1. Introduction
“My experience is what I agree to attend to. Only those items which I notice shape my mind.” – William James
During the last decade, there has been increasing interest in negative symptoms (NS) of schizophrenia together with a re-evaluation of the scales measuring them. Novel instruments have been developed although they have yet to be generally adopted, whilst studies based on traditional scales appear to be to some extent unreliable.
The characteristics of the Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS) [Reference Kay, Fiszbein and Opler1] and the Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symptoms (SANS) [Reference Andreasen2] have been a matter of discussion over the last 20 years. For example, further PANSS-subscales including four, five, or six factors have been proposed with several studies underlining that five-factor models show an adequate reliability when tested in different subgroups of individuals with schizophrenia, confirming the suitability of this approach [Reference Wallwork, Fortgang, Hashimoto, Weinberger and Dickinson3, Reference Nicotra, Casu, Piras and Marchese4]. On the other hand, cross-sectional studies of the SANS identify three, four and five different symptom factors; and longitudinal research has replicated three factors [Reference Levine and Leucht5]. In particular, the NS subscale within the traditional PANSS consists of seven items tapping blunted affect, emotional withdrawal, poor rapport, passive/apathetic social withdrawal, difficulty in abstract thinking, lack of spontaneity, and stereotyped thinking. And the original SANS consists of 19 items representing five domains: affective flattering, alogia, avolition-apathy, anhedonia-asociality, and attention.
A number of studies have adapted these scales to provide the two dimensions of NS, Diminished Expression and Amotivation/Avolition. For the PANSS, Liemburg et al. [Reference Liemburg, Castelein, Stewart, van der Gaag, Aleman and Knegtering6] studied the two-factor structure for NS in early psychosis participants. These factors were named “core NS”, related to the expressive deficits, and “social emotive withdrawal”, described as social amotivation. Similarly, Fervaha et al. [Reference Fervaha, Foussias, Agid and Remington7] extended these findings to patients with chronic schizophrenia, calling the two factors “diminished expression” and “amotivation”; and comparable two-factor results were recently published by Lim and colleagues [Reference Lim, Lee, Lam, Rapisarda, Kraus and Keefe8]. For the SANS, similar factor models accounting for diminished expression and anhedonia-asociality have emerged. For example Sayers et al. [Reference Sayers, Curran and Mueser9] confirmed a general three factor approach for the SANS including “diminished expression”, “inattention-alogia” and “social amotivation”, while Kelley et al. studied primary and secondary negative symptoms of schizophrenia employing the two factor approach of “affective flattening” and “diminished amotivation” [Reference Kelley, van Kammen and Allen10].
Factor analysis studies have indicated that cognitive items in the PANSS and SANS do not cohere well with the other NS ratings [Reference Blanchard, Kring, Horan and Gur11], and cognitive deficit appears to be conceptually distinct from NS [e.g. [Reference Harvey, Koren, Reichenberg and Bowie12]. Possible confounding instances include items of “difficulty of abstract thinking” and “stereotyped thinking” in the PANSS, and the “attention” subscale in the SANS (See Blanchard and Cohen for a review [Reference Blanchard and Cohen13]). As an illustration, Bell et al. [Reference Bell and Mishara14] demonstrated that performance on neuropsychological tests was associated with the cognitive component of the PANSS (“abstract thinking” and “stereotyped thinking”) but not with other NS items within the PANSS. For the SANS, Vadhan et al. [Reference Vadhan, Serper, Harvey, Chou and Cancro15] found a correlation between the “attention” subscale and neuropsychological tasks which discriminated “attention” from the other SANS subscales. Similarly, Liemburg et al. [Reference Liemburg, Castelein, Stewart, van der Gaag, Aleman and Knegtering6] and Lim et al. [Reference Lim, Lee, Lam, Rapisarda, Kraus and Keefe8] reported an association between the “diminished expression” PANSS factor and cognition.
Both cognitive impairment and NS are formally considered as core features of schizophrenia contributing to poor functional and community outcomes (e.g. [Reference Green, Kern and Heaton16, Reference Milev, Ho, Arndt and Andreasen17]). The present study was motivated by the ongoing utilization of traditional approaches to the PANSS and the SANS albeit the limitations stated above. Our aim is to show possible misleading associations between negative symptoms and cognition when using the original PANSS and SANS factors, and to offer alternatives to overcome them while still utilizing the PANSS and the SANS.
