Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-gvvz8 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-23T17:44:29.826Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Brain SCALE: Brain Structure and Cognition: an Adolescent Longitudinal Twin Study into the Genetic Etiology of Individual Differences

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  15 June 2012

Inge L. C. van Soelen
Affiliation:
Department of Biological Psychology, VU University, Amsterdam, The Netherlands. Rudolf Magnus Institute of Neuroscience, Department of Psychiatry, University Medical Center, Utrecht, The Netherlands.
Rachel M. Brouwer
Affiliation:
Rudolf Magnus Institute of Neuroscience, Department of Psychiatry, University Medical Center, Utrecht, The Netherlands.
Jiska S. Peper
Affiliation:
Rudolf Magnus Institute of Neuroscience, Department of Psychiatry, University Medical Center, Utrecht, The Netherlands. Institute of Psychology, Brain and Development Laboratory, Leiden University, The Netherlands.
Marieke van Leeuwen
Affiliation:
Department of Biological Psychology, VU University, Amsterdam, The Netherlands. Department of Psychosocial Research and Epidemiology, The Netherlands Cancer Institute, Amsterdam, The Netherlands.
Marinka M. G. Koenis
Affiliation:
Rudolf Magnus Institute of Neuroscience, Department of Psychiatry, University Medical Center, Utrecht, The Netherlands.
Toos C. E. M. van Beijsterveldt
Affiliation:
Department of Biological Psychology, VU University, Amsterdam, The Netherlands.
Suzanne C. Swagerman
Affiliation:
Department of Biological Psychology, VU University, Amsterdam, The Netherlands.
René S. Kahn
Affiliation:
Rudolf Magnus Institute of Neuroscience, Department of Psychiatry, University Medical Center, Utrecht, The Netherlands.
Hilleke E. Hulshoff Pol*
Affiliation:
Rudolf Magnus Institute of Neuroscience, Department of Psychiatry, University Medical Center, Utrecht, The Netherlands.
Dorret I. Boomsma*
Affiliation:
Department of Biological Psychology, VU University, Amsterdam, The Netherlands.
*
address for correspondence: Dorret Boomsma, Department of Biological Psychology, VU University, Van der Boechorststraat 1, 1081 BT, Amsterdam, The Netherlands. E-mail: [email protected] OR Hilleke Hulshoff Pol, Department of Psychiatry, A01.126, University Medical Center, Utrecht, Heidelberglaan 100, 3584 CX Utrecht, The Netherlands. E-mail: [email protected]
address for correspondence: Dorret Boomsma, Department of Biological Psychology, VU University, Van der Boechorststraat 1, 1081 BT, Amsterdam, The Netherlands. E-mail: [email protected] OR Hilleke Hulshoff Pol, Department of Psychiatry, A01.126, University Medical Center, Utrecht, Heidelberglaan 100, 3584 CX Utrecht, The Netherlands. E-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

From childhood into adolescence, the child's brain undergoes considerable changes in both structure and function. Twin studies are of great value to explore to what extent genetic and environmental factors explain individual differences in brain development and cognition. In The Netherlands, we initiated a longitudinal study in which twins, their siblings and their parents are assessed at three year intervals. The participants were recruited from The Netherlands Twin Register (NTR) and at baseline consisted of 112 families, with 9-year-old twins and an older sibling. Three years later, 89 families returned for follow-up assessment. Data collection included psychometric IQ tests, a comprehensive neuropsychological testing protocol, and parental and self-ratings of behavioral and emotional problems. Physical maturation was measured through assessment of Tanner stages. Hormonal levels (cortisol, luteinizing hormone, follicle-stimulating hormone, testosterone, and estrogens) were assessed in urine and saliva. Brain scans were acquired using 1.5 Tesla Magnetic Resonance Imaging (MRI), which provided volumetric measures and measures of cortical thickness. Buccal swabs were collected for DNA isolation for future candidate gene and genome-wide analysis studies. This article gives an overview of the study and the main findings. Participants will return for a third assessment when the twins are around 16 years old. Longitudinal twin-sibling studies that map brain development and cognitive function at well-defined ages aid in the understanding of genetic influences on normative brain development.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © The Authors 2012

Some decades ago, the brain of an adolescent was considered to be comparable to an adult brain. This view has been revised, largely due to the development of non-invasive neuroimaging techniques, in particular MRI. These techniques have initiated a new field of research on the neurobiology of the adolescent brain. Gaining insight into the maturational processes that take place during this period of brain development, and into the etiology of individual differences in these processes, will aid in understanding the development of psychiatric disorders, since many psychiatric disorders become apparent around or right after puberty (Kessler et al., Reference Kessler, Angermeyer, Anthony, Graff, Demyttenaere, Gasquet, de Girolamo, Gluzman, Gureje, Haro, Kawakami, Karam, Levinson, Medina Mora, Oakley Browne, Posada-Villa, Stein, Adley Tsang, Aguilar-Gaxiola, Alonso, Lee, Heeringa, Pennell, Berglund, Gruber, Petukhova, Chatterji and Ustün2007; Paus et al., Reference Paus, Keshavan and Giedd2008).

The transition from childhood to adolescence is characterized by cognitive maturation (Casey et al., Reference Casey, Tottenham, Liston and Durston2005b; Keating, Reference Keating and Steinberg2004; Steinberg, Reference Steinberg2005; Crone, Reference Crone2009). Children improve considerably in specific cognitive functions, including information processing (Demetriou et al., Reference Demetriou, Christou, Spanoudis and Platsidou2002), working memory (Gathercole et al., Reference Gathercole, Pickering, Ambridge and Wearing2004), and verbal learning (van den Burg & Kingma, Reference van den Burg and Kingma1999). During adolescence, complex, non-linear, and region-specific structural changes occur in the human brain. White matter (WM) tissue shows an almost linear increase in volume with age, and continues to increase in adults until approximately 45 years (Bartzokis et al., Reference Bartzokis, Beckson, Lu, Nuechterlein, Edwards and Mintz2001; Lebel & Beaulieu, Reference Lebel and Beaulieu2011), while gray matter (GM) tissue initially increases in volume in childhood and, after peaking around puberty, starts to decrease (Giedd et al., Reference Giedd, Clasen, Lenroot, Greenstein, Wallace, Ordaz, Molloy, Blumenthal, Tossell, Stayer, Samango-Sprouse, Shen, Davatzikos, Merke and Chrousos2006; Paus, Reference Paus2005). This decrease in GM volume is most often associated with overall thinning of the cortex (Gogtay et al., Reference Gogtay, Giedd, Lusk, Hayashi, Greenstein, Vaituzis, Nugent, Herman, Clasen, Toga, Rapoport and Thompson2004; Sowell et al., Reference Sowell, Trauner, Gamst and Jernigan2002). In general, GM cortical changes occur first in primary cortical areas (e.g., sensory-motor cortex), followed by secondary and multimodal cortical areas throughout childhood and adolescence (Gogtay et al., Reference Gogtay, Giedd, Lusk, Hayashi, Greenstein, Vaituzis, Nugent, Herman, Clasen, Toga, Rapoport and Thompson2004; Sowell et al., Reference Sowell, Trauner, Gamst and Jernigan2002). The biological processes underlying these structural changes in GM and WM around puberty are incompletely understood. One of the hypotheses for GM loss is the process of synaptic pruning (Huttenlocher & Dabholkar, Reference Huttenlocher and Dabholkar1997). Decrease in GM, and increase in WM, are also thought to be a reflection of progressive, age-related axonal myelination (Benes et al., Reference Benes, Turtle, Khan and Farol1994; Yakovlev & Lecours, Reference Yakovlev, Lecours and Minkowski1967).

The development of psychopathology and psychiatric disorders might be influenced, or may even be initiated, by processes influencing development of the brain (Insel, Reference Insel2010; Karmiloff-Smith, Reference Karmiloff-Smith2010; Shaw et al., Reference Shaw, Gogtay and Rapoport2010). Integrating the tools of genomics and neuroscience may reveal causes of neuropsychiatric illnesses, and suggest new strategies for treatment and prevention (Akil et al., Reference Akil, Martone and Van2011). Nearly all psychiatric disorders have a genetic origin, whether they originate in childhood (e.g., Faraone et al., Reference Faraone, Perlis, Doyle, Smoller, Goralnick, Holmgren and Sklar2005; Muhle et al., Reference Muhle, Trentacoste and Rapin2004), or in adolescence/adulthood (e.g., Bienvenu et al., Reference Bienvenu, Davydow and Kendler2011). To understand disrupted brain development, it is vital to understand normal or healthy brain development (Luna & Sweeney, Reference Luna and Sweeney2001), and how healthy brain development is influenced by genotype and environmental exposures. For this purpose, twin studies are of great value to explore to what extent genetic and environmental factors explain individual differences in brain development and cognition.

The longitudinal twin study described in this paper aims to examine how genetic and environmental influences affect individual differences in developmental changes in brain structure and cognition during the transition from childhood to adolescence. A population-based study of a cohort of twins and their siblings was started in 2005. At baseline, 9-year-old twins and an older sibling were included in a study of brain development and cognition. Their parents also took part in cognitive testing. Around the 12th birthday of the twins, the families were invited for follow-up and a third assessment is currently scheduled. In this paper, we present an overview of data acquisition, a characterization of the participants, and a summary of the main results so far. This is followed by a discussion on how these results should be viewed in the light of current knowledge on healthy brain development, and what their possible implications are for further developmental studies in health and psychopathology.

