Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-xbtfd Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-17T14:09:45.319Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Inhibitory and facilitative effects of lexical neighbors in spoken word recognition: The role of language experience

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  29 November 2023

Mona Roxana Botezatu*
Affiliation:
Department of Speech, Language and Hearing Sciences, University of Missouri, Columbia, MO, USA
Dalia L. Garcia
Affiliation:
San Diego State University/University of California, San Diego, Joint Doctoral Program in Language and Communicative Disorders, San Diego, CA, USA
*
Corresponding author: Mona Roxana Botezatu; Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

The study evaluated whether the direction (inhibitory or facilitative) of the phonological neighborhood density effect in English spoken word recognition was modulated by the relative strength of competitor activation (neighborhood type) in two groups of English-dominant learners of Spanish who differed in language experience. Classroom learners and heritage learners of Spanish identified spoken English words from dense (e.g., BEAR) and sparse (e.g., BOAT) phonological neighborhoods presented in moderate noise. The phonological neighborhood was separately manipulated at word onset (cohort) and word offset (rhyme). Classroom learners were overall slower in recognizing spoken words from denser neighborhoods. Strongly active (onset) neighbors exerted inhibitory effects in both classroom and heritage learners. Critically, weakly active (offset) neighbors exerted inhibitory effects in classroom learners but facilitative effects in heritage learners. The results suggest that the activation of both within and cross-language neighbors should be considered in determining the direction of neighbor effects in bilingual lexical processing.

Type
Research Report
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Achieving proficiency in a second language can be a matter of choice or circumstance. For those who choose to learn a second language (L2), the process typically involves classroom-based instruction with or without L2 immersion opportunities. In contrast, for those who learn an L2 as a result of circumstance, the process typically involves naturalistic exposure to a heritage language in the home. The resulting language learning experience differs on many other aspects, including the timing of L2 exposure (late in classroom learners, early in heritage learners) and input modality (spoken and written input for classroom learners, spoken input for heritage learners; for a review, see Montrul, Reference Montrul2012). Both groups of learners maintain dominance of the majority language and achieve varying degrees of L2 proficiency. Functionally, the majority of classroom learners remains monolingual, whereas heritage learners become bilingual.

Although much research has evaluated how such differences in language learning experience may affect L2 outcomes (Dewey, Reference Dewey2004; Hartshorne et al., Reference Hartshorne, Tenenbaum and Pinker2018), there has been considerably less work on how the distinct paths to L2 proficiency may affect lexical processing in the dominant language (Botezatu et al., Reference Botezatu, Kroll, Trachsel and Guo2022b; Linck et al., Reference Linck, Kroll and Sunderman2009). In the current study, we considered whether the distinct language experience of classroom and heritage learners of Spanish affects the relative strength of within and cross-language lexical neighbor activation, influencing the direction (inhibitory or facilitative) of the effect of lexical neighbors during spoken word recognition in the dominant language (i.e., English).

Spoken word recognition involves mapping of the unfolding speech signal onto the lexicon (for a review, see Mirman, Reference Mirman and Mirkovic2016). Interactive activation models such as the TRACE model (McClelland & Elman, Reference McClelland and Elman1986) propose that this mapping occurs incrementally, as listeners temporarily activate a broad network of similar-sounding words in their lexicon that partially match the input, resulting in competition among multiple lexical candidates. The competition represents an index of the relative difficulty of mapping the speech signal onto the lexicon and may be operationalized in terms of phonological neighborhood density (Luce, Reference Luce1986), a measure of the number and average frequency of phonologically similar lexical candidates that partially match the unfolding speech (Luce & Pisoni, Reference Luce and Pisoni1998).

Phonological neighbors have generally been reported to exert inhibitory effects on spoken word recognition (e.g., Dufour & Frauenfelder, Reference Dufour and Frauenfelder2010; Luce & Pisoni, Reference Luce and Pisoni1998; Magnuson et al., Reference Magnuson, Dixon, Tanenhaus and Aslin2007), but facilitative effects have also been reported (Vitevitch & Rodríguez, Reference Vitevitch and Rodríguez2005). Inhibitory effects have been related to the activation of a larger number of phonologically similar neighbors during recognition of spoken words from denser than sparser neighborhoods, resulting in slower and less accurate recognition of words from denser neighborhoods. Phonological neighbors may also exert an indirect facilitative effect in spoken word recognition through excitatory feedback connections that support shared phonological units. However, this facilitative effect is generally weaker than the direct inhibitory effect. Phonological neighbors have also been shown to exert facilitative effects in visual word recognition (see Andrews, Reference Andrews1997, for a review) and spoken word production (Kittredge et al., Reference Kittredge, Dell, Verkuilen and Schwartz2008; Vitevitch & Sommers, Reference Vitevitch and Sommers2003), suggesting that the inhibitory or facilitative direction of the effect may differ across tasks. In an attempt to provide a unified account of the inhibitory and facilitative effects of lexical neighbors, Chen and Mirman (Reference Chen and Mirman2012) used a simple interactive activation and competition model to simulate phonological, orthographic, and semantic neighbor effects in word recognition and production tasks. The authors used the same model and parameter values across simulations to discover the computational principle that determined whether neighbor effects were facilitative or inhibitory across domains. For the spoken domain, the architecture of the interactive activation and competition model used to simulate phonological neighbor effects in spoken word recognition tasks was closely related to the TRACE model. The results revealed that the overall strength of neighbor activation determined the direction (inhibitory or facilitative) of neighbor effects across simulations. Specifically, Chen and Mirman (Reference Chen and Mirman2012) found that the inhibitory effect outweighed the facilitative effect for strongly active neighbors, whereas the facilitative effect outweighed the inhibitory effect for weakly active neighbors. In the current study, we seek to determine whether this computational principle (i.e., the strength of neighbor activation determines the inhibitory or facilitative direction of neighbor effects) is supported in the spoken domain by performance in two groups of English-dominant learners of Spanish who differ in the strength of within and cross-language competitor activation as a function of language experience.