Specifically, our aim is to illustrate how the traditional PANSS and SANS may perform differently on the associations between NS and cognition depending on the factor approaches utilized. Findings from a previous study by our group suggested that NS could hamper the expression of cognition on behavioural tasks and functional outcomes [Reference Farreny, Aguado, Ochoa, Haro and Usall18]. These findings were of interest since common theoretical backgrounds generally assume that cognition would have an effect on NS (e.g., [Reference Cella, Stahl, Morris, Keefe, Bell and Wykes19]) but not vice versa.
Finally, illness duration will be taken into account as a confounder variable since the study included chronic and institutionalized participants and this might perform a detrimental effect on both cognition and NS. Our hypothesis is that longer illness duration may have an impact on the association between NS and cognitive performance, particularly in hospitalized participants. A decline in cognition has been also reported after ten years of illness duration [Reference Øie, Sundet and Rund20] and in geriatric patients with schizophrenia [Reference Harvey, Silverman, Mohs, Parrella, White and Powchik21]. Likewise, chronicity and hypostimulating environments can cause secondary negative symptoms such as decreased spontaneity, reduced curiosity, reduced drive to interact and blunted affect [Reference Oshima, Mino and Inomata22, Reference Kirschner, Aleman and Kaiser23].
2. Method
2.1. Participants
Two samples of participants with schizophrenia were retrospectively studied. Both groups belonged to the same mental health services from Barcelona metropolitan area and were recruited in independent investigations. Group 1 involved outpatients recruited with the purpose of studying the efficacy of Cognitive Remediation group treatment [Reference Farreny, Aguado, Ochoa, Huerta-Ramos, Marsà and López-Carrilero24]. Group 2 included inpatients recruited to study cognitive impairment in schizophrenia [Reference Brébion, Stephan-Otto, Huerta-Ramos, Ochoa, Usall and Abellán-Vega25]. Both studies were approved by the Parc Sanitari Sant Joan de Déu Ethics Committee.
Group 1- Sixty-two participants with a diagnosis of schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder following DSM-IV criteria were recruited from Parc Sanitari Sant Joan de Déu community services [Reference American Psychiatric Association26]. To verify the stability of the diagnosis we checked the medical histories to corroborate that the required DSM-IV criteria were appropriately described. Two cases were unconfirmed and the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (SCID; [Reference First, Spitzer, Gibbon and Williams27]) was utilized to verify their diagnoses. The participants included were between 18 and 65 years of age, with disease duration of over two years. Patients were excluded if they were suffering acute illness exacerbation that required hospitalization, had intellectual disability or neurological disorder, had switched antipsychotic drugs the month before the assessment, and/or had a diagnosis of alcohol or drug dependence within 6 months prior to inclusion. Initially, 70 participants referred by their community teams or rehabilitation services were assessed for eligibility. Of these, two were excluded for not meeting inclusion criteria (change of diagnosis to bipolar disorder and presence of learning disability), four refused consent, and two were not interested.
Group 2 – Fifty-five participants with schizophrenia were recruited from Parc Sanitari Sant Joan de Déu inpatient services. The diagnosis was made by consensus on the basis of DSM-IV criteria by two experienced psychiatrists who used patient histories and chart reviews. Inclusion criteria were age between 18 and 65, fluency in Spanish, and the capacity to provide informed consent. Exclusion criteria were current or recent alcohol or drug abuse (DSM-IV criteria), organic mental disease, intellectual disability, history of brain injury, dementia, and current severe physical disease. Participants were hospitalized and had been receiving stabilized doses of antipsychotic medication over two weeks at the time of testing. Clinical records were reviewed thoroughly by the psychiatrist recruiting the participants (J Cuevas-Esteban) and only those inpatients meeting all inclusion criteria were asked to participate. The rates of consent were about 75% of those eligible to take part.
