to identify genetic influences on alcohol dependance and related psychiatric phenotypes (Begleiter et al., Reference Begleiter, Reich, Hesselbrock, Porjesz, Li, Schuckit and Rice1995; Edenberg, Reference Edenberg2002). Probands were identified through alcohol treatment programs at seven U.S. sites and were invited to participate if they had a sufficiently large family (usually sibships of more than three with parents available) with two or more members in the COGA catchment areas. Comparison families were also recruited from the community. Data collection for COGA started in 1991 (Phase I) when adults in the target extended families were invited to complete the Semi-Structured Assessment for the Genetics of Alcoholism, a comprehensive interview that assesses demographic factors, alcohol use disorders, and a variety of psychiatric phenotypes (Bucholz et al., Reference Bucholz, Cadoret, Cloninger, Dinwiddie, Hesselbrock, Nurnberger, Reich, Schmidt and Schuckit1994). Participants were followed up about 5 years after they completed the first assessment (Phase II). The average time to follow up at Phase II was 5.7 years. Participants also completed questionnaires, including the Perceived Social Support from Family/Friends Scale at Phase II. COGA participants were asked to provide a DNA sample via blood or saliva. Institutional Review Boards at all sites approved this study, and written consents were obtained from participants.

For the purpose of the present study, we used data from COGA Phase II interviews, including measures of social support, to address our primary research questions as that is when social support was measured. We focused on COGA participants of European or African ancestry as determined by their genetic information who also self-identified as European American or Black/African American, respectively. We included participants who (1) were between ages 18–65 at the Phase II assessment, (2) had genomic data available for the calculation of alc-PRS, and (3) indicated that they had ever used alcohol. Our analytic sample included 4,011 (mean age = 38.43, SD = 11.96, 54.5% female) EAs and 1,274 (mean age = 35.97, SD = 11.11, 54.8% female) AAs. Results from independent sample t-tests indicated that individuals who were included in the analytic sample of the present study reported more drinks per week (p < .001) and were younger (p < .001) compared to those who completed the COGA Phase II assessment but were not included in the present study; however, they did not differ in terms of sex, educational attainment, and household income. About 79.1% of the COGA Phase II sample provided a DNA sample for genotyping. Those who provided a DNA sample did not differ significantly from those who did not in terms of alcohol use and social support (p > .05).

Measures

Alcohol use

Alcohol use was measured as the number of drinks consumed in a typical week in the past 6 months (Bucholz et al., Reference Bucholz, Cadoret, Cloninger, Dinwiddie, Hesselbrock, Nurnberger, Reich, Schmidt and Schuckit1994). Participants reported the number of drinks of different kinds of alcoholic beverages (beer, wine, liquor, other) they consumed on a typical day of the week (Monday, Tuesday, … Sunday) over the past 6 months. Scores were summed across days and kinds of drinks to derive a composite score that indicated standard drinks per week. A total of 966 (24.1%) EA participants and 340 (26.7%) AA participants indicated that they consumed no alcohol (zero drinks) during the past 6 months and they were coded as zero. Extreme values (> 140 drinks per week; n = 24 (0.6%) for EAs and n = 12 (0.9%) for AAs) were winsorized (Keselman et al., Reference Keselman, Algina, Lix, Wilcox and Deering2008). Preliminary analysis indicated that the alcohol use variable was positively skewed (skewness = 4.19 and 3.42 for EAs and AAs, respectively), and thus was log transformed for subsequent regression analyses. The skewness for the log-transformed alcohol use variable was .65 and .69 for EAs and AAs, respectively.

Social support

Participants completed the Perceived Social Support from Family Scale and the Perceived Social Support from Friends Scale (Procidano & Heller, Reference Procidano and Heller1983). This is a 20-item self-report measure used to assess social support from family members or friends. Sample items include “My family/friends give me the moral support I need,” and “I rely on my family/friends for emotional support.” Participants responded to each statement on a 4-point scale ranging from 1 (generally false) to 4 (generally true). Scores were averaged across items; higher scores represent higher levels of support. Cronbach’s alpha was .95 and .93 for family support, and .91 and .86 for friend support, for EAs and AAs, respectively.

Genotyping and genome-wide polygenic scores

Participants’ DNA samples were genotyped using the Illumina Human1M array (Illumina, San Diego, CA), the Illumina Human OmniExpress 12V1 array, the Illumina 2.5M array, or the Smokescreen genotyping array (Biorealm LLC, Walnut, CA; Baurley et al., Reference Baurley, Edlund, Pardamean, Conti and Bergen2016). Data processing, quality control and imputation have been described elsewhere (Lai et al., Reference Lai, Wetherill, Bertelsen, Carey, Kamarajan, Kapoor, Meyers, Anokhin, Bennett, Bucholz, Chang, De Jager, Dick, Hesselbrock, Kramer, Kuperman, Nurnberger, Raj, Schuckit, Scott and Foroud2019). Genotypes were imputed to 1000 Genomes using the cosmopolitan reference panel (Phase 3, Version 5, NCBI GRCh37) using SHAPPEIT2 and Minimac 3. Single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) with a genotyping rate < 0.95, that violated Hardy-Weinberg equilibrium (p < 10−6) or had a minor allele frequency < 0.01, were excluded from analysis. More details on genotyping, quality control, and imputation for the COGA sample has been reported elsewhere (Wetherill et al., Reference Wetherill, Lai, Johnson, Anokhin, Bauer, Bucholz, Dick, Hariri, Hesselbrock, Kamarajan, Kramer, Kuperman, Meyers, Nurnberger, Schuckit, Scott, Taylor, Tischfield, Porjesz and Agrawal2019).

The predictive power and accuracy of PRS depends largely on the statistical power of the discovery GWAS and the genetic ancestral similarities between the discovery and target samples (Duncan et al., Reference Duncan, Shen, Gelaye, Meijsen, Ressler, Feldman, Peterson and Domingue2019; Martin et al., Reference Martin, Kanai, Kamatani, Okada, Neale and Daly2019; Peterson et al., Reference Peterson, Kuchenbaecker, Walters, Chen, Popejoy, Periyasamy, Lam, Iyegbe, Strawbridge, Brick, Carey, Martin, Meyers, Su, Chen, Edwards, Kalungi, Koen, Majara, Schwarz and Duncan2019). We calculated alc-PRS for EAs using estimates from a GWAS of alcohol use (drinks per week) in nearly 1 million individuals of European ancestry from the GWAS and Sequencing Consortium of Alcohol and Nicotine Use (Liu et al., Reference Liu, Jiang, Wedow, Li, Brazel, Chen, Datta, Davila-Velderrain, McGuire, Tian, Zhan, Choquet, Docherty, Faul, Foerster, Fritsche, Gabrielsen, Gordon and Vrieze2019), one of the largest GWAS on alcohol-related phenotypes among EAs to date. We constructed alc-PRS for EAs using the PRS-CS method, a Bayesian regression and continuous shrinkage prior method shown to improved predictive power above traditional methods of PRS construction (Ge et al., Reference Ge, Chen, Ni, Feng and Smoller2019). An average of 2,069,117 SNPs was included in the construction of alc-PRS for EAs. For AAs, we used estimates from the GWAS of alcohol consumption (AUDIT-C) in the Million Veterans Project (MVP) sample (Kranzler et al., Reference Kranzler, Zhou, Kember, Vickers Smith, Justice, Damrauer, Tsao, Klarin, Baras, Reid, Overton, Rader, Cheng, Tate, Becker, Concato, Xu, Polimanti, Zhao and Gelernter2019), the largest published GWAS of alcohol phenotypes with a multi-ancestry sample including 209,020 EAs and 57,340 AAs. We calculated alc-PRS for our AA subsample using PRS-CSx, an extension of the PRS-CS method that integrates GWAS summary statistics from multiple populations and leveraging linkage disequilibrium (LD) diversity across discovery samples to improve polygenic prediction in non-EA samples (Ruan et al., Reference Ruan, Lin, Feng, Chen, Lam, Guo, He, Sawa, Martin, Qin and Huang2022). Given GWAS summary statistics and ancestry-matched LD reference panels, PRS-CSx calculates one polygenic score for each discovery sample, and integrates them by learning an optimal linear combination to produce the final PRS. For the AA sample in the present study, Alc-PRS was calculated using PRS-CSx with GWAS summary statistics from the MVP EA and AA samples and 1000 Genomes ancestry-matched reference panels. An average of 865,352 SNPs was included in the construction of alc-PRS for AAs.

