Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-rdxmf Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-22T05:48:12.494Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

A machine learning ensemble to predict treatment outcomes following an Internet intervention for depression

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 November 2018

Rahel Pearson
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Institute for Mental Health Research, University of Texas at Austin, Austin, TX, USA
Derek Pisner
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Institute for Mental Health Research, University of Texas at Austin, Austin, TX, USA
Björn Meyer
Affiliation:
Gaia AG, Hamburg, Germany University of London, London, England, UK
Jason Shumake
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Institute for Mental Health Research, University of Texas at Austin, Austin, TX, USA
Christopher G. Beevers*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Institute for Mental Health Research, University of Texas at Austin, Austin, TX, USA
*
Author for correspondence: Christopher G. Beevers, E-mail: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Some Internet interventions are regarded as effective treatments for adult depression, but less is known about who responds to this form of treatment.

Method

An elastic net and random forest were trained to predict depression symptoms and related disability after an 8-week course of an Internet intervention, Deprexis, involving adults (N = 283) from across the USA. Candidate predictors included psychopathology, demographics, treatment expectancies, treatment usage, and environmental context obtained from population databases. Model performance was evaluated using predictive R2$\lpar R_{{\rm pred}}^2\rpar\comma $ the expected variance explained in a new sample, estimated by 10 repetitions of 10-fold cross-validation.

Results

An ensemble model was created by averaging the predictions of the elastic net and random forest. Model performance was compared with a benchmark linear autoregressive model that predicted each outcome using only its baseline. The ensemble predicted more variance in post-treatment depression (8.0% gain, 95% CI 0.8–15; total $R_{{\rm pred}}^2 \; $= 0.25), disability (5.0% gain, 95% CI −0.3 to 10; total $R_{{\rm pred}}^2 \; $= 0.25), and well-being (11.6% gain, 95% CI 4.9–19; total $R_{{\rm pred}}^2 \; $= 0.29) than the benchmark model. Important predictors included comorbid psychopathology, particularly total psychopathology and dysthymia, low symptom-related disability, treatment credibility, lower access to therapists, and time spent using certain Deprexis modules.

Conclusion

A number of variables predict symptom improvement following an Internet intervention, but each of these variables makes relatively small contributions. Machine learning ensembles may be a promising statistical approach for identifying the cumulative contribution of many weak predictors to psychosocial depression treatment response.

Type
Original Articles
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2018

Self-guided Internet interventions appear to effectively reduce symptoms of depression. A recent meta-analysis of 16 randomized clinical trials found that self-guided CBT interventions were significantly more effective at reducing depression symptoms than control treatments (number needed to treat = 8) (Karyotaki et al., Reference Karyotaki, Riper, Twisk, Hoogendoorn, Kleiboer, Mira, Mackinnon, Meyer, Botella, Littlewood, Andersson, Christensen, Klein, Schröder, Bretón-López, Scheider, Griffiths, Farrer, Huibers, Phillips, Gilbody, Moritz, Berger, Pop, Spek and Cuijpers2017). However, despite the effectiveness of these treatments on average, it is clear that these interventions are neither equally effective nor effective for all participants. Identifying which individuals are most likely to benefit from a potentially effective treatment is essential to developing an efficient and personalized health care system (Cuijpers et al., Reference Cuijpers, Ebert, Acarturk, Andersson and Cristea2016).

Precision medicine, aimed at identifying individual differences that predict beneficial and/or adverse effects, has recently emerged as a major goal in health care (Hamburg and Collins, Reference Hamburg and Collins2010). Although many predictors of response to depression treatment using traditional statistical methods have been examined (Huang et al., Reference Huang, LePendu, Iyer, Tai-Seale, Carrell and Shah2014), the predictive power of each variable in isolation is often relatively weak. As a result, data-based personalized treatment recommendations are not routinely offered for any form of depression treatment (although see Fisher, Reference Fisher2015; Fernandez et al., Reference Fernandez, Fisher and Chi2017), despite the known benefits of actuarial approaches (Dawes et al., Reference Dawes, Faust and Meehl1989).