Cohort Description and Testing Procedures

Participants

All twin families were recruited from The Netherlands Twin Registry (NTR) (Boomsma et al., Reference Boomsma, Geus, Vink, Stubbe, Distel, Hottenga, Posthuma, van Beijsterveldt, Hudziak, Bartels and Willemsen2006). Families from birth cohorts 1995–1996 were selected based on zygosity of the twins, and whether they had an older brother or sister aged less than 14 years. This age limit for siblings was set to optimize future comparisons with the twin data. Exclusion criteria were: having a pacemaker; metal materials in the head (except for dental braces); chronic use of medication; a known major medical or psychiatric history; participation in special education; and physical or sensory disabilities. In total, 214 families were invited by letter, which also included information brochures for parents and children. Two weeks later they were contacted by phone and 112 families agreed to take part in the study. In this group, 103 twin pairs had a full sibling who met inclusion criteria and was willing to participate. The most common reasons for refusal were: no time or too much effort (n = 52); children did not want to participate or were scared (n = 13); parents did not want their children to participate (n = 10); and problems with children, like hospitalization, psychiatric problems of which we were not aware, and divorce of parents (n = 9). Data acquisition was randomized across time with respect to sex and zygosity of twins. At baseline, mean age of the twins was 9.2 years (SD = 0.5, range 8.9–9.6). Zygosity of the same-sex twins was determined by DNA polymorphisms. There were 48 monozygotic (MZ) pairs (23 male/25 female), 44 same-sex dizygotic (DZ) pairs (23 male/21 female), and 20 dizygotic pairs of opposite sex. Mean age of siblings (N = 105, 59 female) was 11.9 years (range = 9.9–14.9). Mean age of fathers was 43.9 years (SD = 4.2), and of mothers 41.8 years (SD = 3.3). Table 1 provides more details on the participants, including handedness and IQ. IQ scores were somewhat higher in sibs than in twins. In a larger project (de Zeeuw et al., Reference de Zeeuw, van Beijsterveldt, de Geus and Boomsma2012), we found that this difference is explained by birth order, with older sibs in the family having a higher IQ.

TABLE 1 Number of Participants, Age, IQ, and Handedness at Baseline and Follow-up

MZ = monozygous, DZ = dizygous.

a Data on handedness are missing for 6 twins and 3 siblings at baseline.

When the twins reached the age of 12, the families were contacted by letter and phone. Eighty-nine families agreed to participate at the follow-up, resulting in a response rate of approximately 80%. In the 23 families that did not return, the most important reasons for non-participation were no time, too much effort (n = 8); some or all family members did not want to participate (n = 11); moved to another country (n = 2); and families who moved and new contact details could not be retrieved in time (n = 2). Mean age of twins at follow-up was 12.2 years (SD = 0.3, range = 11.7–13.1). The follow-up group comprised 40 MZ (20 male/20 female), 34 DZ (17 male/17 female), and 15 opposite-sex DZ pairs. Mean age of the siblings (N = 82; 45 female) was 14.8 years (SD = 1.3, range 9.8–18.0, one younger sibling was included) (see also Table 1).

The study was approved by the Central Committee on Research Involving Human Subjects of The Netherlands (CCMO), and studies were performed in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. Parents signed informed consent forms for the children and for themselves. Parents were financially compensated for travel expenses, and children received a present or gift voucher at the end of the testing days. In addition, a summary of cognition scores and a printed image of their MRI scan, when available, were provided afterwards.

Procedures

At baseline the data were collected on two separate days. On one day children were tested at the VU University (VU) in Amsterdam (neuropsychological testing and sample collection of urine and saliva), and on the other day at the University Medical Center in Utrecht (UMCU; MRI scan acquisition). Average time between cognitive testing and MRI scan was 50 days (range = 4–128). At follow-up, all data were collected at the same day at the UMCU. An overview of the available scans and cognitive data is illustrated in Figure 1.

FIGURE 1 Data collection at baseline and at follow-up for cognitive functioning (number of psychometric IQ tests for twins and siblings, and Raven Advanced Progressive matrices for parents) and brain structure (number of MRI scans after image processing).

Neuropsychological Tests

Data on cognitive abilities were collected with psychometric IQ and neuropsychological tests. A small number of tests that exhibited low reliability and/or limited genetic influences at baseline were not repeated at follow-up: Tower of Hanoi (Simon, Reference Simon1975), Eriksen flanker task (Eriksen & Eriksen, Reference Eriksen and Eriksen1974; Eriksen & Schultz, Reference Eriksen and Schultz1979), and the ‘Reading the Mind in the Eyes’ task (Baron-Cohen et al., Reference Baron-Cohen, Wheelwright, Hill, Raste and Plumb2001). The description of the neuropsychological tests in the section below includes the tests collected at both baseline and follow-up.

The main phenotypic outcome measures of the cognitive tests, hormonal levels, puberty stage, height, and weight are given in Table 2 for twins and siblings. All children were tested by a series of subtests from the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children — Third version (WISC-III) (Wechsler et al., Reference Wechsler, Kort, Compaan, Belichrodt, Resing, Schittekatte, Bosmans, Vermeir and Verhaeghe2002; see van Soelen et al., Reference van Soelen, Brouwer, van Leeuwen, Kahn, Hulshoff Pol and Boomsma2011c for more details), and the Raven Standard Progressive matrixes (Raven, Reference Raven1960; van Leeuwen et al., Reference van Leeuwen, van den Berg and Boomsma2008) at baseline and follow-up. Additionally, the Raven Advanced Progressive Matrices (Raven et al., Reference Raven, Raven and Court1998) were completed by the parents of twins (van Leeuwen et al., Reference van Leeuwen, van den Berg and Boomsma2008). To assess working memory, children performed a spatial variant of the N-back task (Gevins & Cutillo, Reference Gevins and Cutillo1993; Jansma et al., Reference Jansma, Ramsey, Coppola and Kahn2000) that was adapted to make it more appealing for young participants, and with increasing levels of difficulty (van Leeuwen et al., Reference van Leeuwen, van den Berg, Hoekstra and Boomsma2007). Both short-term and long-term memory were assessed, using the Dutch version of the Rey's Auditory Verbal Learning Test (AVLT; 15-words task) (van den Burg & Kingma, Reference van den Burg and Kingma1999), and the Corsi block-tapping task (Corsi, Reference Corsi1974). Details on these memory paradigms can be found in van Leeuwen et al. (Reference van Leeuwen, van den Berg, Hoekstra and Boomsma2009b) and van Soelen et al. (Reference van Soelen, van den Berg, Dekker, van Leeuwen, Peper, Hulschoff Pol and Boomsma2009). Speed of processing was assessed with the π-inspection task (Luciano et al., Reference Luciano, Smith, Wright, Geffen, Geffen and Martin2001; van Leeuwen et al., 2007). Selective attention was assessed by the Stroop colour-word task (Stroop, Reference Stroop1935). The Controlled Oral Word Association task (COWA), a subtest of the Multilingual Aphasia Exam, was used to measure verbal fluency. Reading ability was assessed by asking the children to read out loud as many words as possible from a card in one minute, which consisted of a maximum of 120 words (van Leeuwen et al., Reference van Leeuwen, van den Berg, Peper, Hulschoff Pol and Boomsma2009c). This list of words was adapted from the so-called Three Minutes Reading Task, which is frequently used in The Netherlands educational system (Cito, 1995). Handedness was determined based on the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory (Oldfield, Reference Oldfield1971).

TABLE 2 Mean Scores for Main Variables Collected at Baseline and Follow-up for Twins and Their Siblings

SD = standard deviation, LH = lutenizing hormone, FSH = follicle stimulating hormone

a At baseline, none of the female twins reported menarche, 14 siblings reported menarche (5 reported a regular menstrual cycle), and none were using oral contraceptives; at follow-up, 21 reported menarche, (3 reported a regular menstrual cycle), and none used oral conceptives, 38 siblings reported menarche (32 girls reported a regular menstrual cycle, 6 used oral contraceptives).

b At baseline, collection of questionnaires was at different moments for twins and siblings. The mean scores on internalizing and externalizing behaviour of twins are based on a mailed survey, and for siblings the scores are based on questionnaires filled in around the date of lab visit.

cYSR was not collected at baseline.

Behavioral Data

All parents of young twin pairs registered at the NTR receive survey questionnaires that are mailed at birth, and at ages 1, 2, 3, 5, 7, 9, and 12 years. From age 7 onwards, teachers of twins are also approached. Surveys include the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL), the Teacher's Report Form (TRF), and questions about growth, health, developmental milestone, demographics, lifestyle, and school performance (Bartels et al., Reference Bartels, de Geus, Kirschbaum, Sluyter and Boomsma2007; Bartels et al., Reference Bartels, van Beijsterveldt and Boomsma2009; Boomsma et al., Reference Boomsma, van Beijsterveldt and Hudziak2005; Brouwer et al., Reference Brouwer, van Beijsterveldt, Bartels, Hudziak and Boomsma2006; Estourgie-van Burk et al., Reference Estourgie-van Burk, Bartels, Boomsma and Delemarre-van de Waal2010; Felsenfeld et al., Reference Felsenfeld, van Beijsterveldt and Boomsma2010; Franic et al., Reference Franic, Middeldorp, Dolan, Ligthart and Boomsma2010; van Beijsterveldt & Boomsma, Reference van Beijsterveldt and Boomsma2007). The CBCL, which is completed by both parents, contains scales to assess behavioral and emotional problems in children (Achenbach, Reference Achenbach1991). At baseline and follow-up, mothers completed the CBCL for siblings. At follow-up, twins and their siblings were asked to fill out the Dutch Health and Behavior Questionnaire (DHBQ), which includes the Youth Self Report (Achenbach & Rescorla; Reference Achenbach and Rescorla2001 Verhulst et al., Reference Verhulst, van der Ende and Koot1997), to assess problem behavior, self esteem, life events, religiosity, happiness, life satisfaction, family situation, and family functioning (Bartels et al., Reference Bartels, de Geus, Kirschbaum, Sluyter and Boomsma2007). The DBHQ was mailed to the twins and siblings before their visit to the lab, so they could complete it at home.

Physical Examination

Pubertal stage was determined by a trained project supervisor on the basis of secondary sexual characteristics (Marshall & Tanner, Reference Marshall and Tanner1969; Marshall & Tanner, Reference Marshall and Tanner1970). Tanner stages are classified on a 5-item scale (stage 1 represents no pubertal development and stage 5 represents full maturation). If children were not comfortable with this assessment by the project supervisor, they were given a choice to point out their status based on black and white photographs of the different puberty stages, which were accompanied by oral explanation by the researcher. Body weight and height were assessed for each child, and girls were asked about menarche, regularity of their menstrual cycle, and the use of oral contraceptives.