The current study

The study evaluated whether the direction (inhibitory or facilitative) of the phonological neighborhood density effect in English spoken word recognition was modulated by the relative strength of competitor activation (i.e., neighborhood type) in two groups of English-dominant learners of Spanish who differed in language experience: classroom learners of Spanish who were exposed to Spanish in an educational setting after the age of 10 and were functionally English monolingual and heritage learners of Spanish who were exposed to Spanish in the home environment prior to the age of 5 and were functionally bilingual. We chose to test classroom and heritage learners of Spanish for this study because both groups activate a broad set of within-language competitors during spoken word recognition in the dominant language (Marian & Spivey, Reference Marian and Spivey2003a, Reference Marian and Spivey2003b) and may experience at least occasional coactivation of their nondominant language (Blumenfeld & Marian, Reference Blumenfeld and Marian2007; Canseco-Gonzalez et al., Reference Canseco-Gonzalez, Brehm, Brick, Brown-Schmidt, Fischer and Wagner2010; Ju & Luce, Reference Ju and Luce2004). Critically, the two groups differ in age of Spanish acquisition and Spanish proficiency, which have been shown to influence the number of cross-language competitors and strength of their activation (Blumenfeld & Marian, Reference Blumenfeld and Marian2013; Canseco-Gonzalez et al., Reference Canseco-Gonzalez, Brehm, Brick, Brown-Schmidt, Fischer and Wagner2010). Bilinguals who acquire their L2 relatively early (i.e., before the age of 6) experience cross-language competition during spoken word recognition (Canseco-Gonzalez et al., Reference Canseco-Gonzalez, Brehm, Brick, Brown-Schmidt, Fischer and Wagner2010). This group comparison is critical in determining whether the direction of the phonological neighborhood density effect in spoken word recognition is only dependent on the strength of competitor activation or whether the relationship is mediated by participants’ language experience.

The spoken domain provides a unique opportunity to test this question because the activation of phonological neighbors that partially match the unfolding speech differs in timing and strength as a result of the incremental mapping of the speech signal onto the lexicon. Simulations with the TRACE model (McClelland & Elman, Reference McClelland and Elman1986) and empirical evidence from eye-tracking studies using the visual-world paradigm (Allopenna et al., Reference Allopenna, Magnuson and Tanenhaus1998; Magnuson et al., Reference Magnuson, Dixon, Tanenhaus and Aslin2007) suggest that the overall amount of activation and competition for cohort neighbors, which share the onset of the spoken word (operationalized as the first two phonemes), is much higher than for rhyme neighbors, which share the offset of the spoken word (operationalized as the vowel and coda). Early in the time course of spoken word recognition, cohorts are strongly active and compete more strongly for selection than rhyme neighbors. Late in the time course of spoken word recognition, rhymes might be more active than cohorts, but both are weakly active at that point. Most research on bilingual spoken word recognition has focused on competition dynamics among cohort neighbors, revealing robust within- (Bruggeman & Cutler, Reference Bruggeman and Cutler2019; Canseco-Gonzalez et al., Reference Canseco-Gonzalez, Brehm, Brick, Brown-Schmidt, Fischer and Wagner2010; Marian & Spivey, Reference Marian and Spivey2003b; Shin et al., Reference Shin, Bauman, MacPhee and Zevin2015) and across-language effects (Blumenfeld & Marian, Reference Blumenfeld and Marian2007; Canseco-Gonzalez et al., Reference Canseco-Gonzalez, Brehm, Brick, Brown-Schmidt, Fischer and Wagner2010; Marian & Spivey, Reference Marian and Spivey2003a, Reference Marian and Spivey2003b). Competition among offset neighbors has remained largely unexplored in bilinguals. The available evidence suggests that within-language rhyme competition in bilinguals may be either similar to that experienced by monolinguals (Shin et al., Reference Shin, Bauman, MacPhee and Zevin2015) or very weak (Bruggeman & Cutler, Reference Bruggeman and Cutler2019). Therefore, it is critical to consider whether differences in language experience may affect rhyme competition.