Consistent with prior research, we account for the first 10 genetic ancestry principal components (PC1-PC10) to adjust for potential population stratification. Specifically, we created residualized alc-PRS scores that account for PC1-PC10. The residualized alc-PRS scores were then standardized by creating Z-scores for subsequent analyses to aid in interpretation of results.

Covariates

Participants’ self-reported age, educational attainment, and household income, as well as sex recorded as observed by the interviewer, were included as covariates, given their demonstrated associations with alcohol use (Collins, Reference Collins2016; Grant et al., Reference Grant, Goldstein, Saha, Chou, Jung, Zhang, Pickering, Ruan, Smith, Huang and Hasin2015). Participants reported their highest level of education by responding to the question “What is the highest grade in school you completed?” Scores were converted to the number of years typically required to complete that level of education and ranged from 0 to 17 years. Participants also reported on their current household gross income based on a 9-point scale ranging from 0 (none) to 9 ($150,000 or more per year). Given the wide range of age in our sample, we created an age group variable to classify participants into three groups: emerging adulthood (ages 18–29, n = 1,068 EA and n = 390 AA), young adulthood (ages 30–44, n = 1,772 EA and n = 626 AA), and middle adulthood (ages 45–65, n = 1,234 EA and n = 275 AA). These age groups are consistent with studies in the epidemiology literature (e.g., Grant et al., Reference Grant, Goldstein, Saha, Chou, Jung, Zhang, Pickering, Ruan, Smith, Huang and Hasin2015). This allowed us to examine similarities and differences in genetic and environmental influences across developmental periods.

Analytic strategy

We first conducted preliminary analyses to examine descriptive statistics and correlations between study variables using SPSS 25.0. ANOVA analyses were also conducted to examine mean differences in social support and alcohol use across developmental stages. All analyses were stratified by ancestry. To test our hypotheses, we conducted a series of hierarchical linear regression analyses using Mplus version 8.3 separately for EAs and AAs. We included participants’ age, sex, educational attainment, and household income as covariates in all analyses. We first examined the main effects of alc-PRS on alcohol use (Model 1). Next, we added social support from family and friends as additional predictors to the regression model to examine main effects of family and friend support simultaneously (Model 2). To examine interaction effects between alc-PRS and family/friend support, we added product terms of alc-PRS and mean-centered family and friend support as additional predictors to the regression model (Model 3). We also examined potential gene-environment correlations (rGE) by testing correlations between alc-PRS and family/friend support. Significant rGE were accounted for in our models testing G×E effects by specifying alc-PRS and family/friend support to be correlated using the “WITH” command in Mplus. To check the robustness of significant G×E effects, we conducted additional analysis that included alc-PRS × covariate as well as social support × covariate interaction terms in the regression model to further account for potential confounding effects, following recommendations by Keller (Reference Keller2014).

We conducted multigroup analyses to examine whether the patterns of associations differed across sex and age groups by removing sex from the model and then comparing a model with the coefficients of interest constrained to equality with another model that had all coefficients freely estimated across females and females. Multigroup analyses across age groups were conducted by removing age from the model and then comparing a model with the coefficients of interest constrained to equality with another model that had all coefficients freely estimated across age groups (emerging adulthood vs young adulthood vs middle adulthood). A statistically significant Wald chi-square test of parameter equalities would indicate significant differences in regression coefficients across groups. Full information maximum likelihood estimation method was used to account for missing data and clustering within families was accounted using the CLUSTER command in Mplus. To account for the fact that we tested the associations independently in two ancestral groups, we used Bonferroni corrected p < .025 to evaluate statistical significance.

Finally, we conducted two sets of supplemental sensitivity analyses to further evaluate our findings. First, although the GSCAN GWAS sample is ideal for creating alc-PRS for our EA subsample given that it is the largest GWAS of alcohol phenotype (drinks per week) which also matched the alcohol phenotype of the present study, we recognize that the GSCAN sample is much larger than the MVP sample that we used to creating alc-PRS for AAs, which may be contributing to our finding of significant effects of alc-PRS in EAs but not AAs. Thus, we created alc-PRS for EAs using GWAS estimates from the MVP EA sample and ran analyses using this alc-PRS for EAs. Second, we evaluated developmental differences in G×E effects across emerging, young, and middle adulthood by creating three age bands that represent theoretically defined developmental periods (ages 18–29, 30–44, and 45–65). However, we recognize that these age bands are relatively large, and that there are vast differences within each age band concerning alcohol use. For example, alcohol use at age 18 is riskier and more problematic than at age 29. Thus, we conducted analyses using smaller age bands to further evaluate the developmental G×E effects. Given limitations by sample size, we created six smaller age bands. Specially, we split the “emerging adults” group into two groups: one group (ages 18–21) at or below the legal drinking age and another group (ages 22–29) above the legal drinking age; we further split the “young adulthood” and “middle adulthood” groups into two groups, cut at the middle of the age bands: (30–37 and 38–44 for young adults, 45–55 and 56–65 for middle adults). We note that creation of these smaller age bands is atheoretical and these analyses are exploratory.

Results

Preliminary analysis

Descriptive statistics and correlations between variables are presented in Table 1. Alc-PRS was positively correlated with alcohol use among EAs but not AAs. Both family and friend support were negatively correlated with alcohol use among EAs and AAs. Alc-PRS was negatively correlated with family and friend support among EAs. Results from ANOVA analyses indicated significant mean differences in social support and alcohol use across developmental stages (Supplemental Table 1).

Table 1. Bivariate correlations among key study variables for European Americans and African Americans

Alc-PRS = alcohol use genome-wide polygenic score; Supp = support; Sex is coded as 1 = male; 0 = female.

a proportion of males.

b alc-PRS are standardized. Descriptive statistics for raw scores of alcohol use are presented.

*p < .05.

**p < .01.

Means and standard deviations for variables are presented for European Americans/African Americans.

Predicting alcohol use from alc-PRS and social support

European Americans

Results from hierarchical multiple regression models predicting alcohol use are presented in Table 2. For EAs, there was a significant main effect of alc-PRS such that higher alc-PRS was associated with higher levels of alcohol use, above and beyond the effects of covariates (Model 1). Higher social support from friends was associated with lower alcohol use, but social support from family was not significantly associated with alcohol use (Model).

Table 2. Predicting alcohol use from alcohol use polygenic scores and social support among European American adults

N = 4,011; Alc-PRS = alcohol consumption genome-wide polygenic score.

Statistically significant effects of Alc-PRS and social support are bolded.

There was an interaction between alc-PRS and friend support in predicting alcohol use (Model #). As illustrated in Figure 1, simple slope analysis indicated that alc-PRS was more strongly associated with alcohol use when friend support was low (−1 SD; B = .17, SE = .03, β = .14, p < .001) than when friend support was high (+1 SD; B = .07, SE = .03, β = .06, p = .011). The interaction effect between alc-PRS and friend support in relation to alcohol use remained statistically significant in follow up analysis where alc-PRS by covariate (age, sex, educational attainment, household income) and friend support by covariate interaction terms were included in the regression model (p = .013, see Supplemental Table 1), suggesting robustness of the interaction effect. There was no significant interaction between alc-PRS and family support in relation to alcohol use.

Figure 1. Alc-PRS by friend support interaction in relation to alcohol use among European Americans. Predicted values of log-transformed alcohol use (drinks per week) are plotted at prototypical values (+1/−1 SD) of alc-PRS and friend support.