Sample Collection for Hormone Assessment

In the week before the laboratory visit, children collected samples of saliva and morning urine. Samples were stored in the refrigerator at home. All samples were handed in on arrival at the laboratory and were stored in a freezer. Hormone levels were determined in saliva (cortisol and testosterone) and urine (estradiol, luteinizing hormone and follicle-stimulating hormone). Multiple assessments at a consistent time of the day are required to decrease the influence of fluctuations in hormonal levels, particularly in children (Riad-Fahmy et al., Reference Riad-Fahmy, Read, Walker, Walker and Griffiths1987; Read et al., Reference Read, Walker, Wilson and Griffiths1990; Walker et al., Reference Walker, Read, Wilson, Riad-Fahmy and Griffiths1990). Therefore, children were instructed to collect all the samples on two school days, and to restrict the awakening time and time of sampling, because in The Netherlands the starting time of primary school is approximately similar throughout the country. Participants were asked to write down the exact sample time in a time schedule, and to note any exceptional events interfering with normal daily routine. Additionally, children were asked to collect saliva at five time points during the morning for cortisol level determination (previously described by Bartels et al., 2003).

Buccal Cell Collection

Both parents and children collected buccal swabs for DNA isolation. Subjects were asked to take 2 series of swabs on 2 days, by rubbing cotton buds along the inside of the mouth. DNA was used to determine zygosity and will be used for genome-wide association (GWA) studies in the future. Zygosity was initially determined by a set of eight highly polymorphic di-, tri-, and tetra-nucleotide genetic markers, and confirmed by additional single nucleotide polymorphism (SNP) genotyping of 64 SNPs.

MRI Scan Protocol

Scanning and image processing took place at the UMCU. All children were initially offered a practice session in a dummy scanner to become familiarized with the scan procedure, small space, and the sounds of the MRI machine (Durston et al., Reference Durston, Nederveen, van Dijk, van Belle, de Zeeuw, Langen and van Dijk2009). Exclusion criteria for the MRI scanner were any metal in or around the body that could interfere with the MRI scanner (e.g., metal braces).

For both measurements, whole head MRI scan was acquired with a 1.5 Tesla Philips Achieva scanner (Philips, Best, The Netherlands). For both baseline and follow-up, the same scan sequence parameters were applied to limit possible differences in scan acquisition. The scanning protocol included T1-weighted Magnetization Transfer Imaging (MTI) and Diffusion Tensor Imaging (DTI) scans. At follow-up, the scanning protocol was extended with a T2-weighted image and a resting-state functional MRI (fMRI) scan (for more details, see Brouwer et al., 2010; Peper et al., 2008; van Soelen et al., Reference van Soelen, Brouwer, van Baal, Schnack, Peper, Chen, Kahn, Boomsma and Hulshoff Pol2011a). Children were allowed to watch a movie or listen to music during the scans, except that, during the resting-state fMRI scan, the children were asked to close their eyes and try to avoid thinking of anything in particular. The total scan protocol took approximately 45–50 minutes for each child.

Future Follow-up

Participants will be invited for a third visit when the twins are 16 years old. Cognitive assessment will include several new computerized tasks and some earlier tests will be replaced by computerized versions. The Computerized Neuropsychological Testing System (WebCNP) developed by the University of Pennsylvania (Gur et al., Reference Gur, Ragland, Moberg, Turner, Bilker, Kohler, Siegel and Gur2001; Gur et al., Reference Gur, Richard, Hughett, Calkins, Macy, Bilker, Brensinger and Gur2010) will be added to the protocol. The CNP consists of a set of cognitive tasks that take approximately one hour to administer. It provides measures of speed and accuracy on 10 neurocognitive domains. The subtests show good reliability and construct validity. In addition, they meet the criteria on which the tasks in the first two cognitive assessments were selected: they measure heritable traits, and have been linked to specific brain systems in functional neuroimaging studies. IQ will be assessed by the shortened four-test version of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS), namely the Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (WASI), including the vocabulary, similarities, matrix reasoning, and block design subtests. The Corsi block-tapping task and the Three Minutes Reading List will remain part of the cognitive assessment, and the scan protocol and scan parameters remain as previous described. New additions to the experimental protocol are the computerized Bechara's gambling task (van Leijenhorst et al., Reference van Leijenhorst, Moor, de Macks, Rombouts, Westenberg and Crone2010), an autism questionnaire (Hoekstra et al., Reference Hoekstra, Bartels, Cath and Boomsma2008), and blood pressure measurements.

Main Findings

An overview of main findings is presented in Table 3. Considering the focus of this special issue, we will limit our summary of results to the imaging-related publications that have resulted from this cohort. At age 9, variability in brain volumes was highly heritable (e.g. > 90% of the variability in total brain volume) (Peper et al., Reference Peper, Schnack, Brouwer, van Baal, Pjetri, Szekely, van Leeuwen, van den Berg, Collins, Evans, Boomsma, Kahn and Hulshoff Pol2009c). We explored GM and WM density with voxel-based morphometry (VBM). Variation in density of posterior WM, as well as of anterior (pre-)frontal and temporal GM areas, was primarily influenced by genes (Peper et al., Reference Peper, Schnack, Brouwer, van Baal, Pjetri, Szekely, van Leeuwen, van den Berg, Collins, Evans, Boomsma, Kahn and Hulshoff Pol2009c). At follow-up, heritability estimates of global brain volume were still very high (van Soelen et al., Reference van Soelen, Brouwer, van Baal, Schnack, Peper, Chen, Kahn, Boomsma and Hulshoff Pol2011a). For all brain volumes, high genetic correlations with age were observed, indicating that individual variation in these brain volumes at the ages of 9 and 12 years were explained by the same genetic factors (van Soelen et al., Reference van Soelen, Brouwer, van Baal, Schnack, Peper, Chen, Kahn, Boomsma and Hulshoff Pol2011a). Our findings of high heritability estimates in childhood are in line with other studies reporting relatively high estimates for brain volumes during childhood and adolescence (Schmitt et al., Reference Schmitt, Eyler, Giedd, Kremen, Kendler and Neale2007; Wallace et al., Reference Wallace, Eric, Lenroot, Viding, Ordaz, Rosenthal, Molloy, Clasen, Kendler, Neale and Giedd2006; Yoon et al., Reference Yoon, Fahim, Perusse and Evans2010).

TABLE 3 Main Findings for Cognition and Brain Structure

details are given on the included participants (age in years, and whether twins [tw], siblings [sib], or both were included), variables used (phenotype/task/measure used), and the study design (cross-sectional [cross] or longitudinal [long]); A = additive genetic influences; AVLT = Auditory Verbal Learning Task; BB = big brain/cerebrum; BW = birth weight; C = shared environmental influences; CB = cerebellum; CC = corpus callosum; Cing = cingulum; CT = cortical thickness; CVLT = California Verbal Learning Task; DS = digit span; E = estradiol; GA = gestational age; FA = fractional anisotropy; FS = forgetting speed; FSH = follicle stimulating hormone; GM = gray matter; IQ = intelligence quotient; Lat = lateral; LH = luteinizing hormone; LS = learning speed; Mid = middle; Post = posterior; re / rg / rp = unique environmental / genetic / and phenotypic correlations; SOF = superior occipitofrontal fascicle; SLF = superior longitudinal fascicle; SSC = secondary sexual characteristics; Sup = superior; T = testosterone; TB = total brain; VBM = Voxel-based morphometry; WISC = Wechsler Intelligence Scale for children (third version); WM = white matter;

a In these studies two cohorts were combined, namely the baseline assessment of the present pediatric cohort and another young adolescent cohort.

We also estimated the heritability of WM microstructure at age 9, through DTI and MTI (Brouwer et al., Reference Brouwer, Mandl, Peper, van Baal, Kahn, Boomsma and Hulshoff Pol2010). DTI provides information about the directionality, or so-called fiber integrity, of the underlying WM fiber bundles. MTI can be used to compute the magnetization transfer ratio (MTR), which can be seen as a measure of the amount of macromolecules in tissue, including myelin. Modest influences of genetic factors were found on diffusion parameters (longitudinal, 33–46%, radial diffusivity, 27–64%). Heritability estimates of the MTR signal ranged from 31–61% and were largest in the posterior part of the brain (Brouwer et al., Reference Brouwer, Mandl, Peper, van Baal, Kahn, Boomsma and Hulshoff Pol2010).

Between age 9 and 12 years, total brain volume increased, on average, by 14.6 ml (van Soelen et al., Reference van Soelen, Brouwer, van Baal, Schnack, Peper, Chen, Kahn, Boomsma and Hulshoff Pol2011a), which is an increase of 1.1%. The most pronounced volumetric change was observed for the cerebellum (increase of 4.2%). Cerebral GM volume was the only volumetric measure that showed a decrease between 9 and 12 years of age (decrease of 1.6%; see Figure 2). These volumetric changes are in line with previously reported volumetric changes based on pediatric samples, including large age ranges from young childhood up to young adulthood (e.g., Giedd et al., Reference Giedd, Blumenthal, Jeffries, Castellanos, Liu, Zijdenbos, Paus, Evans and Rapoport1999). The relatively large increase in cerebellar volume observed between age 9 and 12 is consistent with previous findings that the cerebellum is maturing at a later age than the cerebrum (Lenroot et al., Reference Lenroot, Gogtay, Greenstein, Wells, Wallace, Clasen, Blumenthal, Lerch, Zijdenbos, Evans, Thompson and Giedd2007; Tiemeier et al., Reference Tiemeier, Lenroot, Greenstein, Tran, Pierson and Giedd2010). It can also be speculated that, in this sample, the relatively large increase in cerebellar volume that was found at age 9 is partly the result of compensating for the effect of preterm birth. Shorter pregnancy duration was found to be associated with a relatively smaller cerebellar volume at age 9. The cerebellum is under rapid development in the last trimester of the pregnancy (Limperopoulos et al., Reference Limperopoulos, Soul, Gauvreau, Huppi, Warfield, Bassan, Robertson, Volpe and du Plessis2005), and therefore the development of this structure in postnatal life might be particularly vulnerable to preterm birth as observed in this cohort (van Soelen et al., Reference van Soelen, Brouwer, Peper, van Beijsterveldt, van, de Vries, Kahn, Hulshoff Pol and Boomsma2010).