We hypothesize that if language experience modulates the overall strength of neighbor activation, then both monolinguals and bilinguals should exhibit an inhibitory effect for within-language neighbors that compete strongly for selection (i.e., cohort neighbors) but that the direction (inhibitory or facilitative) of the effect exhibited by within-language neighbors that exert weak competition (i.e., rhyme neighbors) may differ between groups as a function of degree of interference from cross-language competitors. If the overall strength of neighbor activation determines the direction of neighbor effects in lexical processing, as proposed by Chen and Mirman (Reference Chen and Mirman2012), then regardless of participants’ language experience, cohort neighbors should exert an inhibitory effect on spoken word recognition because they compete strongly for selection, whereas rhyme neighbors should exert a facilitative effect on spoken word recognition because they represent weaker competitors. Alternatively, if the strength of neighbor activation does not determine the direction of neighbor effect in lexical processing, then both cohort and rhyme neighbors should exert inhibitory effects on spoken word recognition, with the magnitude of the effect being larger for cohort than for rhyme neighbors. The results are expected to hold beyond individual differences in language proficiency, cognitive control, and working memory.

Method

Participants

Forty-five English-dominant classroom learners of Spanish (15 male, mean age = 20.3, age range = 18–27 years) and 37 English-dominant heritage learners of Spanish (8 male, mean age = 19.3, age range = 18–24 years) were recruited from the University of Missouri and the University of California, Riverside and completed the study for payment. Classroom learners lived in either unilingual (Midwest; n = 29) or diverse (West Coast; n = 16) linguistic environments on the United States and learned Spanish through classroom instruction after age 6. Heritage learners lived in a linguistically diverse environment on the U.S. West Coast, where they grew up in Spanish-speaking households and were exposed to English before the age of 6, being schooled almost exclusively in English. All participants reported normal hearing and vision and no history of neurological, language, reading, or learning disorders. Participants varied on lab-based measures of language proficiency (see Table 1) and cognitive resources (see Table 2). Both groups exhibited higher proficiency in English than Spanish and reported a strong preference for speaking and reading English over Spanish.

Table 1. Mean (standard error) psycholinguistic data

Table 2. Mean (standard error) for demographic and cognitive measures

Materials and procedure

Participants completed a set of tasks in E-Prime 2.0 (Psychology Software Tools Incorporated, 2012) that measured English spoken word recognition, language production proficiency, working memory, and cognitive control. A brief description of each task is provided below. For full details of the experimental procedures, see Botezatu, Guo, et al. (Reference Botezatu, Guo, Kroll, Peterson and Garcia2022).

English spoken word recognition

Spoken word recognition was measured using a spoken-to-written word-matching task. Phonological neighborhood density effects have been observed using this task in a variety of populations, such as monolinguals (Botezatu et al., Reference Botezatu, Landrigan, Chen and Mirman2015; Botezatu & Mirman, Reference Botezatu and Mirman2019), bilinguals (Botezatu et al., Reference Botezatu, Kroll, Trachsel and Guo2022a, Reference Botezatu, Kroll, Trachsel and Guo2022b) and individuals with poststroke aphasia (Botezatu & Mirman, Reference Botezatu and Mirman2019), although the effect has not been observed in other close-set formats (Sommers et al., Reference Sommers, Kirk and Pisoni1997). Spoken words were 120 monosyllabic English words that varied systematically in phonological neighborhood density, which was separately manipulated at word onset (i.e., first two phonemes, cohort neighborhood) and word offset (i.e., vowel and coda, rhyme neighborhood). In the cohort condition, density was operationalized as the number, t(58) = 4.4, p < .001, and summed frequency of onset neighbors, t(58) = 4.23, p < .001, while the equivalent measures of the offset neighborhood were held constant (all ps > .05). In the rhyme condition, density was operationalized as the number, t(58) = 10.47, p < .001, and summed frequency of offset neighbors, t(58) = 6.86, p < .001, while the equivalent measures of the onset neighborhood were held constant (all ps > .05). The manipulations of neighborhood density (dense versus sparse) and neighborhood type (cohort versus rhyme) resulted in four conditions of interest: dense cohorts (e.g., BEAR), sparse cohorts (e.g., BOAT), dense rhymes (e.g., BEAN), and sparse rhymes (e.g., BENCH). There were 30 trials in each condition. The conditions were matched on number of phonemes, lexical frequency, and auditory file length (all ps > .05). The conditions were further matched on the number and summed frequency of Spanish orthographic neighbors and on the number of Spanish phonological neighbors (all ps > 0.05; see Table A1 in the Appendix). The analysis of the summed frequency of Spanish phonological neighbors yielded a significant interaction between neighborhood density and neighborhood type, F(1, 26) = 5.09, p = .033. Follow-up analyses revealed a trend toward a significant difference in the summed frequency of Spanish phonological neighbors at the level of cohorts, t(59) = 2.26, p = .06, such that on average, dense cohorts tended to have higher summed frequency of Spanish phonological neighbors than sparse cohorts. This difference was driven by a few extreme values, as most items had no Spanish neighbors. In contrast, no difference on the summed frequency of Spanish phonological neighbors was detected at the level of rhymes, t(59) = -0.77, p = .46. The list of experimental items is found in Table A2 in the Appendix.