For both groups the antipsychotic medication included first-generation antipsychotics (clotiapine, fluphenazine, haloperidol, levomepromazine, zuclopenthixol) as well as second-generation (amisulpride, aripiprazole, clozapine, olanzapine, quetiapine, risperidone). Predominantly within Group 2, participants were taking a combination of two or more antipsychotic drugs and/or were administered benzodiazepines (clonazepam, diazepam, flunitrazepam, lorazepam, lormetazepam) and/or antidepressant (duloxetine, fluoxetine, paroxetine, trazodone) medication.
2.2. Measures
Group 1- The cognitive assessment included the following domains: Executive Function using the Behavioural Assessment of the Dysexecutive Syndrome (BADS) [Reference Wilson, Alderman, Burgess, Emslie and Evans28], which consists of six tests involving cognitive flexibility, inhibition of impulsive responses, planning and organization, working memory, and time-estimation capacity. Attention, processing speed, and cognitive flexibility were measured with The Trail Making Test forms A and B (TMT A; TMT B) [Reference Reitan and Wolfson29]. Verbal memory, both immediate and delayed, was assessed with the Logical Memory I and II subscales respectively, from the Wechsler Memory Scale (WMS-III) [Reference Wechsler30].
Negative symptoms were measured with the Spanish validation of the PANSS [Reference Peralta Martín and Cuesta Zorita31]. The negative PANSS factors employed in this study were the original 3-factor approach by Kay et al. [Reference Kay, Fiszbein and Opler1] including 7 items: blunted affect, emotional withdrawal, poor rapport, passive-apathetic social withdrawal, lack of spontaneity, difficulty in abstract thinking, and stereotyped thinking. Also used was the consensual 5-factor approach by Wallwork et al. [Reference Wallwork, Fortgang, Hashimoto, Weinberger and Dickinson3] including 6 items: blunted affect, emotional withdrawal, poor rapport, passive-apathetic social withdrawal, lack of spontaneity, and motor retardation.
Group 2- Cognition was evaluated through several tasks: Cognitive speed was measured using the WAIS-III [Reference Wechsler32] Digit Symbol Substitution Test (DSST). The time required to read the whole list of the colour reading part of the Stroop test [Reference Stroop33] was recorded to evaluate motor speed. Verbal recall (frequent and rare words) was assessed with 2 lists of words consisting of 16 high-frequency words and 16 low-frequency words [Reference Algarabel, Ruiz and Sanmartin34]. Working memory was measured through the WAIS-III Letter-number span using the total number of correct trials.
Negative symptoms were assessed by means of the Spanish version of the SANS [Reference Peralta and Cuesta35] following the traditional evaluation of 5 domains: affective flattening, alogia, avolition-apathy, anhedonia-asociality, and attention.
2.3. Statistical analysis
Linear regression models were computed to explore the effect of NS on the cognitive variables targeted. The dependent variables were the BADS, TMT A, TMT B, and the WMS-III subscales for Group 1. In contrast, the Digit Symbol Substitution Test, Stroop Test, memory of high-frequency and low-frequency words, and Letter-number span were the dependent variables for Group 2. Independent variables were NS measured with the PANSS and the SANS, respectively.
In a first step we computed the regression model with each NS scale for every cognitive outcome without modifications. For Group 1 this was done using both the PANSS (K) negative by Kay et al. and the PANSS (W) negative by Wallwork et al. For Group 2 we used the traditional SANS. Next, if the association between NS and cognitive outcomes resulted in p < 0.1 in the regression model, the negative factor was computed again without including the cognitive items (“abstract thinking” in the PANSS and “attention” in the SANS) to test them as separate predictors. Therefore, PANSS6 corresponded to the PANSS (K) negative excluding the item of “abstract thinking”, and SANS4 was used to refer the SANS without the “attention” subscale. No modifications were applied to the PANSS (W) negative since this factor already excludes the items of “abstract thinking” and “stereotyped thinking”. Finally, if the association between NS and the cognitive outcome was still significant, illness duration was controlled in the model. The statistical analysis was conducted using SPSS 17.0 [Reference IBM36].
3. Results
Descriptive characteristics for each group are shown in Table 1.