FIGURE 2 Volumetric changes between ages 9 and 12 years. Absolute brain volume of total cerebral volume (A), cerebral gray matter (B), cerebral white matter (C), and total cerebellar volume (D) are given on all included subjects at baseline and at follow-up. Longitudinal data points are connected, with boys depicted in blue, and girls in red (van Soelen et al., 2001a, reprinted with permission).

Individual differences in volumetric brain changes were partly under genetic control (change in cerebral volume was 20% heritable and change in cerebellar volume 45%). The association between changes in cerebellar and cerebral volumes (r p = .49) was driven by shared genetic influences and, to a smaller extent, by shared unique environmental influences. As children undergo a growth spurt during puberty (Tanner & Whitehouse, Reference Tanner and Whitehouse1976), increase in height was included in bivariate genetic analyses. We observed an increase in height of 13.8 cm (van Soelen et al., Reference van Soelen, Brouwer, van Baal, Schnack, Peper, Chen, Kahn, Boomsma and Hulshoff Pol2011a), which was highly heritable (73%), but was influenced by genetic factors other than those implicated in changes in cerebral volume. The modest correlation between height and cerebellar growth (r p = .24), could be attributed partly to shared genetic influences (r g = .48).

We conclude that developmental brain changes are evident in this 3-year interval, and that these changes are heritable. Furthermore, these changes in cerebral volume and the increase in height do not stem from the same genetic origin, while the change in cerebellar volume is partly (genetically) correlated with changes in height.

Brain Structure and Puberty

It has been assumed that puberty-related increases in sex steroids are linked to brain development (Giedd et al., Reference Giedd, Clasen, Lenroot, Greenstein, Wallace, Ordaz, Molloy, Blumenthal, Tossell, Stayer, Samango-Sprouse, Shen, Davatzikos, Merke and Chrousos2006), but a direct association has only recently been examined (e.g., Bramen et al., Reference Bramen, Hranilovich, Dahl, Forbes, Chen, Toga, Dinov, Worthman and Sowell2011; Herting et al., Reference Herting, Maxwell, Irvine and Nagel2011; Neufang et al., Reference Neufang, Specht, Hausmann, Gunturkun, Herpertz-Dahlmann, Fink and Konrad2009; Peper et al., Reference Peper, Brouwer, Schnack, van Baal, van Leeuwen, van den Berg, Delemarre-van de Waal, Boomsma, Kahn and Hulshoff Pol2009a; Peper et al., Reference Peper, Brouwer, Schnack, van Baal, van Leeuwen, van den Berg, Delemarre-van de Waal, Janke, Collins, Evans, Boomsma, Kahn and Hulshoff Pol2008). Puberty is characterized by the (re-)activation of the HPG axis. At that time, release of gonadotropin releasing hormone (GnRH) is stimulated and induces, in turn, secretion of luteinizing hormone (LH) and follicle stimulating hormone (FSH) from the pituitary. LH and FSH activate the maturation of the testes and ovaries, and are also associated with the production of the sex steroids testosterone and estrogen, which trigger the maturation of secondary sexual characteristics (Delemarre-van de Waal, Reference Delemarre-van de Waal2002; Sisk & Foster, Reference Sisk and Foster2004). We found that 9-year-old girls with the first signs of secondary sexual characteristics already had lower GM density in prefrontal and parietal brain regions compared to 9-year-old girls without the first signs of secondary sexual characteristics (Peper et al., Reference Peper, Schnack, Brouwer, van Baal, Pjetri, Szekely, van Leeuwen, van den Berg, Collins, Evans, Boomsma, Kahn and Hulshoff Pol2009c). Similar findings were also observed by Bramen et al. (Reference Bramen, Hranilovich, Dahl, Forbes, Chen, Toga, Dinov, Worthman and Sowell2011), where a more advanced pubertal stage (also assessed by Tanner criteria) was associated with more decrease in cortical GM volume. One of the first endocrinological markers of puberty is LH secretion during the night. We found that an increased production of LH at 9 years was associated with larger WM volumes in both sexes (Peper et al., Reference Peper, Brouwer, Schnack, van Baal, van Leeuwen, van den Berg, Delemarre-van de Waal, Janke, Collins, Evans, Boomsma, Kahn and Hulshoff Pol2008). Regionally, higher levels of LH were associated with greater WM density within the splenium of the corpus callosum, middle temporal gyri, and the cingulum. In addition, this association between WM density and level of LH concentration was explained by overlapping genetic influences acting on both traits (Peper et al., Reference Peper, Brouwer, Schnack, van Baal, van Leeuwen, van den Berg, Delemarre-van de Waal, Janke, Collins, Evans, Boomsma, Kahn and Hulshoff Pol2008). From this study, it could be concluded that the very first marker of puberty, LH production, predicts WM development.

Effect of estradiol and testosterone were explored in the siblings, aged 10–14 years. In girls, higher estradiol levels were associated with a decrease in GM density in frontal and parietal areas, and with an increase in GM density in temporal and occipital areas (not in WM). There was no effect of testosterone on brain structure in girls. In boys, no association between testosterone or estradiol was found with brain structure (Peper et al., Reference Peper, Brouwer, Schnack, van Baal, van Leeuwen, van den Berg, Delemarre-van de Waal, Boomsma, Kahn and Hulshoff Pol2009a). In the same group of siblings it was found that pituitary volume was associated with hormonal levels. After correction for age, a larger pituitary volume was associated with higher FSH levels in girls (Peper et al., Reference Peper, Brouwer, van Leeuwen, Schnack, Boomsma, Kahn and Hulshoff Pol2010). Hypothalamic volume did not change with age and was not associated with hormonal levels. To summarize, these studies demonstrate that pubertal hormone levels are associated with GM and WM structure. However, given the large individual differences in puberty onset, longitudinal designs are needed in future research on brain development and sex hormone levels (Blakemore et al., Reference Blakemore, Burnett and Dahl2010; Peper et al., Reference Peper, Hulshoff Pol, Crone and Honk2011).

Brain Structure and Cognition

The relationship of total brain volume and general cognitive abilities has been well established in adults (Deary et al., Reference Deary, Penke and Johnson2010; Hulshoff Pol et al., Reference Hulshoff Pol, Schnack, Posthuma, Mandl, Baare, van Oel, van Haren, Collins, Evans, Amunts, Burgel, Zilles, De Geus, Boomsma and Kahn2006; Posthuma et al., Reference Posthuma, de Geus, Baare, Hulshoff Pol, Kahn and Boomsma2002). This association is mediated by genes shared between structural brain measures and full scale IQ in adulthood (Hulshoff Pol et al., Reference Hulshoff Pol, Schnack, Posthuma, Mandl, Baare, van Oel, van Haren, Collins, Evans, Amunts, Burgel, Zilles, De Geus, Boomsma and Kahn2006; Posthuma et al., Reference Posthuma, de Geus, Baare, Hulshoff Pol, Kahn and Boomsma2002). In our pediatric twin cohort we demonstrated that this genetic association between brain structure and IQ is already present at a young age. Van Leeuwen et al. (Reference van Leeuwen, Peper, van den Berg, Brouwer, Hulschoff Pol, Kahn and Boomsma2009a) confirmed the positive association between general cognitive abilities and brain volumes at age 9 (r p = .20–.33), with genetic correlations ranging between .26 and .35.

Concluding Remarks

To gain a better understanding of individual differences in brain development, twin studies explore the influence of genetic and environmental factors. This ongoing longitudinal project in Dutch twins and siblings aims to develop a more comprehensive view of normative brain development by looking at the effects of genetic and environmental influences on different aspects of brain structure and cognition around puberty. Below we discuss how these results may be viewed in the light of current knowledge of healthy development, and developmental models of psychopathology.

Because the majority of psychiatric disorders emerge during adolescence (Shaw et al., Reference Shaw, Gogtay and Rapoport2010; Gogtay et al., Reference Gogtay, Giedd and Rapoport2002), understanding the relative influences of genetic and environmental effects on brain development during this crucial period may elucidate biological processes underlying these illnesses. Why an individual develops a psychiatric disorder is a result of genetic predisposition and environmental factors that a person may encounter early or later in life. Cross-sectional studies are limited with respect to information regarding the developmental trajectory of individual brains (Karmiloff-Smith, Reference Karmiloff-Smith2010). Because of this, longitudinal studies are of great importance. Mapping normal development can create opportunities for modeling disrupted trajectories in psychopathology (Gogtay & Thompson, Reference Gogtay and Thompson2010; Shaw et al., Reference Shaw, Eckstrand, Sharp, Blumenthal, Lerch, Greenstein, Clasen, Evans, Giedd and Rapoport2007; Shaw et al., Reference Shaw, Gogtay and Rapoport2010; Wallace et al., Reference Wallace, Dankner, Kenworthy, Giedd and Martin2010). Future research should focus on the developmental changes of the human brain, and the genetic and environmental influences that can act upon these changes. When selecting phenotypes to map brain development, it is important to analyze brain measures that capture different aspects of brain structure like cortical volume, surface area, cortical thickness, sulcal depth, and measures of cortical folding or gyrification, which all have a genetic component (Kochunov et al., Reference Kochunov, Glahn, Fox, Lancaster, Saleem, Shelledy, Zilles, Thompson, Coulon, Mangin, Blangero and Rogers2010; Rogers et al., Reference Rogers, Kochunov, Lancaster, Shelledy, Glahn, Blangero and Fox2007; Rogers et al., Reference Rogers, Kochunov, Zilles, Shelledy, Lancaster, Thompson, Duggirala, Blangero, Fox and Glahn2010). Age-related changes in heritability observed for measures of brain structures or cognitive functioning (Lenroot et al., Reference Lenroot, Schmitt, Ordaz, Wallace, Neale, Lerch, Kendler, Evans and Giedd2009; van Soelen et al., 2011c; Wallace et al., Reference Wallace, Eric, Lenroot, Viding, Ordaz, Rosenthal, Molloy, Clasen, Kendler, Neale and Giedd2006) can be linked to the timing of gene expression patterns, and this, in turn, might be related to the age of onset of disorders. Identifying the actual genes that are involved in developmental changes of brain structure and function is the next challenge.