On each trial, participants listened to one of 120 monosyllabic English words presented at a signal-to-noise ratio of -2dB over headphones and used a mouse to select the word they heard from among an array of response options presented on the screen after a 1,000-ms delay. Each array of response options included the target word, two close distractors (i.e., a cohort neighbor and a rhyme neighbor), two distant distractors (i.e., an onset neighbor of the rhyme neighbor and an offset neighbor of the cohort neighbor), and an undecided response option “?” that were presented at a random location on each trial. Response latencies and accuracy rates were collected on each trial.

Proficiency

Proficiency was assessed using measures of verbal fluency (semantic and phonemic), expressive vocabulary, and word/nonword reading, as well as self-ratings. Semantic fluency measured the average number of correct responses (excluding repetitions) produced across four named semantic categories (i.e., animals, clothing, fruit, furniture), whereas phonemic fluency provided an equivalent measure in response to three named letters (i.e., “F,” “A,” and “S” for English; “P,” “M,” and “R” for Spanish). The Multilingual Naming Test (MiNT; Gollan et al., Reference Gollan, Weissberger, Runnqvist, Montoya and Cera2011) provided a measure of expressive vocabulary, indexed as the average correct-response latencies and accuracy rates in naming 68 line drawings of increasing difficulty. Speeded word reading was assessed using the Test of Word Reading Efficiency in English (TOWRE; Torgesen et al., Reference Torgesen, Wagner and Rashotte1999) and a Spanish equivalent (Miller et al., Reference Miller, Heilmann, Nockerts, Iglesias, Fabiano and Francis2006), each measuring the number of words of increasing difficulty (out of 104) read aloud correctly within 45 s. Phonemic decoding was tested in English only using the Phonemic Decoding subtest of the TOWRE (Torgesen et al., Reference Torgesen, Wagner and Rashotte1999), which measured the number of nonwords of increasing difficulty (out of 63) read correctly within 45 s. Participants self-reported their language background and proficiency using the Language Experience and Proficiency Questionnaire (Marian et al., Reference Marian, Blumenfeld and Kaushanskaya2007). Proficiency was computed for each language by adding the z scores of the semantic fluency, phonemic fluency, MiNT accuracy, sight word reading, and phonemic decoding (English only) measures in the relevant language.

Working memory

The operation span task (Turner & Engle, Reference Turner and Engle1989) was administered in English, the dominant language, to measure verbal working memory. On each of the 60 equation–word pairs presented in sets of two to six, participants made a button-press judgment of correctness in response to an arithmetic equation (e.g., 8 / 4 + 3 = 1) and then stored in memory a visually presented English word (e.g., FATHER) for recall at the end of a set. The working memory score reflected the number of correctly recalled words from trials with correct equation judgements. The only restriction on recall was that participants could not list the last word of a set in the initial position unless that was the only item from that list that they recalled.

Cognitive control

The Simon task (Simon & Rudell, Reference Simon and Rudell1967) was used to measure cognitive control. On each of the 126 trials, participants pressed one of two color-coded keys on the standard keyboard/Chronos Box in response to the color (not the location) of a red or blue square that appeared on the screen in three locations: in the middle of the screen (neutral), on the same side as the correct-response key (congruent), and on the opposite side of the correct-response key (incongruent). Cognitive control was operationalized as the difference in response latency between incongruent and congruent trials after excluding incorrect responses, absolute outliers (response latencies faster than 200 ms and slower than 2000 ms), and relative outliers (average response latency ±3 standard deviations).

Data analysis

The latencies of correct-response spoken word recognition were analyzed using linear mixed-effects models (Baayen et al., Reference Baayen, Davidson and Bates2008; Barr et al., Reference Barr, Levy, Scheepers and Tily2013). Response accuracy was analyzed using mixed-effects logistic regression (Baayen et al., Reference Baayen, Davidson and Bates2008; Barr et al., Reference Barr, Levy, Scheepers and Tily2013; Jaeger, Reference Jaeger2008). Analyses were implemented in R version 3.6.0 (R Development Core Team, 2016; http://cran.us.r-project.org/) using the lme4 package version 1.1-11 (Bates et al., Reference Bates, Maechler, Bolker, Walker, Bojesen Christensen, Singmann, Dai, Grothendieck and Green2016).

For each analysis, the base model included the fixed effect of neighborhood density (dense versus sparse) and a maximal random-effect structure consisting of random effects of participants and items and by-participant random slopes of density. Neighborhood type (cohort versus rhyme) was entered in the analysis as a fixed effect and was evaluated in terms of its overall effect on response latencies and accuracy rates, then in terms of its interaction with density (dense versus sparse) and group (classroom versus heritage speakers). Improvement in model fit for each of these steps was evaluated using the likelihood ratio test (χ2 test with degrees of freedom equal to the number of parameters added). Models controlled for individual differences in English and Spanish proficiency, working memory, and cognitive control. To ease the interpretation of the models, control measures were centered before being entered in the analysis. Parameter estimates for the full models are presented in Table 3. Simplified models that do not account for individual difference variables are reported in Table A3 in the Appendix.