New statistical methods, such as machine learning, may allow for the development of treatment algorithms that can predict with high accuracy whether or not treatment may be successful for a given individual with a specific set of attributes. Specifically, for traditional linear models to be stable and reproducible, the number of predictors must be kept small relative to the sample size, and subsets of predictors cannot be highly correlated with one another. As a consequence, traditional multiple regression is not well suited to data-mining challenges, i.e. when there are hundreds of potentially relevant predictors, many of which share at least some degree of redundancy. For such problems, machine learning methods can outperform traditional stepwise selection methods by capturing the simultaneous effect of all relevant predictors rather than considering discrete subsets of predictors one at a time and discarding all but the strongest (Hastie et al., Reference Hastie, Tibshirani and Friedman2009). This approach has been successfully used in medicine, most notably in oncology, where machine learning approaches have been applied to predict cancer progression and treatment outcome (Cruz and Wishart, Reference Cruz and Wishart2007). More recently, machine learning was successfully used to predict treatment response to antidepressant medication (Khodayari-Rostamabad et al., Reference Khodayari-Rostamabad, Reilly, Hasey, de Bruin and Maccrimmon2013; Chekroud et al., Reference Chekroud, Zotti, Shehzad, Gueorguieva, Johnson, Trivedi, Cannon, Krystal and Corlett2016) and electroconvulsive therapy (Redlich et al., Reference Redlich, Opel, Grotegerd, Dohm, Zaremba, Bürger, Münker, Mühlmann, Wahl, Heindel, Arolt, Alferink, Zwanzger, Zavorotnyy, Kugel and Dannlowski2016). The current study applies machine learning methods to predict response to a psychosocial intervention for depression (for a review, see Cohen and DeRubeis, Reference Cohen and DeRubeis2018).

The current study involved a secondary analysis of data from a recently published clinical trial examining the effectiveness of an Internet intervention for depression among adults recruited from across the USA (Beevers et al., Reference Beevers, Pearson, Hoffman, Foulser, Shumake and Meyer2017). An ensemble of elastic net and random forest learners was used to predict treatment outcomes. Symptom outcomes included interviewer-rated depression, symptom-related disability, and well-being (i.e. positive affect). Candidate predictors included relatively low-cost and easy to obtain self-report data including concurrent psychopathology, demographics, treatment expectancies, treatment usage, and, given the geographic diversity of our sample, environmental context variables mined from population databases based on participants’ postal address.

Methods

Study design

Data were obtained from a recently published clinical trial comparing a depression-focused Internet intervention, Deprexis, to an 8-week waitlist control condition. Treatment response was comparable for participants who received Deprexis immediately v. after an 8-week waiting period (Beevers et al., Reference Beevers, Pearson, Hoffman, Foulser, Shumake and Meyer2017). Thus, to maximize sample size for machine learning analyses, participants who received Deprexis immediately (N = 222) or after an 8-week waiting period (N = 61) were combined for analyses. Whether or not treatment was administered immediately or after a delay was also included as a predictor variable, but it was not a highly important predictor of post-treatment outcome in any model (depending on outcome and importance metric, it ranked between 67 and 114 out of 120 predictors). We used a complete case approach, utilizing data from participants who provided data at pre- and post-treatment assessments (N ranged from 283 to 271, depending on missing outcome data).

Deprexis is an Internet treatment for unipolar depression that was provided with relatively minimal user support. The intervention consists of 10 content modules that include behavioral activation, cognitive modification, relaxation, exercise and lifestyle modification, acceptance and mindfulness, problem-solving, childhood experiences, interpersonal skills, positive psychology, and dream work [for more detail, see Table 1 from Beevers et al. (Reference Beevers, Pearson, Hoffman, Foulser, Shumake and Meyer2017)]. Further, daily brief messages are sent automatically by the program and are intended to remind and motivate the users to engage with the program. A recent meta-analyses of eight Deprexis trials with a total of 2402 participants has yielded a medium effect size of Hedges’ g = 0.54 for this intervention, compared with control conditions, with no evidence of publication bias or developer involvement bias (Twomey et al., Reference Twomey, O'Reilly and Meyer2017).