Our study focuses on multiple aspects of structural brain changes and cognition, and is one of the few to explore how changes in brain anatomy are linked to cognitive functions from a genetic perspective at specific ages. Shaw et al. (Reference Shaw, Greenstein, Lerch, Clasen, Lenroot, Gogtay, Evans, Rapoport and Giedd2006) showed that cortical thickness trajectories between childhood and adolescence (ages 4 to 17 years) differed based on the level of intelligence, giving one of the first indications that the developmental trajectory of cortical thickness is associated with cognitive ability. Performance on different cognitive tasks indicative for executive control was associated with the amount of thinning observed in a large cross-sectional sample of children from 8 to 19 years old (Tamnes et al., Reference Tamnes, Ostby, Fjell, Westlye, Due-Tonnessen and Walhovd2010). A recent study indicated that changes in IQ during adolescence was associated with changes in GM volume, indicating the importance of large individual differences in brain developmental trajectories, and how this is linked with changes in cognitive performance (Ramsden et al., Reference Ramsden, Richardson, Josse, Thomas, Ellis, Shakeshaft, Seghier and Price2011). Heritability for local cortical thickness throughout the cortex was previously reported in children (Lenroot et al., Reference Lenroot, Schmitt, Ordaz, Wallace, Neale, Lerch, Kendler, Evans and Giedd2009; Yoon et al., Reference Yoon, Fahim, Perusse and Evans2010) and adults (Brans et al., Reference Brans, Kahn, Schnack, van Baal, Posthuma, van Haren, Lepage, Lerch, Collins, Evans, Boomsma and Hulshoff Pol2010; Kremen et al., Reference Kremen, Prom-Wormley, Panizzon, Eyler, Fischl, Neale, Franz, Lyons, Pacheco, Perry, Stevens, Schmitt, Grant, Seidman, Thermenos, Tsuang, Eisen, Dale and Fennema-Notestine2010; Panizzon et al., Reference Panizzon, Fennema-Notestine, Eyler, Jernigan, Prom-Wormley, Neale, Jacobson, Lyons, Grant, Franz, Xian, Tsuang, Fischl, Seidman, Dale and Kremen2009; Rimol et al., Reference Rimol, Panizzon, Fennema-Notestine, Eyler, Fischl, Franz, Hagler, Lyons, Neale, Pacheco, Perry, Schmitt, Grant, Seidman, Thermenos, Tsuang, Eisen, Kremen and Dale2010). The study of Lenroot et al., consisting of a sample with a large age range from 5 to 19 years, observed indirect indications that the increase of heritability seems to be related to the timing of maturation processes. We are currently exploring the genetics of changes in cortical thickness (van Soelen et al., Reference van Soelen, Brouwer, van Baal, Schnack, Peper, Collins, Evans, Kahn, Boomsma and Hulshoff Pol2011b), and its genetic relation with intelligence in our cohort.

How the maturation processes of structural and functional brain changes in adolescents are linked together, and how these reflect or interact with improvement in cognitive abilities, remain to be elucidated. Cognitive maturation during adolescence may not be a direct reflection of brain regions that mature at different ages, but rather a reflection of the development of networks enabling more efficient strategies to perform a task (e.g., Casey et al., Reference Casey, Galvan and Hare2005a; Durston et al., Reference Durston, Davidson, Tottenham, Galvan, Spicer, Fossella and Casey2006). Many studies nowadays also explore the connectivity of the brain at rest (van den Heuvel & Hulshoff Pol, Reference van den Heuvel and Hulshoff Pol2010; van den Heuvel et al., Reference van den Heuvel, Mandl, Kahn and Hulshoff Pol2009). A rapidly increasing number of studies now illustrate that indeed the development of the brain is not only reflected by structural changes, but is also associated with changes in connectivity between brain regions (Dosenbach et al., Reference Dosenbach, Nardos, Cohen, Fair, Power, Church, Nelson, Wig, Vogel, Lessov-Schlaggar, Barnes, Dubis, Feczko, Coalson, Pruett, Barch, Petersen and Schlaggar2010; Fair et al., Reference Fair, Cohen, Power, Dosenbach, Church, Miezin, Schlaggar and Petersen2009; Supekar et al., Reference Supekar, Musen and Menon2009; Supekar et al., Reference Supekar, Uddin, Prater, Amin, Greicius and Menon2010; Power et al., Reference Power, Fair, Schlaggar and Petersen2010). Furthermore, genetic influences on brain activity at rest have been found (Fornito et al., Reference Fornito, Zalesky, Bassett, Meunier, Ellison-Wright, Yucel, Wood, Shaw, Connor, Nertney, Mowry, Pantelis and Bullmore2011; Glahn et al., Reference Glahn, Winkler, Kochunov, Almasy, Duggirala, Carless, Curran, Olvera, Laird, Smith, Beckmann, Fox and Blangero2010; Smit et al., Reference Smit, Stam, Posthuma, Boomsma and de Geus2008), and during performance of cognitive tasks (Blokland et al., Reference Blokland, McMahon, Thompson, Martin, Zubicaray and Wright2011; Koten et al., Reference Koten, Wood, Hagoort, Goebel, Propping, Willmes and Boomsma2009).

It is important to embrace longitudinal projects and to continue ongoing projects that assess brain development and related phenotypes. Following a large group of children for several years is time-consuming and expensive, and researchers face challenges to avoid drop-outs. Our cohort is one of a few pediatric family cohorts that allow the modeling of genetic influences across multiple ages in childhood and puberty. The participants will be invited for a second follow-up. This will bring new opportunities for nonlinear modeling of brain changes and cognitive functioning, and for exploring brain function with functional MRI. This will provide a better understanding of how individual differences in brain development occur during the transition from childhood into adolescence.

Acknowledgments and Funding

We would like to thank all the participants for making this study possible. This work was supported by The Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research (NWO 51.02.060 (H.H.), 668.772 (D.B.); NWO-MagW 480-04-004 (D.B.); NWO/SPI 56-464-14192 (D.B.); NWO/Veni 451-10-007 (J.S.P.)), the European Research Council (ERC-230374) (D.B.), High Potential Grant Utrecht University (H.H.) and with the support of the Neuroscience Campus Amsterdam (NCA).