Table 3. Predictor models of spoken word recognition latency and accuracy

Note. Full model formula: RT (or accuracy) ~ English proficiency + Spanish proficiency + Cognitive control + Working memory + Density*Neighborhood type*Group + (Density | participant) + (1 | item).

Results

First, incorrect-response trials (representing 22.3% of the data) were excluded from the response latency data but not from the response accuracy data. Then, relative outliers (trials with average response latency ± 3 standard deviations from participants and items means, representing 2.2% of the data) were excluded. Overall, participants were equally fast, b = 37.59, SE = 29.58, 95% CI [-20.39, 95.57], t = 1.27, p = .204, and accurate b = -0.12, SE = 0.17, 95% CI [-0.46, 0.22], z = -0.70, p = .485, in recognizing spoken English words from dense and sparse phonological neighborhoods. The effect of density on response latencies was modulated by group, b = -20.13, SE = 6.84, 95% CI [-33.52, -6.73], t = -2.94, p = .003. The standard inhibitory effect of phonological neighborhood density in spoken word recognition was replicated in classroom learners of Spanish but not heritage learners of Spanish.

There was also a trend toward a significant interaction between density and neighborhood type on response latencies, b = 52.99, SE = 29.58, 95% CI [-4.98, 110.97], t = 1.79, p = .073, which revealed that the density effect was larger in cohort than in rhyme neighborhoods. There was also an interaction between neighborhood type and group on response accuracy, χ2(3) = 9.70, p = .021, b = -0.09, SE = 0.03, 95% CI [-0.15, -0.03], t = -2.99, p = .003, such that heritage learners were more accurate in responding to rhymes, whereas classroom learners were equally accurate in both conditions. Critically, there was a three-way interaction among density, neighborhood type, and group on response latencies, χ2(3) = 13.11, p = .004, b = 14.14, SE = 6.83, 95% CI [0.76, 27.52], t = 2.07, p = .038, which revealed that dense cohort neighborhoods were associated with inhibitory effects in both groups. In contrast, dense rhyme neighborhoods yielded inhibitory effects in classroom learners but facilitative effects in heritage learners (see Figure 1, Panel A).

Figure 1. Spoken word recognition latencies (Panel A) and accuracy rates (Panel B) to English words from dense versus sparse cohort and rhyme neighborhoods in heritage and classroom learners of Spanish. Error bars indicate standard errors.

Discussion

The study evaluated whether the overall strength of neighbor activation determines the direction of phonological neighborhood density effects in spoken word recognition by experimentally manipulating the phonological neighborhood of target words at word onset and word offset (in separate conditions). Two groups of speakers who differed in the strength of within and cross-language competitor activation as a function of language experience were tested. Strongly active (i.e., cohort) neighbors exerted inhibitory effects in both classroom and heritage learners, whereas weakly active (i.e., rhyme) neighbors exerted inhibitory effects in classroom learners but facilitative effects in heritage learners.

The results support the hypothesis that language experience modulates the overall strength of neighbor activation. Strongly active (i.e., cohort) competitors (Allopenna et al., Reference Allopenna, Magnuson and Tanenhaus1998) have been shown to exert robust inhibitory effects in English spoken word recognition (Magnuson et al., Reference Magnuson, Dixon, Tanenhaus and Aslin2007). The current study replicates this pattern with monolinguals and bilinguals, suggesting that both groups experience competition from onset neighbors, leading to robust inhibitory effects of dense cohort neighborhoods. Rhyme neighbors have been shown to be weakly active competitors in monolinguals (Allopenna et al., Reference Allopenna, Magnuson and Tanenhaus1998). Conflicting evidence from research on bilingual spoken word recognition points to either robust rhyme competition in the L2 (Shin et al., Reference Shin, Bauman, MacPhee and Zevin2015) or no competition in either the L1 or the L2 (Bruggeman & Cutler, Reference Bruggeman and Cutler2019). In the current study, we found a trend toward a significant interaction between neighborhood density and neighborhood type, suggesting that, overall, participants exhibited smaller density effects for rhymes than cohorts. This suggests that overall participants experienced weaker activation of rhyme competitors, consistent with previous reports from monolinguals (Allopenna et al., Reference Allopenna, Magnuson and Tanenhaus1998) and bilinguals (Bruggeman & Cutler, Reference Bruggeman and Cutler2019).