African Americans

Consistent with findings for EAs, higher social support from friends was also associated with lower alcohol use, but social support from family was not significantly associated with alcohol use among AAs. However, there was no significant main effect of alc-PRS on alcohol use. In addition, we did not find any significant interactions between alc-PRS and social support from family or friends in relation to alcohol use in the AA subsample (see Table 3).

Table 3. Predicting alcohol use from alcohol use polygenic scores and social support among African American adults

N = 1,274; Alc-PRS = alcohol consumption genome-wide polygenic score.

Statistically significant effects of social support are bolded.

Examining differences across developmental stages

European Americans

There were significant differences in the associations between social support and alcohol use across different developmental stages in adulthood for EAs (Table 4). Specifically, higher family support was associated with lower alcohol use (B = −.14, CI [−.23, -.06], SE = .05, β = −.09, p = .007) for emerging adults (age 18-29), higher alcohol use (B = .13, CI [.04, .22], SE = .05, β = .07, p = .013) for young adults (age 30–44), and was not significantly associated with alcohol use in middle adulthood (age 45–65; B = .03, CI [−.08, .13], SE = .06, β = .02, p = .671) (χ2 = 13.81, df = 2, p = .001). These associations significantly differed between emerging adults and young adults (χ2 = 13.61, df = 1, p < .001), but did not significantly differ between young adults and middle-aged adults (χ2 = 1.62, df = 1, p = .203) or between emerging adults and middle-aged adults (χ2 = 4.31, df = 1, p = .038). The association between friend support and alcohol use also varied across adulthood (χ2 = 17.94, df = 2, p < .001). Greater social support from friends was associated with higher alcohol use in emerging adulthood (B = .17, CI [ .05, .28], SE = .07, β = .08, p = .018), but lower alcohol use in young adulthood (B = −.22, CI [−.32, −.11], SE = .06, β = −.10, p < .001) and middle adulthood (B = −.18, [−.29, −.07], SE = .07, β = -.09, p = .008). These associations significantly differed between emerging adults and young adults (χ2 = 16.05, df = 1, p < .001) and between emerging adults and middle-aged adults (χ2 = 11.92, df = 1, p < .001), but did not differ between young adults and middle-aged adults (χ2 = .16, df = 1, p = .687). There were no significant differences in the effect of alc-PRS in relation to alcohol use across developmental stages. The interaction between alc-PRS and social support from friends was significant in young adulthood but not significant in emerging or middle adulthood; the interaction effect significantly differed between young adulthood and middle adulthood (χ2 = 6.85, df = 1, p = .009) but did not differ significantly between emerging and young adulthood (χ2 = 2.18, df = 1, p = .140) or between emerging and middle adulthood (χ2 = .57, df = 1, p = .452).

Table 4. Predicting alcohol use from alcohol use polygenic scores and social support: testing developmental differences among European Americans adults

Alc-PRS = alcohol consumption genome-wide polygenic score.

Emerging adulthood ages 18–29, young adulthood ages 30–44, and middle adulthood ages 45–65. Coefficients that are statistically significantly different across age groups are bolded.

African Americans

As presented in Table 5, there were significant differences in the association between friend support and alcohol use across developmental stages for AAs. Specifically, friend support was significantly associated with lower alcohol use in emerging adulthood (B = −.28, CI [−.48, −.08], SE = .12, β = −.14, p = .023) and middle adulthood (B = −.59, CI [−.83, −.35], SE = .15, β = -.24, p < .001), but not in young adulthood (B = −.05, CI [−.22, .13], SE = .11, β = −.02, p = .667). These associations significantly differed between young adults and middle-aged adults (χ2 = 7.97, df = 1, p = .005), but there were no statistically significant difference in the associations between emerging adults and young adults (χ2 = 2.03, df = 1, p = .154) or between emerging adults and middle-aged adults (χ2 = 2.57, df = 1, p = .109).

Table 5. Predicting alcohol use from alcohol use polygenic scores and social support: testing developmental differences among African American adults

Alc-PRS = alcohol consumption genome-wide polygenic score.

Emerging adulthood ages 18–29, young adulthood ages 30–44, and middle adulthood ages 45–65. Coefficients that are statistically significantly different across age groups are bolded.

There were also significant differences in the interaction between alc-PRS and friend support in relation to alcohol use across developmental stages. This interaction between alc-PRS and friend support was significantly associated with alcohol use in middle adulthood (B = −.40, CI [−.56, −.24], SE = .10, β = −.18, p < .001), but not in emerging adulthood or young adulthood. These associations significantly differed between young adults and middle-aged adults (χ2 = 7.05, df = 1, p = .008) and between emerging adults and middle-aged adults (χ2 = 7.37, df = 1, p = .007), but did not significantly differ between emerging adults and young adults (χ2 = .29, df = 1, p = .593). As illustrated in Figure 2, simple slope analysis indicated that alc-PRS was significantly associated with alcohol use among middle-aged AA adults when friend support was low (−1 SD; B = .24, SE = .10, β = .16, p = .018), but not when friend support was high (+1 SD; B = −.18, SE = .09, β = -.13, p = .035). This interaction effect remained statistically significant after accounting for alc-PRS × covariate and friend support × covariate interactions (p = .007, see supplemental Table 2).

Figure 2. Alc-PRS by friend support interaction in relation to alcohol use among African Americans in middle adulthood. Predicted values of log-transformed alcohol use (drinks per week) are plotted at prototypical values (+1/−1 SD) of alc-PRS and friend support.

There were no significant differences in the associations between alc-PRS, family support, as well as the interaction between alc-PRS and family support, and alcohol use across developmental age groups.

Examining sex differences

Results from multigroup analyses indicated that there was no significant difference in the associations between alc-PRS, social support, and their interactions, in relation to alcohol use between males and females for EAs (Walt test χ2 = 11.09, df = 5, p = .05; see Supplemental Table 3) or AAs (Walt test χ2 = 5.23, df = 5, p = .39; see Supplemental Table 4).

Supplemental sensitivity analyses

Results from analyses with alc-PRS calculated based on GWAS summary statistics with the MVP EA sample were generally consistent with results presented above for the EA subsample, except that the interaction effect between alc-PRS and friend support was not significant. The main effect of alc-PRS on alcohol use was significant, although the effect size was relatively smaller in the sensitivity analysis, which may have hindered the detection of G´ E effects. Detailed results from this set of sensitivity analyses are presented in Supplemental Tables 68. Results from the sensitivity analyses with smaller age bands are presented in Supplemental Tables 9 and 10. Results are largely consistent with those conducted with larger age bands, except that the alc-PRS by friend support interaction significantly differed across developmental periods among EAs in the sensitivity analysis. The interaction is significant during ages 30–37 and the effect differed from those in other developmental periods. In addition, the main effect of friend support on alcohol use did not significantly differ across developmental periods among AAs in the sensitivity analysis, which could be due to smaller sample sizes with the smaller age bands and reduced statistical power.

Discussion

The overarching goal of this study was to examine the interactive effects between genetic risk and social support in relation to alcohol use among EA and AA adults, using a genome-wide polygenic risk score approach. Utilizing data from a large diverse sample of adults from extended families enriched for alcohol use disorders, our findings indicated that perceived social support from friends buffers the effect of genetic risk for alcohol use on phenotypic alcohol use among both EA and AA (middle adulthood only for AAs) adults. We found no significant difference in the role of alc-PRS and social support in relation to alcohol use between males and females; however, important developmental differences in the role of social support in relation to alcohol use were observed among both EAs and AAs. This study extends the literature by taking a polygenic score approach to examine developmental G×E effects among both EA and AA adults, contributing novel insights regarding interactions between polygenic risk and social support in relation to alcohol use across ancestral groups, developmental stages, and sex.