References

Achenbach, T. M. (1991). Manual for the Child Behavior Checklist/ 4-18 and 1991 Profile. Burlington, VT: Department of Psychiatry, University of Vermont.Google Scholar
Achenbach, T. M., & Rescorla, L. A. (2001). Manual for the ASEBA School-age Forms & Profiles. Burlington VT: Research Center for Children, Youth & Families, University of Vermont.Google Scholar
Akil, H., Martone, M. E., & Van, E. (2011). Challenges and opportunities in mining neuroscience data. Science, 331, 708712.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Hill, J., Raste, Y., & Plumb, I. (2001). The ‘Reading the Mind in the Eyes’ test revised version: A study with normal adults, and adults with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 42, 241251.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bartels, M., de Geus, E. J., Kirschbaum, C., Sluyter, F., & Boomsma, D. I. (2003). Heritability of daytime cortisol levels in children. Behavior Genetics, 33, 421433.Google Scholar
Bartels, M., van Beijsterveldt, C. E., & Boomsma, D. I. (2009). Breastfeeding, maternal education and cognitive function: A prospective study in twins. Behavior Genetics, 39, 616622.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bartels, M., van Beijsterveldt, C. E., Derks, E. M., Stroet, T. M., Polderman, T. J., Hudziak, J. J., & Boomsma, D. I. (2007). Young Netherlands Twin Register (Y-NTR): A longitudinal multiple informant study of problem behavior. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 10, 311.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bartzokis, G., Beckson, M., Lu, P. H., Nuechterlein, K. H., Edwards, N., & Mintz, J. (2001). Age-related changes in frontal and temporal lobe volumes in men: A magnetic resonance imaging study. Archives of General Psychiatry, 58, 461465.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Benes, F. M., Turtle, M., Khan, Y., & Farol, P. (1994). Myelination of a key relay zone in the hippocampal formation occurs in the human brain during childhood, adolescence, and adulthood. Archives of General Psychiatry, 51, 477484.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bienvenu, O. J., Davydow, D. S., & Kendler, K. S. (2011). Psychiatric ‘diseases’ versus behavioral disorders and degree of genetic influence. Psychological Medicine, 41, 3340.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blakemore, S. J., Burnett, S., & Dahl, R. E. (2010). The role of puberty in the developing adolescent brain. Human Brain Mapping, 31, 926933.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blokland, G. A., McMahon, K. L., Thompson, P. M., Martin, N. G., de Zubicaray, G. I., & Wright, M. J. (2011). Heritability of working memory brain activation. Journal of Neuroscience, 31, 1088210890.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boomsma, D. I., de Geus, E. J., Vink, J. M., Stubbe, J. H., Distel, M. A., Hottenga, J. J., Posthuma, D., van Beijsterveldt, T. C., Hudziak, J. J., Bartels, M., & Willemsen, G. (2006). Netherlands Twin Register: From twins to twin families. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 9, 849857.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boomsma, D. I., van Beijsterveldt, C. E., & Hudziak, J. J. (2005). Genetic and environmental influences on Anxious/Depression during childhood: A study from The Netherlands Twin Register. Genes, Brain and Behavior, 4, 466481.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bramen, J. E., Hranilovich, J. A., Dahl, R. E., Forbes, E. E., Chen, J., Toga, A. W., Dinov, I. D., Worthman, C. M., & Sowell, E. R. (2011). Puberty influences medial temporal lobe and cortical gray matter maturation differently in boys than girls matched for sexual maturity. Cerebral Cortex, 21, 636646.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brans, R. G., Kahn, R. S., Schnack, H. G., van Baal, G. C., Posthuma, D., van Haren, N. E., Lepage, C., Lerch, J. P., Collins, D. L., Evans, A. C., Boomsma, D. I., & Hulshoff Pol, H. E. (2010). Brain plasticity and intellectual ability are influenced by shared genes. Journal of Neuroscience, 30, 55195524.Google Scholar
Brouwer, R. M., Mandl, R. C., Peper, J. S., van Baal, G. C., Kahn, R. S., Boomsma, D. I., & Hulshoff Pol, H. E. (2010). Heritability of DTI and MTR in nine-year-old children. Neuroimage, 53, 10851092.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brouwer, S. I., van Beijsterveldt, T. C., Bartels, M., Hudziak, J. J., & Boomsma, D. I. (2006). Influences on achieving motor milestones: A twin-singleton study. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 9, 424430.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Casey, B. J., Galvan, A., & Hare, T. A. (2005a). Changes in cerebral functional organization during cognitive development. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 15, 239244.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Casey, B. J., Tottenham, N., Liston, C., & Durston, S. (2005b). Imaging the developing brain: What have we learned about cognitive development? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9, 104110.Google Scholar
Cito (1995). Drie minuten leestest. Arnhem: Cito.Google Scholar
Corsi, P. M. (1974). Human memory and the medial temporal region of the brain. Dissertation Abstracts International, 34, 819B (University Microfilms No. AAI05–77717).Google Scholar
Crone, E. A. (2009). Executive functions in adolescence: Inferences from brain and behavior. Developmental Science, 12, 825830.Google Scholar
Deary, I. J., Penke, L., & Johnson, W. (2010). The neuroscience of human intelligence differences. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 11, 201211.Google Scholar
Delemarre-van de Waal, H. (2002). Regulation of puberty. Best Practice and Research Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism, 16, 112.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Demetriou, A., Christou, C., Spanoudis, G., & Platsidou, M. (2002). The development of mental processing: Efficiency, working memory, and thinking. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 67, Serial Number 268.Google Scholar
de Zeeuw, L. E. J., van Beijsterveldt, C. E. M., de Geus, E. J. C., & Boomsma, D. I. (2012). Twin specific risk factors in primary school achievements. Manuscript submitted for publication.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dosenbach, N. U., Nardos, B., Cohen, A. L., Fair, D. A., Power, J. D., Church, J. A., Nelson, S. M., Wig, G. S., Vogel, A. C., Lessov-Schlaggar, C. N., Barnes, K. A., Dubis, J. W., Feczko, E., Coalson, R. S., PruettJ. R., Jr. J. R., Jr., Barch, D. M., Petersen, S. E., & Schlaggar, B. L. (2010). Prediction of individual brain maturity using fMRI. Science, 329, 13581361.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Durston, S., Davidson, M. C., Tottenham, N., Galvan, A., Spicer, J., Fossella, J. A., & Casey, B. J. (2006). A shift from diffuse to focal cortical activity with development. Developmental Science, 9, 18.Google Scholar
Durston, S., Nederveen, H., van Dijk, S., van Belle, J., de Zeeuw, P., Langen, M., & van Dijk, A. (2009). Magnetic resonance simulation is effective in reducing anxiety related to magnetic resonance scanning in children. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 48, 206207.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eriksen, B. A., & Eriksen, C. W. (1974). Effects of noise letters upon identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task. Perception & Psychophysics, 16, 143149.Google Scholar
Eriksen, C. W., & Schultz, D. W. (1979). Information processing in visual-search–continuous-flow conception and experimental results. Perception & Psychophysics, 25, 249263.Google Scholar
Estourgie-van Burk, G. F., Bartels, M., Boomsma, D. I., & Delemarre-van de Waal, H. A. (2010). Body size of twins compared with siblings and the general population: From birth to late adolescence. Journal of Pediatrics, 156, 586591.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fair, D. A., Cohen, A. L., Power, J. D., Dosenbach, N. U., Church, J. A., Miezin, F. M., Schlaggar, B. L., & Petersen, S. E. (2009). Functional brain networks develop from a ‘local to distributed’ organization. PLoS Computational Biology, 5, e1000381.Google Scholar
Faraone, S. V., Perlis, R. H., Doyle, A. E., Smoller, J. W., Goralnick, J. J., Holmgren, M. A., & Sklar, P. (2005). Molecular genetics of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 57, 13131323.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Felsenfeld, S., van Beijsterveldt, C. E., & Boomsma, D. I. (2010). Attentional regulation in young twins with probable stuttering, high nonfluency, and typical fluency. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 53, 11471166.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fornito, A., Zalesky, A., Bassett, D. S., Meunier, D., Ellison-Wright, I., Yucel, M., Wood, S. J., Shaw, K., O’ Connor, J., Nertney, D., Mowry, B. J., Pantelis, C., & Bullmore, E. T. (2011). Genetic influences on cost-efficient organization of human cortical functional networks. Journal of Neuroscience, 31, 32613270.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Franic, S., Middeldorp, C. M., Dolan, C. V., Ligthart, L., & Boomsma, D. I. (2010). Childhood and adolescent anxiety and depression: Beyond heritability. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 49, 820829.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gathercole, S. E., Pickering, S. J., Ambridge, B., & Wearing, H. (2004). The structure of working memory from 4 to 15 years of age. Developmental Psychology, 40, 177190.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gevins, A., & Cutillo, B. (1993). Spatiotemporal dynamics of component processes in human working memory. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 87, 128143.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Giedd, J. N., Blumenthal, J., Jeffries, N. O., Castellanos, F. X., Liu, H., Zijdenbos, A., Paus, T., Evans, A. C., & Rapoport, J. L. (1999). Brain development during childhood and adolescence: A longitudinal MRI study. Nature Neuroscience, 2, 861863.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Giedd, J. N., Clasen, L. S., Lenroot, R., Greenstein, D., Wallace, G. L., Ordaz, S., Molloy, E. A., Blumenthal, J. D., Tossell, J. W., Stayer, C., Samango-Sprouse, C. A., Shen, D., Davatzikos, C., Merke, D., Chrousos, G. P. (2006). Puberty-related influences on brain development. Molecular and Cellular Endocrinology, 254–255, 154162.Google Scholar
Glahn, D. C., Winkler, A. M., Kochunov, P., Almasy, L., Duggirala, R., Carless, M. A., Curran, J. C., Olvera, R. L., Laird, A. R., Smith, S. M., Beckmann, C. F., Fox, P. T., & Blangero, J. (2010). Genetic control over the resting brain. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 107, 12231228.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gogtay, N., Giedd, J. N., Lusk, L., Hayashi, K. M., Greenstein, D., Vaituzis, A. C., Nugent, T. F., Herman, D. H., Clasen, L. S., Toga, A. W., Rapoport, J. L., & Thompson, P. M. (2004). Dynamic mapping of human cortical development during childhood through early adulthood. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 101, 81748179.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gogtay, N., Giedd, J., & Rapoport, J. L. (2002). Brain development in healthy, hyperactive, and psychotic children. Archives of Neurology, 59, 12441248.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gogtay, N., & Thompson, P. M. (2010). Mapping gray matter development: Implications for typical development and vulnerability to psychopathology. Brain and Cognition, 72, 615.Google Scholar