Consideration of group differences in language experience suggests that the overall weaker activation of rhyme competitors results in inhibitory effects of dense rhyme neighborhoods in classroom learners, who were functionally monolingual speakers of English, but in facilitative effects of dense rhyme neighborhoods in heritage learners, who were functionally bilingual. The reversal of the density effect in weakly active competitors (i.e., rhymes) experienced by heritage learners may be interpreted in the context of a dynamic language system, where weakly active offset neighbors may become more open to the influence of neighbors from the nontarget language. Previous studies reported that bilinguals who acquire an L2 at a younger age, as is the case for heritage learners in this study, experience cross-language competition during spoken word recognition (Canseco-Gonzalez et al., Reference Canseco-Gonzalez, Brehm, Brick, Brown-Schmidt, Fischer and Wagner2010). Heritage learners also had higher Spanish proficiency than classroom learners in this study (all ps < .05), resulting in higher coactivation of Spanish competitors (Blumenfeld & Marian, Reference Blumenfeld and Marian2013). The current results suggest that in the context of weakly active within-language neighbors, the direction (inhibitory or facilitative) of the phonological neighborhood density effect may be influenced by the degree of cross-language competition.

The presence of noise in the speech signal might have influenced the overall activation of rhyme competitors in the two participant groups. The addition of noise in spoken word recognition has been shown to boost the activation of word offset neighbors in monolingual listeners (Brouwer & Bradlow, Reference Brouwer and Bradlow2011, Reference Brouwer and Bradlow2016; McQueen & Huettig, Reference McQueen and Huettig2012), which may explain why classroom learners, who were functionally monolingual, exhibited an inhibitory effect of phonological neighbors in both cohort and rhyme conditions. In contrast, bilingual listeners have been reported to strongly rely on word-initial information during spoken word recognition in noise (Coumans et al., Reference Coumans, Hout and Scharenborg2014; Hintz et al., Reference Hintz, Voeten, McQueen, Scharenborg, Fitch, Lamm, Leder and Teßmar-Raible2021), which may have contributed to the weak activation of rhyme competitors in the group of heritage learners, who were functionally bilingual. Additionally, noise has been shown to increase cross-language lexical competition during spoken word recognition (Guediche et al., Reference Guediche, Navarra-Barindelli and Martin2023).

Limitations and future directions

The current study suggests that differences in language experience may mediate the relationship between the strength of neighbor activation and the direction of neighbor effects in lexical processing, pointing to the dynamic nature of the bilingual lexicon. We consider the limitations of the current study. First, the spoken-to-written word-matching task used a close-set response format, which makes it more difficult to observe neighborhood density effects (Sommers et al., Reference Sommers, Kirk and Pisoni1997). Although we had successfully observed phonological neighborhood density effects using this task in a variety of participant populations (Botezatu et al., Reference Botezatu, Landrigan, Chen and Mirman2015; 2022a, 2022b; Botezatu & Mirman, Reference Botezatu and Mirman2019), future studies should test whether the results are replicated using open-set response formats. Second, the current study did not fully control of cross-language neighbors (i.e., the dense cohort condition had higher summed frequency of Spanish phonological neighbors than the sparse cohort condition due to a few extreme values). Future studies should evaluate whether results are replicated in conditions that manipulate within-language competitors while fully controlling for cross-language neighbors as well as in conditions in which within- and cross-language competitors are covaried. Third, as noted in the previous paragraph, the presence of noise in the speech signal might have influenced the overall activation of rhyme competitors. Future research should also test whether results are replicated when stimuli are not presented in noise and whether the pattern persists when evaluating the time course of spoken word recognition using eye-tracking, computer-mouse tracking, or electrophysiological methods, which provide a higher temporal resolution than behavioral methods. A fourth limitation is the relatively small sample of participants in each group. Results should be replicated with larger participant samples.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at http://doi.org/10.1017/S0272263123000578.

Competing interest

The author(s) declare none.

Acknowledgements

This research was supported by a Catalyst Award and a Richard Wallace Faculty Incentive Grant from the University of Missouri and an Advancing Academic-Research Careers Award from the American Speech-Language-Hearing Association to M. R. Botezatu. D. L. Garcia’s work on the project was supported by an NIH NIDCD T32 Training Grant 5T32DC007361-15. We would like to thank Judith F. Kroll for help with data collection at UCR. We would also like to thank Kathleen Acord, Madison Backes, Kinsey Bice, Ashley Bramer, Jennifer Calvin, Sierra Cheung, Sierra Clemetson, Sarah D’Amico, Ryley Ewy, Laura Fry, Madison Hinmon, Jaclyn Johnson, Hanna Lowther, Sarah Marx, Carlos Martinez Villar, Allie Mitan, Istvan Romhany, and Jason Wong for help with data collection, transcription, and coding.