Consistent with our hypothesis, higher alc-PRS was associated with greater alcohol use among EAs. Social support from friends (but not family) was associated with lower levels of alcohol use among EAs in the whole sample. In addition, we found evidence of an interaction effect between alc-PRS and perceived social support from friends in relation to alcohol use, such that high levels of friend support attenuated/buffered the association between alc-PRS and alcohol use. Our finding is consistent with prior research conducted in EAs indicating a buffering effect of social support on measured genetic risk (Reinelt et al., Reference Reinelt, Barnow, Stopsack, Aldinger, Schmidt, John and Grabe2015; Su et al., Reference Su, Supple, Leerkes and Kuo2019). Previous studies using twin-designs have also demonstrated interaction effects between genetic factors and social support; however, some studies found that genetic influences were stronger when social support was higher (Jarnecke & South, Reference Jarnecke and South2014). Barr et al. (Reference Barr, Salvatore, Maes, Korhonen, Latvala, Aliev, Viken, Rose, Kaprio and Dick2017) found that genetic influences on alcohol use were stronger when social support was higher for women, and the opposite was found for men. Differences in G×E findings across studies could be attributed to differences in genetic methodology, sample design, and measurement of constructs. By using a molecular genetic design, we characterized a specific dimension of genetic risk (i.e., alc-PRS), whereas twin studies quantify overall latent genetic influences. It is possible that high social support buffers genetic risk in relation to a particular developmental outcome, while also providing a context for the overall genetic predisposition to be expressed (rather than being suppressed). Future research is needed to replicate our findings and further understand the role of social support in moderating molecular genetic influences on alcohol use outcomes. Furthermore, we note that there was a small negative correlation between alc-PRS and family support and friend support. Although the present study focused on examining G×E effects, we note that these gene-environment correlation (rGE) processes may also underlie polygenic influences on alcohol use. Future research is warranted to examine rGE in addition to G×E processes to further understand mechanisms linking genetic risk and social support to alcohol use outcomes.

Similar to the findings for EAs, social support from friends (but not family) was associated with lower levels of alcohol use among AAs, suggesting that friend support is particularly important in adulthood. However, there was no significant main effect of alc-PRS on alcohol use among AAs. There was also no significant interaction effect between alc-PRS and social support from friends or family in relation to alcohol use among the whole AA subsample, which could be, at least in part, due to the low predictive ability of alc-PRS among AAs. This finding of limited predictive utility of polygenic scores among AAs is likely due to the underrepresentation and small sample size of AAs in the GWAS studies that the alc-PRS was derived from (Martin et al., Reference Martin, Kanai, Kamatani, Okada, Neale and Daly2019; Su et al., Reference Su, Kuo, Meyers, Guy and Dick2018). These findings highlight the continued need for increased representation of AAs in genetic research to better characterize genetic risk among this population. Additionally, we note that alc-PRS for AAs were generated for the AUDIT-C while alc-PRS for EAS were generated for drinks per week, which matched more closely to the outcome of the present study. Prior research indicates that PRS prediction is the strongest with matched phenotypes between the discovery GWAS and the target sample (Docherty et al., Reference Docherty, Moscati, Dick, Savage, Salvatore, Cooke and Kendler2018). Thus, it is possible that the reduced predictability of alc-PRS among AAs in our sample could be due to the mismatch in phenotypes between the discovery GWAS and the current study.

Notably, prior G×E research on alcohol use outcomes primarily focused on adolescents and young adults, and limited research has examined G×E influences across a broader age range of adulthood (Su et al., Reference Su, Kuo, Aliev, Chan, Edenberg, Kamarajan, McCutcheon, Meyers, Schuckit, Tischfield and Dick2021). This study examined potential developmental differences in the role of alc-PRS and social support in alcohol use across the adulthood years. For EAs, the main effect of alc-PRS on alcohol use appears to be fairly consistent across adulthood. This appears to be inconsistent with prior evidence that heritability of alcohol use increases from adolescence to adulthood (Dick et al., Reference Dick, Pagan, Viken, Purcell, Kaprio, Pulkkinen and Rose2007; Kendler et al., Reference Kendler, Schmitt, Aggen and Prescott2008). The inconsistent findings may be due to different methodology (twin studies vs. polygenic scores). The limited predictability of alc-PRS in the present study may also have hindered the detection of developmental differences. Alternatively, this may be due to the fact that our sample was comprised of high-risk adult alcohol users, the majority of whom may have established their drinking patterns and thus genetic influences on their alcohol use may be more stable. Future research is needed to further examine developmental changes in polygenic effects on alcohol use. However, the role of social support on alcohol use varied across adulthood. Specifically, friend support was associated with more alcohol use in emerging adulthood, but was protective and associated with lower alcohol use in young and middle adulthood. This may be because alcohol use during emerging adulthood typically occurs in peer social settings (LaBrie et al., Reference LaBrie, Hummer and Pedersen2007). Thus, it is possible that support from friends during emerging adulthood may not provide increased social control in limiting alcohol use. Family support was most protective against alcohol use in emerging adulthood, but was associated with more alcohol use in young adulthood. This finding is consistent with prior research showing the continued protective role of parental support for emerging adults (Kouros et al., Reference Kouros, Pruitt, Ekas, Kiriaki and Sunderland2017; Serido et al., Reference Serido, Lawry, Li, Conger and Russell2014). Young adults may be more likely to drink with family members (e.g., spouse) and thus a strong family support may be associated with more drinking in the family context during young adulthood (Homish & Leonard, Reference Homish and Leonard2007). Our preliminary analyses indicated significant mean-level differences in social support across developmental stages. However, there were no clear pattern of connection between differences in levels of social support and differences in the association between social support and alcohol use across developmental stages. Thus, it is unlikely that the observed differences in effects of social support across developmental stages are due to differences in endorsement of social support.

For AAs, family support was not associated with alcohol use across adulthood, whereas friend support was protective against alcohol use in emerging and middle adulthood, above and beyond the effect of age, sex, education, and family income. It is possible that for AAs family support plays a stronger role in alcohol use earlier in development (e.g., adolescence) than in adulthood. We note that family support was negatively correlated with alcohol use among AAs in bivariate correlation analyses. These findings are partially consistent with prior research in AA middle-aged adults, which found that both friend and family emotional support was negatively associated with alcohol use (Boateng-Poku et al., Reference Boateng-Poku, Benca-Bachman, Najera, Whitfield, Taylor, Thorpe and Palmer2020). Our findings also indicated differences in the interaction between alc-PRS and friend support in relation to alcohol use across developmental stages. That is, friend support attenuated genetic risk for alcohol use during middle adulthood but not during emerging or young adulthood. These findings highlight the particularly important role of social support from friends during middle adulthood for AAs. To our knowledge, this is the first study to show developmental differences in the role of social support in moderating genetic influences among AAs. This interaction between alc-PRS and friend support in relation to alcohol use among AA middle adults remained statistically significant (p = .007) in the robustness analysis where alc-PRS × covariate and friend support × covariate interactions were included to further account for potential confounding effects, suggesting the robustness of the effect. However, we acknowledge that the sample size for the AA middle adults subsample is quite small (n = 273). With limited statistical power, it is possible that the detected interaction effect between PRS and friend support was a false positive finding. Thus, we consider our findings preliminary and future research is needed to replicate our findings.

Collectively, these results highlight the importance of examining developmental changes in the role of social support from family and friends in relation to alcohol use. Prior research suggests that the influence of social support on alcohol use outcomes may depend on the drinking behaviors of the social partners. Friends, romantic partners, and spouses who are more congruent in their drinking patterns tend to report higher levels of relationship satisfaction (Homish & Leonard, Reference Homish and Leonard2007; Marshal, Reference Marshal2003; Osgood et al., Reference Osgood, Ragan, Wallace, Gest, Feinberg and Moody2013). Those who perceive higher social support and have friends, romantic partners, or spouses who engage in high levels of drinking may be at higher risk for alcohol use. Future research is warranted to examine the role of social partners’ drinking behavior in moderating the association between social support and alcohol use across developmental stages.