Gur, R. C., Ragland, J. D., Moberg, P. J., Turner, T. H., Bilker, W. B., Kohler, C., Siegel, S. J., & Gur, R. E. (2001). Computerized neurocognitive scanning: I. Methodology and validation in healthy people. Neuropsychopharmacology, 25, 766776.Google Scholar
Gur, R. C., Richard, J., Hughett, P., Calkins, M. E., Macy, L., Bilker, W. B., Brensinger, C., & Gur, R. E. (2010). A cognitive neuroscience-based computerized battery for efficient measurement of individual differences: Standardization and initial construct validation. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 187, 254262.Google Scholar
Herting, M. M., Maxwell, E. C., Irvine, C., & Nagel, B. J. (2011). The impact of sex, puberty, and hormones on white matter microstructure in adolescents. Cerebral Cortex, Oct 14. [Epub ahead of print] Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/22002939Google Scholar
Hoekstra, R. A., Bartels, M., Cath, D. C., & Boomsma, D. I. (2008). Factor structure, reliability and criterion validity of the Autism-Spectrum Quotient (AQ): A study in Dutch population and patient groups. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38, 15551566.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoekstra, R. A., Bartels, M., van Leeuwen, M., & Boomsma, D. I. (2009). Genetic architecture of verbal abilities in children and adolescents. Developmental Science, 12, 10411053.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hulshoff Pol, H. E., Schnack, H. G., Posthuma, D., Mandl, R. C., Baare, W. F., van Oel, C., van Haren, N. E., Collins, D. L., Evans, A. C., Amunts, K., Burgel, U., Zilles, K., De Geus, E., Boomsma, D. I., & Kahn, R. S. (2006). Genetic contributions to human brain morphology and intelligence. Journal of Neuroscience, 26, 1023510242.Google Scholar
Huttenlocher, P. R., & Dabholkar, A. S. (1997). Regional differences in synaptogenesis in human cerebral cortex. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 387, 167178.Google Scholar
Insel, T. R. (2010). Rethinking schizophrenia. Nature, 468, 187193.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jansma, J. M., Ramsey, N. F., Coppola, R., & Kahn, R. S. (2000). Specific versus nonspecific brain activity in a parametric N-back task. Neuroimage, 12, 688697.Google Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2010). Neuroimaging of the developing brain: Taking ‘developing’ seriously. Human Brain Mapping, 31, 934941.Google Scholar
Keating, D. (2004). Cognitive and brain development. In Steinberg, L. (Ed.), Handbook of adolescent psychology (2nd ed.). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Kessler, R. C., Angermeyer, M., Anthony, J. C., de Graff, R., Demyttenaere, K., Gasquet, I., de Girolamo, G., Gluzman, S., Gureje, O., Haro, J. M., Kawakami, N., Karam, A., Levinson, D., Medina Mora, M. E., Oakley Browne, M. A., Posada-Villa, J., Stein, D. J., Adley Tsang, C. H., Aguilar-Gaxiola, S., Alonso, J., Lee, S., Heeringa, S., Pennell, B. E., Berglund, P., Gruber, M. J., Petukhova, M., Chatterji, S., & Ustün, T. B. (2007). Lifetime prevalence and age-of-onset distributions of mental disorders in the World Health Organization's World Mental Health Survey Initiative. World Psychiatry, 6, 168176.Google Scholar
Kochunov, P., Glahn, D. C., Fox, P. T., Lancaster, J. L., Saleem, K., Shelledy, W., Zilles, K., Thompson, P. M., Coulon, O., Mangin, J. F., Blangero, J., & Rogers, J. (2010). Genetics of primary cerebral gyrification: Heritability of length, depth and area of primary sulci in an extended pedigree of Papio baboons. Neuroimage, 53, 11261134.Google Scholar
Koten, J. W., Wood, G., Hagoort, P., Goebel, R., Propping, P., Willmes, K., & Boomsma, D. I. (2009). Genetic contribution to variation in cognitive function: an FMRI study in twins. Science, 323, 17371740.Google Scholar
Kremen, W. S., Prom-Wormley, E., Panizzon, M. S., Eyler, L. T., Fischl, B., Neale, M. C., Franz, C. E., Lyons, M. J., Pacheco, J., Perry, M. E., Stevens, A., Schmitt, J. E., Grant, M. D., Seidman, L. J., Thermenos, H. W., Tsuang, M. T., Eisen, S. A., Dale, A. M., & Fennema-Notestine, C. (2010). Genetic and environmental influences on the size of specific brain regions in midlife: The VETSA MRI study. Neuroimage, 49, 12131223.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lebel, C., & Beaulieu, C. (2011). Longitudinal development of human brain wiring continues from childhood into adulthood. Journal of Neuroscience, 31, 1093710947.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lenroot, R. K., Gogtay, N., Greenstein, D. K., Wells, E. M., Wallace, G. L., Clasen, L. S., Blumenthal, J. D., Lerch, J., Zijdenbos, A. P., Evans, A. C., Thompson, P. M., & Giedd, J. N. (2007). Sexual dimorphism of brain developmental trajectories during childhood and adolescence. Neuroimage, 36, 10651073.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lenroot, R. K., Schmitt, J. E., Ordaz, S. J., Wallace, G. L., Neale, M. C., Lerch, J. P., Kendler, K. S., Evans, A. C., & Giedd, J. N. (2009). Differences in genetic and environmental influences on the human cerebral cortex associated with development during childhood and adolescence. Human Brain Mapping, 30, 163174.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Limperopoulos, C., Soul, J. S., Gauvreau, K., Huppi, P. S., Warfield, S. K., Bassan, H., Robertson, R. L., Volpe, J. J., & du Plessis, A. J. (2005). Late gestation cerebellar growth is rapid and impeded by premature birth. Pediatrics, 115, 688695.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luciano, M., Smith, G. A., Wright, M. J., Geffen, G. M., Geffen, L. B., & Martin, N. G. (2001). On the heritability of inspection time and its covariance with IQ: A twin study. Intelligence, 29, 443457.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Luna, B., & Sweeney, J. A. (2001). Studies of brain and cognitive maturation through childhood and adolescence: A strategy for testing neurodevelopmental hypotheses. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 27, 443455.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marshall, W. A., & Tanner, J. M. (1969). Variations in pattern of pubertal changes in girls. Archives of Diseases in Childhood, 44, 291303.Google Scholar
Marshall, W. A., & Tanner, J. M. (1970). Variations in the pattern of pubertal changes in boys. Archives of Diseases in Childhood, 45, 1323.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Muhle, R., Trentacoste, S. V., & Rapin, I. (2004). The genetics of autism. Pediatrics, 113, e472e486.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Neufang, S., Specht, K., Hausmann, M., Gunturkun, O., Herpertz-Dahlmann, B., Fink, G. R., & Konrad, K. (2009). Sex differences and the impact of steroid hormones on the developing human brain. Cerebral Cortex, 19, 464473.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oldfield, R. C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: The Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia, 9, 97113.Google Scholar
Panizzon, M. S., Fennema-Notestine, C., Eyler, L. T., Jernigan, T. L., Prom-Wormley, E., Neale, M., Jacobson, K., Lyons, M. J., Grant, M. D., Franz, C. E., Xian, H., Tsuang, M., Fischl, B., Seidman, L., Dale, A., & Kremen, W. S. (2009). Distinct genetic influences on cortical surface area and cortical thickness. Cerebral Cortex, 19, 27282735.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paus, T. (2005). Mapping brain maturation and cognitive development during adolescence. Trends in Cognitive Science, 9, 6068.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paus, T., Keshavan, M., & Giedd, J. N. (2008). Why do many psychiatric disorders emerge during adolescence? Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 9, 947957.Google Scholar
Peper, J. S., Brouwer, R. M., Schnack, H. G., van Baal, G. C., van Leeuwen, M., van den Berg, S. M., Delemarre-van de Waal, H. A., Boomsma, D. I., Kahn, R. S., & Hulshoff Pol, H. E. (2009a). Sex steroids and brain structure in pubertal boys and girls. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 34, 332342.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peper, J. S., Brouwer, R. M., Schnack, H. G., van Baal, G. C., van Leeuwen, M., van den Berg, S. M., Delemarre-van de Waal, H. A., Janke, A. L., Collins, D. L., Evans, A. C., Boomsma, D. I., Kahn, R. S., & Hulshoff Pol, H. E. (2008). Cerebral white matter in early puberty is associated with luteinizing hormone concentrations. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 33, 909915.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peper, J. S., Brouwer, R. M., van Baal, G. C., Schnack, H. G., van Leeuwen, M., Boomsma, D. I., Kahn, R. S., & Hulshoff Pol, H. E. (2009b). Does having a twin brother make for a bigger brain? European Journal of Endocrinology, 160, 739746.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peper, J. S., Brouwer, R. M., van Leeuwen, M., Schnack, H. G., Boomsma, D. I., Kahn, R. S., & Hulshoff Pol, H. E. (2010). HPG axis hormones during puberty: A study on the association with hypothalamic and pituitary volumes. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 35, 133140.Google Scholar
Peper, J. S., Hulshoff Pol, H. E., Crone, E. A., & Honk, J. (2011) Sex steroids and brain structure in pubertal boys and girls: A mini-review of neuroimaging studies. Neuroscience, 191, 2837.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peper, J. S., Schnack, H. G., Brouwer, R. M., van Baal, G. C., Pjetri, E., Szekely, E., van Leeuwen, M., van den Berg, S. M., Collins, D. L., Evans, A. C., Boomsma, D. I., Kahn, R. S., & Hulshoff Pol, H. E. (2009c). Heritability of regional and global brain structure at the onset of puberty: A magnetic resonance imaging study in 9-year-old twin pairs. Human Brain Mapping, 30, 21842196.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Posthuma, D., de Geus, E. J., Baare, W. F., Hulshoff Pol, H. E., Kahn, R. S., & Boomsma, D. I. (2002). The association between brain volume and intelligence is of genetic origin. Nature Neuroscience, 5, 8384.Google Scholar
Power, J. D., Fair, D. A., Schlaggar, B. L., & Petersen, S. E. (2010). The development of human functional brain networks. Neuron, 67, 735748.Google Scholar
Ramsden, S., Richardson, F. M., Josse, G., Thomas, M. S. C., Ellis, C., Shakeshaft, C., Seghier, M. L., & Price, C. J. (2011). Verbal and non-verbal intelligence changes in the teenage brain. Nature, 479, 113116.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raven, J. C. (1960). Guide to the Standard Progressive Matrices. London: H.K. Lewis.Google Scholar
Raven, J., Raven, J. C., & Court, J. H. (1998). Raven manual: Section 4 Advanced Progressive Matrices. (1998 ed.) Oxford: Oxford Psychologists Press.Google Scholar