References

Allopenna, P. D., Magnuson, J. S., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (1998). Tracking the time course of spoken word recognition using eye movements: Evidence for continuous mapping models. Journal of Memory and Language, 38, 419439. https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.1997.2558CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Andrews, S. (1997). The effect of orthographic similarity on lexical retrieval: Resolving neighborhood conflicts. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 4, 439461.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baayen, R. H., Davidson, D. J., & Bates, D. M. (2008). Mixed-effects modeling with crossed random effects for subjects and items. Journal of Memory and Language, 59, 390412. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2007.12.005CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barr, D. J., Levy, R., Scheepers, C., & Tily, H. J. (2013). Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: Keep it maximal. Journal of Memory and Language, 68, 255278. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2012.11.001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bates, D., Maechler, M., Bolker, B., Walker, S., Bojesen Christensen, R. H., Singmann, H., Dai, B., Grothendieck, G., & Green, P. (2016). lme4: Linear mixed-effects models using Eigen and S4. R package version 1.1-11. https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/lme4 [Computer software]. https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/lme4Google Scholar
Blumenfeld, H. K., & Marian, V. (2007). Constraints on parallel activation in bilingual spoken language processing: Examining proficiency and lexical status using eye-tracking. Language and Cognitive Processes, 22, 633660. https://doi.org/10.1080/01690960601000746CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blumenfeld, H. K., & Marian, V. (2013). Parallel language activation and cognitive control during spoken word recognition in bilinguals. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25, 547567. https://doi.org/10.1080/20445911.2013.812093CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Botezatu, M. R., Guo, T., Kroll, J. F., Peterson, S., & Garcia, D. (2022). Sources of variation in second and native language speaking proficiency among college-aged second language learners. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 44, 305330. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0272263121000188CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Botezatu, M. R., Kroll, J., F., Trachsel, M., & Guo, T. (2022a). Discourse fluency modulates spoken word recognition in monolingual and L2 speakers. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 25, 430443. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728921001024CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Botezatu, M. R., Kroll, J., F., Trachsel, M., & Guo, T. (2022b). Second language immersion impacts native language lexical production and comprehension. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 12, 347376. https://doi.org/10.1075/lab.19059CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Botezatu, M. R., Landrigan, J.-F., Chen, Q., & Mirman, D. (2015, July 22–25). Phonological neighborhood density modulates errors in spoken word recognition [Paper presentation]. Proceedings of the 37th Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, Pasadena, California, USA.Google Scholar
Botezatu, M. R., & Mirman, D. (2019). Impaired lexical selection and fluency in post-stroke aphasia. Aphasiology, 33, 667688. https://doi.org/10.1080/02687038.2018.1508637CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brouwer, S., & Bradlow, A. R. (2011). The influence of noise on phonological competition during spoken word recognition. Proceedings of the International Congress of Phonetic Sciences, 2011, 364367.Google ScholarPubMed
Brouwer, S., & Bradlow, A. R. (2016). The temporal dynamics of spoken word recognition in adverse listening conditions. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 45, 11511160. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10936-015-9396-9CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bruggeman, L., & Cutler, A. (2019, August 5–9). The dynamics of lexical activation and competition in bilinguals’ first versus second language [Paper presentation]. Nineteenth International Congress of Phonetic Sciences, Melbourne, Australia.Google Scholar
Canseco-Gonzalez, E., Brehm, L., Brick, C. A., Brown-Schmidt, S., Fischer, K., & Wagner, K. (2010). Carpet or carcel: The effect of age of acquisition and language mode on bilingual lexical access. Language and Cognitive Processes, 25, 669705. https://doi.org/10.1080/01690960903474912CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chen, Q., & Mirman, D. (2012). Competition and cooperation among similar representations: Toward a unified account of facilitative and inhibitory effects of lexical neighbors. Psychological Review, 119, 417430. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0027175CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coumans, J., Hout, R. V., & Scharenborg, O. (2014). Non-native word recognition in noise: The role of word-initial and word-final information. Proceedings of Interspeech, 2014, 519523. doi: 10.21437/Interspeech.2014-128Google Scholar
Dewey, D. P. (2004). A comparison of reading development by learners of Japanese in intensive domestic immersion and study abroad contexts. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 26, 303327.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dufour, S., & Frauenfelder, U. H. (2010). Phonological neighbourhood effects in French spoken-word recognition. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63, 226238.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gollan, T. H., Weissberger, G. H., Runnqvist, E., Montoya, R. I., & Cera, C. M. (2011). Self-ratings of spoken language dominance: A Multilingual Naming Test (MINT) and preliminary norms for young and aging Spanish–English bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 15, 594615. https://doi.org/10.1017/s1366728911000332CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Guediche, S., Navarra-Barindelli, E., & Martin, C. D. (2023). Noise modulates crosslinguistic effects on second-language auditory word recognition. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 66, 635647.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hartshorne, J. K., Tenenbaum, J. B., & Pinker, S. (2018). A critical period for second language acquisition: Evidence from 2/3 million English speakers. Cognition, 177, 263277.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hintz, F., Voeten, C. C., McQueen, J. M., & Scharenborg, O. (2021). The effects of onset and offset masking on the time course of non-native spoken-word recognition in noise. In Fitch, T., Lamm, C., Leder, H., and Teßmar-Raible, K. (Eds.), Proceedings of the 43rd Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 133139). Vienna: Cognitive Science Society.Google Scholar