Consistent with previous evidence of different levels of alcohol use and social support between males and females (Grant et al., Reference Grant, Goldstein, Saha, Chou, Jung, Zhang, Pickering, Ruan, Smith, Huang and Hasin2015; Turner, Reference Turner1994), we found that males reported lower levels of social support from family and friends and higher levels of alcohol use compared to females. However, despite these mean-level differences, we did not find any significant sex differences in the associations between alc-PRS, social support, and alcohol use among both EA and AA adults. This is consistent with prior findings suggesting that the effects of alc-PRS, social support, and their interactions on alcohol use are similar for males and females (Su et al., Reference Su, Kuo, Aliev, Chan, Edenberg, Kamarajan, McCutcheon, Meyers, Schuckit, Tischfield and Dick2021). Nevertheless, future research is needed to further examine sex differences in G×E processes in genetically informed alcohol research (Salvatore et al., Reference Salvatore, Cho and Dick2017). Additionally, although the present study is not powered to do so, future research is needed to examine G×E by sex by development effects on alcohol use and related outcomes, as sex differences in G×E effects may vary across development and vice versa.

There are several strengths of this study, including a relatively large sample size and use of the genome-wide polygenic score approach to characterize genetic risk. In addition, we included a sample of both EAs and AAs and our findings contribute to the limited G×E literature among non-European populations (Dick et al., Reference Dick, Barr, Guy, Nasim and Scott2017). Furthermore, our sample of a wide age range allowed for the examination of differences in the role of alc-PRS and social support in relation to alcohol use across emerging, young, and middle adulthood. Despite these strengths, our findings need to be interpreted in light of several limitations. First, our cross-sectional analyses preclude us from making causal inferences regarding the relation between social support and alcohol use. Second, our examination of differences across developmental periods based on these cross-sectional analyses may also be subject to bias due to cohort effects. Future research should employ longitudinal designs to examine within-person changes in the role of genetic risk and social support in relation to alcohol use across development. Third, despite using a relatively large sample size compared to many other prior G×E studies, the sample size for the AA subsample may still be underpower to detect 3-way interactions for testing sex differences or developmental differences in G×E effects. Future studies are needed to replicate the present findings with larger AA samples. Furthermore, COGA is a high-risk sample of participants from extended families enriched for alcohol use disorders and findings from this study may not be generalizable to other samples with different recruitment strategies (Savage et al., Reference Savage, Salvatore, Aliev, Edwards, Hickman, Kendler, Macleod, Latvala, Loukola, Kaprio, Rose, Chan, Hesselbrock, Webb, Adkins, Bigdeli, Riley and Dick2018). Thus, our sample is not representative of the general population, which may increase the potential for collider bias in estimating the effect of alc-PRS (Akimova et al., Reference Akimova, Breen, Brazel and Mills2021). Future research is needed to replicate our findings in community and population-based samples and to apply advanced methodologies such as principal component analysis of measured cofounders as a method to reduce collider bias (Thomas et al., Reference Thomas, Barr, Aliev, Stephenson, Kuo, Chan, Dick, Edenberg, Hesselbrock, Kamarajan, Kuperman and Salvatore2022). Finally, we note that the effect sizes for the significant genetic main and G × E effects are quite small. While these small effect sizes appear to run counter to the heritability estimates from quantitative genetic studies, they are consistent with findings from other studies using a polygenic score approach. These likely reflect methodological differences between the polygenic score approach and quantitative genetic approaches (which estimate the overall latent genetic influences on a phenotype). These findings also indicate limitations of the current polygenic score approach in predicting alcohol use outcomes, and highlight the need for improving predictability of polygenic scores with methodology advancements (e.g., larger discovery GWAS particularly among AAs, functional polygenic scores).

In conclusion, our findings show that perceived social support from friends play an important role in buffering the effects of genetic risk for alcohol use and suggest that the role of social support from both family and friends in relation to alcohol use vary across age. These results suggest that strengthening social support may be one means to reduce alcohol use among EA and AA adults. Given prior evidence that psychosocial interventions may mitigate genetic risk for alcohol use outcomes (Kuo et al., Reference Kuo, Salvatore, Aliev, Ha, Dishion and Dick2019; Neale et al., Reference Neale, Kuo and Dick2021), our findings also suggest that intervention programs that target at strengthening social support from friends may have the potential to reduce alcohol use among those high in genetic risk.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579423001141.

Acknowledgements

The Collaborative Study on the Genetics of Alcoholism (COGA), Principal Investigators B. Porjesz, V. Hesselbrock, T. Foroud; Scientific Director, A. Agrawal; Translational Director, D. Dick, includes ten different centers: University of Connecticut (V. Hesselbrock); Indiana University (H.J. Edenberg, T. Foroud, Y. Liu, M.H. Plawecki); University of Iowa Carver College of Medicine (S. Kuperman, J. Kramer); SUNY Downstate Health Sciences University (B. Porjesz, J. Meyers, C. Kamarajan, A. Pandey); Washington University in St Louis (L. Bierut, J. Rice, K. Bucholz, A. Agrawal); University of California at San Diego (M. Schuckit); Rutgers University (J. Tischfield, D. Dick, R. Hart, J. Salvatore); The Children’s Hospital of Philadelphia, University of Pennsylvania (L. Almasy); Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai (A. Goate, P. Slesinger); and Howard University (D. Scott). Other COGA collaborators include: L. Bauer (University of Connecticut); J. Nurnberger Jr., L. Wetherill, X., Xuei, D. Lai, S. O’Connor, (Indiana University); G. Chan (University of Iowa; University of Connecticut); D.B. Chorlian, J. Zhang, P. Barr, S. Kinreich, G. Pandey (SUNY Downstate); N. Mullins (Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai); A. Anokhin, S. Hartz, E. Johnson, V. McCutcheon, S. Saccone (Washington University); J. Moore, F. Aliev, Z. Pang, S. Kuo (Rutgers University); A. Merikangas (The Children’s Hospital of Philadelphia and University of Pennsylvania); H. Chin and A. Parsian are the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism (NIAAA) Staff Collaborators. We continue to be inspired by our memories of Henri Begleiter and Theodore Reich, founding PI and Co-PI of COGA, and also owe a debt of gratitude to other past organizers of COGA, including Ting- Kai Li, P. Michael Conneally, Raymond Crowe, and Wendy Reich, for their critical contributions. This national collaborative study is supported by NIH Grant U10AA008401 from the NIAAA and the National Institute on Drug Abuse.

Competing interests

None.