Read, G. F., Walker, R. F., Wilson, D. W., & Griffiths, K. (1990). Steroid analysis in saliva for the assessment of endocrine function. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 595, 260274.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Riad-Fahmy, D., Read, G. F., Walker, R. F., Walker, S. M., & Griffiths, K. (1987). Determination of ovarian steroid hormone levels in saliva. An overview. Journal of Reproductive Medicine, 32, 254272.Google ScholarPubMed
Rimol, L. M., Panizzon, M. S., Fennema-Notestine, C., Eyler, L. T., Fischl, B., Franz, C. E., Hagler, D. J., Lyons, M. J., Neale, M. C., Pacheco, J., Perry, M. E., Schmitt, J. E., Grant, M. D., Seidman, L. J., Thermenos, H. W., Tsuang, M. T., Eisen, S. A., Kremen, W. S., & Dale, A. M. (2010). Cortical thickness is influenced by regionally specific genetic factors. Biological Psychiatry, 67, 493499.Google Scholar
Rogers, J., Kochunov, P., Lancaster, J., Shelledy, W., Glahn, D., Blangero, J., & Fox, P. (2007). Heritability of brain volume, surface area and shape: An MRI study in an extended pedigree of baboons. Human Brain Mapping, 28, 576583.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rogers, J., Kochunov, P., Zilles, K., Shelledy, W., Lancaster, J., Thompson, P., Duggirala, R., Blangero, J., Fox, P. T., & Glahn, D. C. (2010). On the genetic architecture of cortical folding and brain volume in primates. Neuroimage, 53, 11031108.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schmitt, J. E., Eyler, L. T., Giedd, J. N., Kremen, W. S., Kendler, K. S., & Neale, M. C. (2007). Review of twin and family studies on neuroanatomic phenotypes and typical neurodevelopment. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 10, 683694.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shaw, P., Eckstrand, K., Sharp, W., Blumenthal, J., Lerch, J. P., Greenstein, D., Clasen, L., Evans, A., Giedd, J., & Rapoport, J. L. (2007). Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder is characterized by a delay in cortical maturation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 104, 1964919654.Google Scholar
Shaw, P., Gogtay, N., & Rapoport, J. (2010). Childhood psychiatric disorders as anomalies in neurodevelopmental trajectories. Human Brain Mapping, 31, 917925.Google Scholar
Shaw, P., Greenstein, D., Lerch, J., Clasen, L., Lenroot, R., Gogtay, N., Evans, A., Rapoport, J., & Giedd, J. (2006). Intellectual ability and cortical development in children and adolescents. Nature, 440, 676679.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Simon, H. A. (1975). Functional equivalence of problem solving skills. Cognitive Psychology, 7, 268288.Google Scholar
Sisk, C. L., & Foster, D. L. (2004). The neural basis of puberty and adolescence. Nature Neuroscience, 7, 10401047.Google Scholar
Smit, D. J., Stam, C. J., Posthuma, D., Boomsma, D. I., & de Geus, E. J. (2008). Heritability of ‘small-world’ networks in the brain: A graph theoretical analysis of resting-state EEG functional connectivity. Human Brain Mapping, 29, 13681378.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sowell, E. R., Trauner, D. A., Gamst, A., & Jernigan, T. L. (2002). Development of cortical and subcortical brain structures in childhood and adolescence: A structural MRI study. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 44, 416.Google Scholar
Steinberg, L. (2005). Cognitive and affective development in adolescence. Trends in Cognitive Science, 9, 6974.Google Scholar
Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18, 643662.Google Scholar
Supekar, K., Musen, M., & Menon, V. (2009). Development of large-scale functional brain networks in children. PLoS Biology, 7, e1000157.Google Scholar
Supekar, K., Uddin, L. Q., Prater, K., Amin, H., Greicius, M. D., & Menon, V. (2010). Development of functional and structural connectivity within the default mode network in young children. Neuroimage, 52, 290301.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tamnes, C. K., Ostby, Y., Fjell, A. M., Westlye, L. T., Due-Tonnessen, P., & Walhovd, K. B. (2010). Brain maturation in adolescence and young adulthood: Regional age-related changes in cortical thickness and white matter volume and microstructure. Cerebral Cortex, 20, 534548.Google Scholar
Tanner, J. M., & Whitehouse, R. H. (1976). Clinical longitudinal standards for height, weight, height velocity, weight velocity, and stages of puberty. Archives of Diseases in Childhood, 51, 170179.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tiemeier, H., Lenroot, R. K., Greenstein, D. K., Tran, L., Pierson, R., & Giedd, J. N. (2010). Cerebellum development during childhood and adolescence: A longitudinal morphometric MRI study. Neuroimage, 49, 6370.Google Scholar
van Beijsterveldt, C. E., & Boomsma, D. I. (2007). Genetics of parentally reported asthma, eczema and rhinitis in 5-yr-old twins. European Respiratory Journal, 29, 516521.Google Scholar
van den Burg, W., & Kingma, A. (1999). Performance of 225 Dutch school children on Rey's Auditory Verbal Learning Test (AVLT): Parallel test-retest reliabilities with an interval of 3 months and normative data. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 14, 545559.Google Scholar
van den Heuvel, M. P. & Hulshoff Pol, H. E. (2010). Exploring the brain network: A review on resting-state fMRI functional connectivity. European Neuropsychopharmacology, 20, 519534.Google Scholar
van den Heuvel, M. P., Mandl, R. C., Kahn, R. S., & Hulshoff Pol, H. E. (2009). Functionally linked resting-state networks reflect the underlying structural connectivity architecture of the human brain. Human Brain Mapping, 30, 31273141.Google Scholar
van Leeuwen, M., Peper, J. S., van den Berg, S. M., Brouwer, R. M., Hulschoff Pol, E. H., Kahn, R. S., & Boomsma, D. I. (2009a). A genetic analysis of brain volumes and IQ in children. Intelligence, 37, 181191.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van Leeuwen, M., van den Berg, S. M., & Boomsma, D. I. (2008). A twin-family study of general IQ. Learning and Individual Differences, 18, 7688.Google Scholar
van Leeuwen, M., van den Berg, S. M., Hoekstra, R. A., & Boomsma, D. I. (2007). Endophenotypes for intelligence in children and adolescents. Intelligence, 35, 369380.Google Scholar
van Leeuwen, M., van den Berg, S. M., Hoekstra, R. A., & Boomsma, D. I. (2009b). The genetic and environmental structure of verbal and visuospatial memory in young adults and children. Neuropsychology, 23, 792802.Google Scholar
van Leeuwen, M., van den Berg, S. M., Peper, J. S., Hulschoff Pol, H. E., & Boomsma, D. I. (2009c). Genetic covariance structure of reading, intelligence and memory in children. Behavior Genetics, 39, 245254.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Leijenhorst, L., Moor, B. G., de Macks, Z. A. O., Rombouts, S. A. R. B., Westenberg, P. M., & Crone, E. A. (2010). Adolescent risky decision-making: Neurocognitive development of reward and control regions. Neuroimage, 51, 345355.Google Scholar
van Soelen, I. L. C., Brouwer, R. M., Peper, J. S., van Beijsterveldt, T. C., van, L. M., de Vries, L. S., Kahn, R. S., Hulshoff Pol, H. E., & Boomsma, D. I. (2010). Effects of Gestational Age and Birth Weight on Brain Volumes in Healthy 9 Year-Old Children. Journal of Pediatrics, 156, 896901.Google Scholar
van Soelen, I. L. C., Brouwer, R. M., van Baal, G. C. M., Schnack, H. G., Peper, J. S., Chen, L., Kahn, R. S., Boomsma, D. I., & Hulshoff Pol, H. E. (2011a). Heritability of volumetric brain changes and height in children entering puberty. Human Brain Mapping. doi:10.1002/hbm.21468Google Scholar
van Soelen, I. L. C., Brouwer, R. M., van Baal, G. C. M., Schnack, H. G., Peper, J. S., Collins, D. L., Evans, A., Kahn, R. S., Boomsma, D. I., & Hulshoff Pol, H. E. (2011b). Genetic influences on thinning of the cerebral cortex during development. NeuroImage, 59, 38713880.Google Scholar
van Soelen, I. L. C., Brouwer, R. M., van Leeuwen, M., Kahn, R. S., Hulshoff Pol, H. E., & Boomsma, D. I. (2011c). Heritability of verbal and performance intelligence in a pediatric longitudinal sample. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 14, 119128.Google Scholar
van Soelen, I. L. C., van den Berg, S. M., Dekker, P. H., van Leeuwen, M., Peper, J. S., Hulschoff Pol, H. E., & Boomsma, D. I. (2009). Individual differences in dynamic measures of verbal learning abilities in young twin pairs and their older siblings. Learning and Individual Differences, 19, 440444.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Verhulst, F. C., van der Ende, J., & Koot, H. M. (1997). Handleiding voor de Youth Self Report (YSR) [Dutch manual for the YSR]. Rotterdam, The Netherlands: Academic Medical Center, Rotterdam / Erasmus University, Sophia Children's Hospital, Department of Child Psychiatry.Google Scholar
Walker, R. F., Read, G. F., Wilson, D. W., Riad-Fahmy, D., & Griffiths, K. (1990). Chronobiology in laboratory medicine: Principles and clinical applications illustrated from measurements of neutral steroids in saliva. Progress in Clinical Biological Research, 341A, 105117.Google Scholar
Wallace, G. L., Dankner, N., Kenworthy, L., Giedd, J. N., & Martin, A. (2010). Age-related temporal and parietal cortical thinning in autism-spectrum disorders. Brain, 133, 37453754.Google Scholar
Wallace, G. L., Eric, S. J., Lenroot, R., Viding, E., Ordaz, S., Rosenthal, M. A., Molloy, E. A., Clasen, L. S., Kendler, K. S., Neale, M. C., & Giedd, J. N. (2006). A pediatric twin study of brain morphometry. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47, 987993.Google Scholar
Wechsler, D., Kort, W., Compaan, E. L., Belichrodt, N., Resing, W. C. M., Schittekatte, M., Bosmans, M., Vermeir, G., & Verhaeghe, P. (2002). Handleiding WISC-III nl [Manual WISC-III nl]. London: The Psychological Corporation Limited, Nederlands Insitiuut van Psychologen Dienstencentrum.Google Scholar
Yakovlev, P. I., & Lecours, A. (1967). The myelogenetic cycles of regional maturation of the brain. In Minkowski, A. (Ed.), Regional development of the brain in early life (pp. 370). Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Yoon, U., Fahim, C., Perusse, D., & Evans, A. C. (2010). Lateralized genetic and environmental influences on human brain morphology of 8-year-old twins. Neuroimage, 53, 11171125.Google Scholar
Figure 0

TABLE 1 Number of Participants, Age, IQ, and Handedness at Baseline and Follow-up

Figure 1

FIGURE 1 Data collection at baseline and at follow-up for cognitive functioning (number of psychometric IQ tests for twins and siblings, and Raven Advanced Progressive matrices for parents) and brain structure (number of MRI scans after image processing).

Figure 2

TABLE 2 Mean Scores for Main Variables Collected at Baseline and Follow-up for Twins and Their Siblings

Figure 3

TABLE 3 Main Findings for Cognition and Brain Structure

Figure 4

FIGURE 2 Volumetric changes between ages 9 and 12 years. Absolute brain volume of total cerebral volume (A), cerebral gray matter (B), cerebral white matter (C), and total cerebellar volume (D) are given on all included subjects at baseline and at follow-up. Longitudinal data points are connected, with boys depicted in blue, and girls in red (van Soelen et al., 2001a, reprinted with permission).