Group 1- Regression models using the PANSS negative factors by Kay et al. [Reference Kay, Fiszbein and Opler1] and Wallwork et al. [Reference Wallwork, Fortgang, Hashimoto, Weinberger and Dickinson3] are shown in Table 2.
Results were different depending on the factor approach utilized for the PANSS. Following the original 3-factor model, Executive function and Visual and motor speed appeared to be predicted by NS, also showing a tendency towards association with delayed Logical memory (p = 0.08). When the item “abstract thinking” was accounted for separately in the model, the predictive value of negative symptoms over cognitive outcomes lost its significance. The case of the BADS was a clear illustration: the standardized regression coefficient (B) for the predictive value of the PANSS negative was initially −0.31, but when controlling for “abstract thinking”, the value was considerably reduced (B = −0.07) whilst the B coefficient for “abstract thinking” was −0.41.
In contrast, the negative factor by Wallwork et al. did not show any significant association with the cognitive outcomes assessed.
Group 2- Results from linear regression using the SANS are shown in Table 3. The regression model applied without controlling for covariables showed that levels of negative symptoms predicted the cognitive outcomes studied, with the exception of Verbal recall of rare words. In the second step, the SANS4 model accounting separately for the “attention” subscale demonstrated a tendency towards significance between Verbal recall of frequent words and “attention” (p = 0.05). In a third step, when illness duration was included in the model as a covariable, the association between Verbal recall of frequent words and “attention” proved stronger, but outcomes for Cognitive processing speed, Motor processing speed and Working memory became significantly predicted by illness chronicity. As an example, the SANS4 standardized B coefficient for Working memory was initially –0.31, but when controlling for “attention” and illness duration it became B = –0.05. However, the B coefficient for illness duration was –0.43.
4. Discussion
Our findings illustrate how traditional factor approaches to NS may actually conflate cognitive processes and negative symptoms (e.g., [Reference Blanchard, Kring, Horan and Gur11, Reference Daniel37]). Additionally, illness duration was demonstrated to predict cognitive performance within the sample of inpatients. According to the present study, the limitations found within the traditional PANSS and SANS can be addressed, so investigations employing these scales are encouraged to adopt more rigorous ways to discriminate NS.
4.1. The PANSS
There is extensive literature covering different factor approaches for the PANSS [Reference Kay, Fiszbein and Opler1] (see Blanchard and Cohen [Reference Blanchard and Cohen13]). Current research has called for a 5-factor solution demonstrating more precise and homogeneous definition of symptom dimensions [Reference Nicotra, Casu, Piras and Marchese4]; the present research shows that the consensual PANSS Negative factor by Wallwork et al. [Reference Wallwork, Fortgang, Hashimoto, Weinberger and Dickinson3] including 6 items (blunted affect, emotional withdrawal, poor rapport, passive-apathetic social withdrawal, lack of spontaneity, and motor retardation) may be a convenient alternative to the original approach. The factor approach of Marder et al. [Reference Marder, Davis and Chouinard38] is among other popular options to fit PANSS data excluding the items of “abstract thinking” and “stereotyped thinking” from the NS domain. In this study, controlling for “abstract thinking” was enough to demonstrate the misleading association between NS and cognition when employing the Kay et al. [Reference Kay, Fiszbein and Opler1] approach to the PANSS supporting previous findings [Reference Bell, Lysaker, Milstein and Beam-Goulet39].
4.2. The SANS
In the case of the SANS this study was carried out involving chronic institutionalized participants. Both the “attention” subscale and illness duration were demonstrated to predict cognitive performance instead of NS. The “attention” subscale was associated with verbal memory; while chronicity predicted deficits in processing speed and working memory ratifying the impact of illness duration on cognition [Reference Øie, Sundet and Rund20, Reference Harvey, Silverman, Mohs, Parrella, White and Powchik21, Reference Rajji and Mulsant40]. There has been ample discussion about the best factor approach fitting the SANS data, with evidence suggesting that the inclusion of “attention” in ratings of NS is somewhat problematic [Reference Blanchard and Cohen13]. Research suggests the 3-factor approach (affective-flattering, asociality, and alogia/attentiveness) might be more parsimonious, and it showed better validity [Reference Sayers, Curran and Mueser9, Reference Levine and Leucht5]; but this approach includes items from the “attention” subscale and this may not resolve the link with cognition. We suggest taking the “attention” subscale into account especially when investigations aim to distinguish precisely between cognition and NS.