References

Akimova, E. T., Breen, R., Brazel, D. M., & Mills, M. C. (2021). Gene-environment dependencies lead to collider bias in models with polygenic. Scientific Reports, 11(1), 9457. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-021-89020-x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barr, P. B., Ksinan, A., Su, J., Johnson, E. C., Meyers, J. L., Wetherill, L., Latvala, A., Aliev, F., Chan, G., Kuperman, S., Nurnberger, J., Kamarajan, C., Anokhin, A., Agrawal, A., Rose, R. J., Edenberg, H. J., Schuckit, M., Kaprio, J., & Dick, D. M. (2020). Using polygenic scores for identifying individuals at increased risk of substance use disorders in clinical and population samples. Translational Psychiatry 10, 196. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41398-020-00865-8 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barr, P. B., Salvatore, J. E., Maes, H. H., Korhonen, T., Latvala, A., Aliev, F., Viken, R., Rose, R. J., Kaprio, J., & Dick, D. M. (2017). Social relationships moderate genetic influences on heavy drinking in young adulthood. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs, 78(6), 817826. https://doi.org/10.15288/jsad.2017.78.817 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baurley, J. W., Edlund, C. K., Pardamean, C. I., Conti, D. V., & Bergen, A. W. (2016). Smokescreen: A targeted genotyping array for addiction research. BMC Genomics, 17(1), 145. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12864-016-2495-7 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Begleiter, H., Reich, T., Hesselbrock, V., Porjesz, B., Li, T. K., Schuckit, M. A., & Rice, J. P. (1995). The collaborative study on the genetics of alcoholism. Alcohol Health and Research World, 19, 228228.Google Scholar
Boateng-Poku, A., Benca-Bachman, C. E., Najera, D. D., Whitfield, K. E., Taylor, J. L., Thorpe, R. J., & Palmer, R. H. C. (2020). The role of social support on the effects of stress and depression on African American tobacco and alcohol use. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 209, 107926. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2020.107926 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bogdan, R., Baranger, D. A. A., & Agrawal, A. (2018). Polygenic risk scores in clinical psychology: Bridging genomic risk to individual differences. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 14(1), 119157. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-050817-084847 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bucholz, K. K., Cadoret, R., Cloninger, C. R., Dinwiddie, S. H., Hesselbrock, V. M., Nurnberger, J. I., Reich, T., Schmidt, I., & Schuckit, M. A. (1994). A new, semi-structured psychiatric interview for use in genetic linkage studies: A report on the reliability of the SSAGA. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 55(2), 149158. https://doi.org/10.15288/jsa.1994.55.149 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Campbell, M. C., & Tishkoff, S. A. (2008). African genetic diversity: Implications for human demographic history, modern human origins, and complex disease mapping. Annual Review of Genomics and Human Genetics, 9(1), 403433. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.genom.9.081307.164258 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cano, M.Á., Sánchez, M., Rojas, P., Ramírez-Ortiz, D., Polo, K. L., Romano, E., & De La Rosa, M. (2018). Alcohol use severity among adult hispanic immigrants: Examining the roles of family cohesion, social support, and gender. Substance Use & Misuse, 53(4), 668676. https://doi.org/10.1080/10826084.2017.1356333 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Causadias, J., & Korous, K. M. (2018). How are genes related to culture? An introduction to the field of cultural genomics. In The handbook of culture and biology interplay (pp. 151177). Wiley.Google Scholar
Collins, S. E. (2016). Associations between socioeconomic factors and alcohol outcomes. Alcohol Research: Current Reviews, 38(1), 8394.Google ScholarPubMed
Dick, D. M., Agrawal, A., Keller, M. C., Adkins, A., Aliev, F., Monroe, S., Hewitt, J. K., Kendler, K. S., & Sher, K. J. (2015). Candidate gene-environment interaction research: Reflections and recommendations. Perspectives on psychological science: A journal of the Association for Psychological Science, 10(1), 3759. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691614556682 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dick, D. M., Barr, P., Guy, M., Nasim, A., & Scott, D. (2017). Review: Genetic research on alcohol use outcomes in African American populations: A review of the literature, associated challenges, and implications. The American Journal on Addictions, 26(5), 486493. https://doi.org/10.1111/ajad.12495 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dick, D. M., Barr, P. B., Cho, S. B., Cooke, M. E., Kuo, S. I., Lewis, T. J., Neale, Z., Salvatore, J. E., Savage, J., & Su, J. (2018). Post-GWAS in psychiatric genetics: a developmental perspective on the "other" next steps. Genes, brain, and behavior, 17(3), e12447. https://doi.org/10.1111/gbb.12447 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dick, D. M., Cho, S. B., Latendresse, S. J., Aliev, F., Nurnberger, J. I., Edenberg, H. J., Schuckit, M., Hesselbrock, V. M., Porjesz, B., Bucholz, K., Wang, J.-C., Goate, A., Kramer, J. R., & Kuperman, S. (2014). Genetic influences on alcohol use across stages of development: GABRA2 and longitudinal trajectories of drunkenness from adolescence to young adulthood. Addiction Biology, 19(6), 10551064. https://doi.org/10.1111/adb.12066 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dick, D. M., & Kendler, K. S. (2012). The impact of gene-environment interaction on alcohol use disorders. Alcohol Research : Current Reviews, 34(3), 318324.Google ScholarPubMed
Dick, D. M., Pagan, J. L., Viken, R., Purcell, S., Kaprio, J., Pulkkinen, L., & Rose, R. J. (2007). Changing environmental influences on substance use across development. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 10(2), 315326. https://doi.org/10.1375/TWIN.10.2.315 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Docherty, A., Moscati, A., Dick, D., Savage, J., Salvatore, J., Cooke, M., & Kendler, K. (2018). Polygenic prediction of the phenome, across ancestry, in emerging adulthood. Psychological Medicine, 48(11), 18141823. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291717003312 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Duncan, L., Shen, H., Gelaye, B., Meijsen, J., Ressler, K., Feldman, M., Peterson, R., & Domingue, B. (2019). Analysis of polygenic risk score usage and performance in diverse human populations. Nature communications, 10(1), 3328. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-019-11112-0 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Duncan, L. E., & Keller, M. C. (2011). A critical review of the First 10 Years of candidate gene-by-environment interaction research in psychiatry. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 168(10), 10411049. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2011.11020191 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Edenberg, H. J. (2002). The collaborative study on the genetics of alcoholism: An update. Alcohol Research & Health, 26, 214218.Google Scholar
Elam, K. K., Bountress, K. E., Ha, T., Shaw, D. S., Wilson, M. N., Aliev, F., Dick, D. M., & Lemery-Chalfant, K. (2022). Developmental genetic effects on externalizing behavior and alcohol use: Examination across two longitudinal samples. Development and Psychopathology, 1–10. Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579422000980 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elam, K. K., Ha, T., Neale, Z., Aliev, F., Dick, D., & Lemery-Chalfant, K. (2021). Age varying polygenic effects on alcohol use in African Americans and European Americans from adolescence to adulthood. Scientific Reports, 11(1), 22425. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-021-01923-x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ertel, K. A., Glymour, M. M., & Berkman, L. F. (2009). Social networks and health: A life course perspective integrating observational and experimental evidence. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 26(1), 7392. https://doi.org/10.1177/0265407509105523 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gabriel, S. B., Schaffner, S. F., Nguyen, H., Moore, J. M., Roy, J., Blumenstiel, B., Higgins, J., DeFelice, M., Lochner, A., Faggart, M., Liu-Cordero, S. N., Rotimi, C., Adeyemo, A., Cooper, R., Ward, R., Lander, E. S., Daly, M. J., & Altshuler, D. (2002). The structure of haplotype blocks in the human genome. Science (New York, N.Y.), 296(5576), 22252229. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1069424 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ge, T., Chen, C.-Y., Ni, Y., Feng, Y.-C. A., & Smoller, J. W. (2019). Polygenic prediction via Bayesian regression and continuous shrinkage priors. Nature Communications, 10(1), 1776. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-019-09718-5 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gelernter, J., Sun, N., Polimanti, R., Pietrzak, R. H., Levey, D. F., Lu, Q., Hu, Y., Li, B., Radhakrishnan, K., Aslan, M., Cheung, K. H., Li, Y., Rajeevan, N., Sayward, F., Harrington, K., Chen, Q., Cho, K., Honerlaw, J., , S., Lencz, T., … Million Veteran Program (2019). Genome-wide association study of maximum habitual alcohol intake in >140,000 U.S. European and African American veterans yields novel risk loci. Biological psychiatry, 86(5), 365376. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2019.03.984 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gilbert, P. A., & Zemore, S. E. (2016). Discrimination and drinking: A systematic review of the evidence. Social Science & Medicine, 161, 178194. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2016.06.009 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grant, B. F., Goldstein, R. B., Saha, T. D., Chou, S. P., Jung, J., Zhang, H., Pickering, R. P., Ruan, W. J., Smith, S. M., Huang, B., & Hasin, D. S. (2015). Epidemiology of DSM-5 alcohol use disorder: Results from the national epidemiologic survey on alcohol and related conditions III. JAMA Psychiatry, 72(8), 757766. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2015.0584 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harris, J. R. (1995). Where is the child’s environment? A group socialization theory of development. Psychological Review, 102(3), 458489. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.102.3.458 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hill, N. E., Bromell, L., Tyson, D. F., & Flint, R. (2007). Developmental commentary: Ecological perspectives on parental influences during adolescence. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 36(3), 367377. https://doi.org/10.1080/15374410701444322 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Homish, G. G., & Leonard, K. E. (2007). The drinking partnership and marital satisfaction: The longitudinal influence of discrepant drinking. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 75(1), 4351. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.75.1.43 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jarnecke, A. M., & South, S. C. (2014). Genetic and environmental influences on alcohol use problems: Moderation by romantic partner support, but not family or friend support. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 38(2), 367375. https://doi.org/10.1111/acer.12263 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnson, E. C., Sanchez-Roige, S., Acion, L., Adams, M. J., Bucholz, K. K., Chan, G., Chao, M. J., Chorlian, D. B., Dick, D. M., Edenberg, H. J., Foroud, T., Hayward, C., Heron, J., Hesselbrock, V., Hickman, M., Kendler, K. S., Kinreich, S., Kramer, J., Kuo, S. I., Kuperman, S., … Edwards, A. C. (2021). Polygenic contributions to alcohol use and alcohol use disorders across population-based and clinically ascertained samples. Psychological Medicine, 51(7), 11471156. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291719004045 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keller, M. C. (2014). Gene x environment interaction studies have not properly controlled for potential confounders: The problem and the (simple) solution. Biological Psychiatry, 75(1), 1824.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kendler, K. S., Schmitt, E., Aggen, S. H., & Prescott, C. A. (2008). Genetic and environmental influences on alcohol, caffeine, cannabis, and nicotine use from early adolescence to middle adulthood. Archives of General Psychiatry, 65(6), 674682. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.65.6.674 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keselman, H. J., Algina, J., Lix, L. M., Wilcox, R. R., & Deering, K. N. (2008). A generally robust approach for testing hypotheses and setting confidence intervals for effect sizes. Psychological Methods, 13(2), 110129. https://doi.org/10.1037/1082-989X.13.2.110 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kouros, C. D., Pruitt, M. M., Ekas, N. V., Kiriaki, R., & Sunderland, M. (2017). Helicopter parenting, autonomy support, and college students’ mental health and well-being: the moderating role of sex and ethnicity. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 26(3), 939949. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10826-016-0614-3 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kranzler, H. R., Zhou, H., Kember, R. L., Vickers Smith, R., Justice, A. C., Damrauer, S., Tsao, P. S., Klarin, D., Baras, A., Reid, J., Overton, J., Rader, D. J., Cheng, Z., Tate, J. P., Becker, W. C., Concato, J., Xu, K., Polimanti, R., Zhao, H., & Gelernter, J. (2019). Genome-wide association study of alcohol consumption and use disorder in 274,424 individuals from multiple populations. Nature Communications, 10(1), 1499. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-019-09480-8 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuo, S. I., Salvatore, J. E., Aliev, F., Ha, T., Dishion, T. J., & Dick, D. M. (2019). The family check-up intervention moderates polygenic influences on long-term alcohol outcomes: results from a randomized intervention trial. Prevention science: the official journal of the Society for Prevention Research, 20(7), 975985. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11121-019-01024-2 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
LaBrie, J., Hummer, J., & Pedersen, E. (2007). Reasons for drinking in the college student context: The differential role and risk of the social motivator. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs, 68(3), 393398. https://doi.org/10.15288/jsad.2007.68.393 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lai, D., Wetherill, L., Bertelsen, S., Carey, C. E., Kamarajan, C., Kapoor, M., Meyers, J. L., Anokhin, A. P., Bennett, D. A., Bucholz, K. K., Chang, K. K., De Jager, P. L., Dick, D. M., Hesselbrock, V., Kramer, J., Kuperman, S., Nurnberger, J. I. Jr, Raj, T., Schuckit, M., Scott, D. M., … Foroud, T. (2019). Genome-wide association studies of alcohol dependence, DSM-IV criterion count and individual criteria. Genes, brain, and behavior, 18(6), e12579. https://doi.org/10.1111/gbb.12579 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liu, M., Jiang, Y., Wedow, R., Li, Y., Brazel, D. M., Chen, F., Datta, G., Davila-Velderrain, J., McGuire, D., Tian, C., Zhan, X., 23 andMe Research Team, HUNT All-In Psychiatry, Choquet, H., Docherty, A. R., Faul, J. D., Foerster, J. R., Fritsche, L. G., Gabrielsen, M. E., Gordon, S. D., …Vrieze, S. (2019). Association studies of up to 1.2 million individuals yield new insights into the genetic etiology of tobacco and alcohol use. Nature Genetics, 51(2), 237244. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41588-018-0307-5 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marshal, M. P. (2003). For better or for worse? The effects of alcohol use on marital functioning. Clinical Psychology Review, 23(7), 959997. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2003.09.002 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martin, A. R., Kanai, M., Kamatani, Y., Okada, Y., Neale, B. M., & Daly, M. J. (2019). Clinical use of current polygenic risk scores may exacerbate health disparities. Nature Genetics, 51(4), 584591. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41588-019-0379-x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martin, J., Khramtsova, E. A., Goleva, S. B., Blokland, G., Traglia, M., Walters, R. K., Hübel, C., Coleman, J., Breen, G., Børglum, A. D., Demontis, D., Grove, J., Werge, T., Bralten, J., Bulik, C. M., Lee, P. H., Mathews, C. A., Peterson, R. E., Winham, S. J., & Wray, N. (2021). …Sex differences cross-disorder analysis group of the psychiatric genomics consortium (2021). Examining sex-differentiated genetic effects across neuropsychiatric and behavioral traits, Biological psychiatry, 89(12), 11271137. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2020.12.024 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mulia, N., Ye, Y., Greenfield, T. K., & Zemore, S. E. (2009). Disparities in alcohol-related problems among white, black, and hispanic Americans. Alcoholism, Clinical and Experimental Research, 33(4), 654662. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1530-0277.2008.00880.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Neale, Z. E., Kuo, S. I., & Dick, D. M. (2021). A systematic review of gene-by-intervention studies of alcohol and other substance use. Development and Psychopathology, 33(4), 14101427. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579420000590 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Osgood, D. W., Ragan, D. T., Wallace, L., Gest, S. D., Feinberg, M. E., & Moody, J. (2013). Peers and the emergence of alcohol use: Influence and selection processes in adolescent friendship networks. Journal of Research on Adolescence: The Official Journal of the Society for Research on Adolescence, 23(3), 500512. https://doi.org/10.1111/jora.12059 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peirce, R. S., Frone, M. R., Russell, M., & Cooper, M. L. (1996). Financial stress, social support, and alcohol involvement: A longitudinal test of the buffering hypothesis in a general population survey. Health Psychology: Official Journal of the Division of Health Psychology, American Psychological Association, 15(1), 3847. https://doi.org/10.1037//0278-6133.15.1.38 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peterson, R. E., Kuchenbaecker, K., Walters, R. K., Chen, C. Y., Popejoy, A. B., Periyasamy, S., Lam, M., Iyegbe, C., Strawbridge, R. J., Brick, L., Carey, C. E., Martin, A. R., Meyers, J. L., Su, J., Chen, J., Edwards, A. C., Kalungi, A., Koen, N., Majara, L., Schwarz, E., … Duncan, L. E. (2019). Genome-Wide Association Studies in Ancestrally Diverse Populations: Opportunities, Methods, Pitfalls, and Recommendations. Cell, 179(3), 589603. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cell.2019.08.051 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Plomin, R., DeFries, J. C., & Loehlin, J. C. (1977). Genotype-Environment interaction and correlation in the analysis of human behavior. Psychological Bulletin, 84(2), 309322. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.84.2.309 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Plomin, R., Haworth, C. M. A., & Davis, O. S. P. (2009). Common disorders are quantitative traits. Nature Reviews. Genetics, 10(12), 872878. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrg2670 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Procidano, M. E., & Heller, K. (1983). Measures of perceived social support from friends and from family: Three validation studies. American Journal of Community Psychology, 11(1), 124. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00898416 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reinelt, E., Aldinger, M., Stopsack, M., Schwahn, C., John, U., Baumeister, S. E., Grabe, H. J., & Barnow, S. (2014). High social support buffers the effects of 5-HTTLPR genotypes within social anxiety disorder. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 264(5), 433439. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00406-013-0481-5 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reinelt, E., Barnow, S., Stopsack, M., Aldinger, M., Schmidt, C. O., John, U., & Grabe, H. J. (2015). Social support and the serotonin transporter genotype (5-HTTLPR) moderate levels of resilience, sense of coherence, and depression. American Journal of Medical Genetics Part B: Neuropsychiatric Genetics, 168(5), 383391. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajmg.b.32322 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rosenberg, N. A., Huang, L., Jewett, E. M., Szpiech, Z. A., Jankovic, I., & Boehnke, M. (2010). Genome-wide association studies in diverse populations. Nature Reviews. Genetics, 11(5), 356366. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrg2760 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ruan, Y., Lin, Y. F., Feng, Y. C. A., Chen, C. Y., Lam, M., Guo, Z., He, L., Sawa, A., Martin, A. R., Qin, S., & Huang, H. (2022). Improving polygenic prediction in ancestrally diverse populations. Nature Genetics 54, 573580. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41588-022-01054-7 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Salvatore, J. E., Cho, S. B., & Dick, D. M. (2017). Genes, environments, and sex differences in alcohol research. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs, 78(4), 494501. https://doi.org/10.15288/jsad.2017.78.494 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Savage, J. E., Salvatore, J. E., Aliev, F., Edwards, A. C., Hickman, M., Kendler, K. S., Macleod, J., Latvala, A., Loukola, A., Kaprio, J., Rose, R. J., Chan, G., Hesselbrock, V., Webb, B. T., Adkins, A., Bigdeli, T. B., Riley, B. P., & Dick, D. M. (2018). Polygenic risk score prediction of alcohol dependence symptoms across population-based and clinically ascertained samples, Alcoholism, Clinical and Experimental Research, 42(3), 520530. https://doi.org/10.1111/acer.13589 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schaefer, J. D., Jang, S.-K, Clark, D. A., Deak, J. D., Hicks, B. M., Iacono, W. G., Liu, M., McGue, M., Vrieze, S. I., & Wilson, S. (2023). Associations between polygenic risk of substance use and use disorder and alcohol, cannabis, and nicotine use in adolescence and young adulthood in a longitudinal twin study. Psychological Medicine, 53(6), 22962306. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291721004116 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Serido, J., Lawry, C., Li, G., Conger, K. J., & Russell, S. T. (2014). The associations of financial stress and parenting support factors with alcohol behaviors during young adulthood. Journal of Family and Economic Issues, 35(3), 339350. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10834-013-9376-x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Su, J., Kuo, S., Meyers, J., Guy, M., & Dick, D. (2018). Examining interactions between genetic risk for alcohol problems, peer deviance, and interpersonal traumatic events on trajectories of alcohol use disorder symptoms among African American college students. Development and Psychopathology, 30(5), 17491761. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579418000962 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Su, J., Kuo, S. I.-C., Aliev, F., Chan, G., Edenberg, H. J., Kamarajan, C., McCutcheon, V. V., Meyers, J. L., Schuckit, M., Tischfield, J., & Dick, D. M. (2021). The associations between polygenic risk, sensation seeking, social support, and alcohol use in adulthood. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 130(5), 525536. https://doi.org/10.1037/abn0000568 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Su, J., Supple, A. J., Leerkes, E. M., & Kuo, S. I.-C. (2019). Latent trajectories of alcohol use from early adolescence to young adulthood: Interaction effects between 5-HTTLPR and parenting quality and gender differences. Development and Psychopathology, 31(2), 457469. https://doi.org/10.1017/S095457941800024X CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Su, J., Trevino, A. D., Kuo, S. I., Aliev, F., Williams, C. D., Guy, M. C., Spit for Science Working Group, & Dick, D. M. (2022). Racial discrimination and alcohol problems: Examining interactions with genetic risk and impulsivity among African American Young Adults. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 51(8), 15521567. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-022-01609-1 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thomas, N. S., Barr, P., Aliev, F., Stephenson, M., Kuo, S. I., Chan, G., Dick, D. M., Edenberg, H. J., Hesselbrock, V., Kamarajan, C., Kuperman, S., & Salvatore, J. E. (2022). Principal component analysis reduces collider bias in polygenic score effect size estimation. Behavior genetics, 52(4-5), 268280. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10519-022-10104-z CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Turner, H. A. (1994). Gender and social support: Taking the bad with the good? Sex roles. A Journal of Research, 30(7-8), 521541. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01420800 Google Scholar
Verhulst, B., Neale, M. C., & Kendler, K. S. (2015). The heritability of alcohol use disorders: A meta-analysis of twin and adoption studies. Psychological Medicine, 45(5), 10611072. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291714002165 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Walen, H. R., & Lachman, M. E. (2000). Social support and strain from partner, family, and friends: Costs and benefits for men and women in adulthood. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 17(1), 530. https://doi.org/10.1177/0265407500171001 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Walters, R. K., Polimanti, R., Johnson, E. C., McClintick, J. N., Adams, M. J., Adkins, A. E., Aliev, F., Bacanu, S.-A., Batzler, A., Bertelsen, S., Biernacka, J. M., Bigdeli, T. B., Chen, L.-S., Clarke, T.-K., Chou, Y.-L., Degenhardt, F., Docherty, A. R., Edwards, A. C., Fontanillas, P., Foo, J. C., … Agrawal, A. (2018). Transancestral GWAS of alcohol dependence reveals common genetic underpinnings with psychiatric disorders. Nature Neuroscience, 21(12), 16561669. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593-018-0275-1 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wetherill, L., Lai, D., Johnson, E. C., Anokhin, A., Bauer, L., Bucholz, K. K., Dick, D. M., Hariri, A. R., Hesselbrock, V., Kamarajan, C., Kramer, J., Kuperman, S., Meyers, J. L., Nurnberger, J. I. Jr, Schuckit, M., Scott, D. M., Taylor, R. E., Tischfield, J., Porjesz, B.Agrawal, A. (2019 Jul). Genome-wide association study identifies loci associated with liability to alcohol and drug dependence that is associated with variability in reward-related ventral striatum activity in African- and European-Americans. Genes Brain and Behavior, 18(6), e12580. https://doi.org/10.1111/gbb.12580 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Williams, D. R., Mohammed, S. A., Leavell, J., & Collins, C. (2010). Race, socioeconomic status, and health: Complexities, ongoing challenges, and research opportunities. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1186(1), 69101. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.05339.x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zapolski, T. C. B., Pedersen, S. L., McCarthy, D. M., & Smith, G. T. (2014). Less drinking, yet more problems: Understanding African American drinking and related problems. Psychological Bulletin, 140(1), 188223. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0032113 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zhou, H., Sealock, J. M., Sanchez-Roige, S., Clarke, T.-M, Levey, D. F., Cheng, Z., Li, B., Polimanti, R., Kember, R. L., Smith, R. V., Thygesen, J. H., Morgan, M. Y., Atkinson, S. R., Thursz, M. R., Nyegaard, M., Mattheisen, M., Børglum, A. D., Johnson, E. C., Justice, A. C., Palmer, A. A., … Gelernter, J. (2020). Genome-Wide meta-analysis of problematic alcohol use in 435,563 individuals yields insights into biology and relationships with other traits. Nature Neuroscience, 23, 809818. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593-020-0643-5 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Bivariate correlations among key study variables for European Americans and African Americans

Figure 1

Table 2. Predicting alcohol use from alcohol use polygenic scores and social support among European American adults

Figure 2

Figure 1. Alc-PRS by friend support interaction in relation to alcohol use among European Americans. Predicted values of log-transformed alcohol use (drinks per week) are plotted at prototypical values (+1/−1 SD) of alc-PRS and friend support.

Figure 3

Table 3. Predicting alcohol use from alcohol use polygenic scores and social support among African American adults

Figure 4

Table 4. Predicting alcohol use from alcohol use polygenic scores and social support: testing developmental differences among European Americans adults

Figure 5

Table 5. Predicting alcohol use from alcohol use polygenic scores and social support: testing developmental differences among African American adults

Figure 6

Figure 2. Alc-PRS by friend support interaction in relation to alcohol use among African Americans in middle adulthood. Predicted values of log-transformed alcohol use (drinks per week) are plotted at prototypical values (+1/−1 SD) of alc-PRS and friend support.

Supplementary material: File

Su et al. supplementary material

Su et al. supplementary material
Download Su et al. supplementary material(File)
File 50.4 KB