Jaeger, T. F. (2008). Categorical data analysis: Away from ANOVAs (transformation or not) and towards logit mixed models. Journal of Memory and Language, 59, 434446.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ju, M., & Luce, P. A. (2004). Falling on sensitive ears: Constraints on bilingual lexical activation. Psychological Science, 15, Article 00675. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0956-7976. 2004.00675.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kittredge, A. K., Dell, G. S., Verkuilen, J., & Schwartz, M. F. (2008). Where is the effect of frequency in word production? Insights from aphasic picture-naming errors. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 25, 463492.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Linck, J. A., Kroll, J. F., & Sunderman, G. (2009). Losing access to the native language while immersed in a second language: Evidence for the role of inhibition in second-language learning. Psychological Science, 20, 15071515. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02480.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Luce, P. A. (1986). Neighborhoods of words in the mental lexicon. Research on Speech Perception, Technical Report No. 6, Issue. [Doctoral Dissertation, Indiana University].Google Scholar
Luce, P. A., & Pisoni, D. B. (1998). Recognizing spoken words: The neighborhood activation model. Ear & Hearing, 19, 136. https://doi.org/10.1097/00003446-199802000-00001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Magnuson, J. S., Dixon, J. A., Tanenhaus, M. K., & Aslin, R. N. (2007). The dynamics of lexical competition during spoken word recognition. Cognitive Science, 31, 133156. https://doi.org/10.1080/03640210709336987CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marian, V., Blumenfeld, H. K., & Kaushanskaya, M. (2007). The Language Experience and Proficiency Questionnaire (LEAP-Q): Assessing language profiles in bilinguals and multilinguals. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research, 50, 940967. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2007/067)CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marian, V., & Spivey, M. (2003a). Bilingual and monolingual processing of competing lexical items. Applied Psycholinguistics, 24, 173193. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0142716403000092CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marian, V., & Spivey, M. (2003b). Competing activation in bilingual language processing: Within- and between-language competition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 6, 97115. https://doi.org/10.1017/s1366728903001068CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McClelland, J. L., & Elman, J. L. (1986). The TRACE model of speech perception. Cognitive Psychology, 18, 186. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(86)90015-0CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McQueen, J. M., & Huettig, F. (2012). Changing only the probability that spoken words will be distorted changes how they are recognized. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 131, 509517. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.3664087CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, J. F., Heilmann, J., Nockerts, A., Iglesias, A., Fabiano, L., & Francis, D. J. (2006). Oral language and reading in bilingual children. Learning Disabilities Research & Practice, 21, 3041. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-5826.2006.00205.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mirman, D. (2016). Zones of proximal development for models of spoken word recognition. In Mirkovic, M. G. G. J. (Ed.), Current issues in the psychology of language: Speech perception and spoken word recognition (pp. 97115). Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Montrul, S. A. (2012). Is the heritage language like a second language? Eurosla Yearbook, 12, 129.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Psychology Software Tools Incorporated. (2012). E-Prime. http://www.pstnet.comGoogle Scholar
R Development Core Team. (2016). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing. https://www.R-project.org/Google Scholar
Shin, H., Bauman, B., MacPhee, I., & Zevin, J. D. (2015, July 22–25). The dynamics of spoken word recognition in second language listeners reveal native-like lexical processing [Paper presentation]. Proceedings of the 37th Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, Pasadena, California, USA.Google Scholar
Simon, J. R., & Rudell, A. P. (1967). Auditory S-R compatibility: The effect of an irrelevant cue on information processing. Journal of Applied Psychology, 51, 300304. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0020586CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sommers, M. S., Kirk, K. I., & Pisoni, D. B. (1997). Some considerations in evaluating spoken word recognition by normal-hearing, noise-masked normal-hearing, and cochlear implant listeners. I: The effects of response format. Ear and hearing, 18, 8999.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Torgesen, J. K., Wagner, R. K., & Rashotte, C. A. (1999). Test of word reading efficiency. PRO-ED.Google Scholar
Turner, M. L., & Engle, R. W. (1989). Is working memory capacity task dependent? Journal of Memory and Language, 28, 127154.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vitevitch, M. S., & Rodríguez, E. (2005). Neighborhood density effects in spoken word recognition in Spanish. Journal of Multilingual Communication Disorders, 3, 6473.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vitevitch, M. S., & Sommers, M. S. (2003). The facilitative influence of phonological similarity and neighborhood frequency in speech production in younger and older adults. Memory & Cognition, 31, 491504.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Mean (standard error) psycholinguistic data

Figure 1

Table 2. Mean (standard error) for demographic and cognitive measures

Figure 2

Table 3. Predictor models of spoken word recognition latency and accuracy

Figure 3

Figure 1. Spoken word recognition latencies (Panel A) and accuracy rates (Panel B) to English words from dense versus sparse cohort and rhyme neighborhoods in heritage and classroom learners of Spanish. Error bars indicate standard errors.

Supplementary material: File

Botezatu supplementary material

Botezatu supplementary material
Download Botezatu supplementary material(File)
File 39.5 KB