New measures for the assessment of NS have been developed following the NHIM consensus meeting on NS [Reference Kirkpatrick, Fenton, Carpenter and Marder41]. These are the Clinical Assessment Interview for Negative Symptoms (CAINS) [Reference Horan, Kring, Gur, Reise and Blanchard42, Reference Kring, Gur, Blanchard, Horan and Reise43] and the Brief Negative Symptom Scale (BNSS) [Reference Kirkpatrick, Strauss, Nguyen, Fischer, Daniel and Cienfuegos44, Reference Strauss, Keller, Buchanan, Gold, Fischer and McMahon45]. These scales are a stimulating outcome and are promising for the field although they remain to be generally implemented. Nonetheless, recent studies have shown small to moderate associations between the CAINS and cognition, comparable to the associations found when utilizing other negative symptom scales [Reference Blanchard, Bradshaw, Garcia, Nasrallah, Harvey and Casey46]. The BNSS has also pointed to some overlap between diminished expression and cognition [Reference Hartmann-Riemer, Hager, Kirschner, Bischof, Kluge and Seifritz47]. On the whole, it seems necessary to further explore the association and shared pathophysiology between NS and cognition. According to the present results, NS would not have an effect on participants’ cognitive performance, but chronicity could have a negative impact on cognition in hospitalized patients. The distinction between NS and cognition has been cause for debate over several years. Some authors have argued that NS may be underpinned by cognitive deficits such as the impaired initiation of novel responses (e.g. [Reference Greenwood, Morris, Sigmundsson, Landau and Wykes48]) while others have suggested that patients with higher levels of NS have particular impairments in reasoning and executive function (e.g. [Reference Ventura, Hellemann, Thames, Koellner and Nuechterlein49]). However, several studies have failed to establish a relationship between NS and cognition, leading to the conclusion that they represent semi-autonomous disease processes (e.g. [Reference Kirkpatrick, Fenton, Carpenter and Marder41, Reference Foussias and Remington50]); and the correlation between cognition and NS may vary as a function of the definition of the NS construct [Reference Harvey, Koren, Reichenberg and Bowie12]. Therefore, research and clinical communities are still facing the challenging situation of properly recognising and targeting NS. Understanding NS means identifying them among other confounder variables, and being able to measure them in the most precise way. This matter has not been resolved yet, in particular with regard to their association with cognition.
This study has a number of limitations that need to ne noted. First, this is a retrospective analysis with two samples initially recruited for two different purposes. As such, cognitive and clinical measures employed were chosen according to the aims of each particular investigation so the measurement of cognitive variables was not performed with the same scales. In addition, some of the participants from Group 2 were chronic and had had years of institutionalization, which carries confusion with regard to the specific causes of their deficit (e.g., [Reference Harvey, Silverman, Mohs, Parrella, White and Powchik21, Reference Kirschner, Aleman and Kaiser23]). Third, the alternatives suggested to compensate for the cognitive items within the SANS and the PANSS may not to be exhaustive and we have not considered alternative models, for example the PANSS with four and six factors. Finally, some question marks have already arisen concerning the proposed factor approaches (e.g., reporting an association between cognition and NS although convenient modifications were adopted [Reference Liemburg, Castelein, Stewart, van der Gaag, Aleman and Knegtering6, Reference Lim, Lee, Lam, Rapisarda, Kraus and Keefe8]), casting some doubt on our findings and supporting the idea that NS and cognitive deficits may overlap and may not be independent to each other.
Despite these limitations, the present study has demonstrated that when relying on old but still widely used measures such as the PANSS and the SANS, it is advisable to avoid items too closely related to cognition. This is especially important in comprehending NS in schizophrenia, and in the development of effective targeted treatment approaches.
Conflicts of interest
None.
Acknowledgement
This research did not receive any specific grant from funding agencies in the public, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors.
Comments
No Comments have been published for this article.