Study inclusion criteria were: (1) age between 18 and 55; (2) English fluency; (3) reliable access to the Internet (i.e. dialup or broadband access); (4) willingness to provide saliva for DNA research; (5) presence of moderate levels of depression or greater (QIDS score ⩾10) at time of eligibility screening; (6) treatment stability (no changes in psychotropic medication or psychosocial treatment in the 30 days prior to study entry); and (7) living in the USA. Exclusion criteria were: (1) presence of psychotic or substance use symptoms; (2) a diagnosis of bipolar disorder; or (3) suicidal risk (defined as having suicidal ideation with intent with/or without a plan in the last 90 days or attempting suicide in the past year).

Participants were primarily female (74.4%) and in their early 30s (mean age, 32 years old, range 18–55). Participants also tended to be single, non-Hispanic white, and have at least some college education. Although the majority of participants were recruited from the state of Texas (50.2%), there was a geographic diversity (see Fig. 1). Approximately 40% of the sample was currently receiving antidepressant treatment and more than 2/3 of the sample had received psychological treatment at some point in their lifetime.

Fig. 1. Geographical distribution of study participants. Note that most participants (approximately 50%) were recruited from the state of Texas.

Clinical outcomes

Machine learning was used to predict three treatment outcomes: the Hamilton Rating Scale for Depression (HRSD; Hamilton, Reference Hamilton1960), Sheehan Disability Scale (SDS; Sheehan et al., Reference Sheehan, Harnett-Sheehan and Raj1996), and the Well-Being subscale of the Inventory of Depressive Symptoms (IDAS; Watson et al., Reference Watson, O'Hara, Chmielewski, McDade-Montez, Koffel, Naragon and Stuart2008). The SDS and IDAS Well-Being were selected because they are important correlates of depression not well captured by the HRSD, were measured at the same time as the HRSD (i.e. at pre- and post-treatment), and, like the HRSD, showed substantial individual differences in treatment outcome. For more information about the psychometrics of the clinical outcomes, see online Supplementary materials, section 1.0.

Candidate predictors

Given the exploratory, data-driven approach to analyses, all available data were utilized for each clinical post-treatment outcome to identify the most important candidate predictors. Total score and subscale scores if available (or individual items if not) were used as potential candidate predictors for the following self-report scales measured at pre-treatment: QIDS-SR, HRSD, Psychiatric Diagnostic Screening Questionnaire (PDSQ; Zimmerman and Mattia, Reference Zimmerman and Mattia2001), SDS, IDAS Ill Temper, IDAS Well-Being, IDAS Social Anxiety, IDAS Traumatic Intrusion, IDAS Panic, Treatment Credibility and Expectancies Questionnaire (Devilly and Borkovec, Reference Devilly and Borkovec2000), and the Risky Families Questionnaire (Taylor et al., Reference Taylor, Lerner, Sage, Lehman and Seeman2004). Clinical candidate predictors were also included, such as past and current psychotherapy, other health problems, family history of mental illness, antidepressant usage, and participant demographic variables, such as age, marital status, household size and composition, income, education level, sexual orientation, and ethnicity.

Given the geographic diversity of our participants, environmental context variables were also obtained by joining participant data to population databases (e.g. census data) based on participants’ zip code (postal address). Examples of available candidate predictors based on zip code of the participant included median household income, ethnic/racial diversity, crime rate, and access to mental healthcare providers. We also included the amount of time the participant spent on each Deprexis module to account for the potential impact of engagement with particular modules. Although these values are unknown prior to treatment, this aspect of the model could be useful when predicting outcomes for new patients by forecasting a range of expected outcomes that assume minimal v. maximal treatment engagement. For more detail on how these predictors were obtained, see online Supplementary materials, section 2.0. There were 120 candidate predictors in total used for each machine learning model. For a list of all predictors, see online Supplementary materials, section 3.0.

Learning algorithms and tuning parameters

We selected two popular machine learning algorithms – the elastic net and the random forest – that perform well with samples of this size (<300). While large for a typical psychosocial intervention study, this would be considered small for many machine learning algorithms. The reason is that most machine learners (e.g. gradient boosting machines) require ‘tuning’ of several hyperparameters to achieve good predictive performance, which is typically done by trying out hundreds of different combinations of hyperparameter values by fitting hundreds of models and selecting the one that achieves the best cross-validation performance. This can result in overfitting, not due to an overly complex model, but rather having tested too many models (Ng, Reference Ng1997). As noted by other researchers (Varma and Simon, Reference Varma and Simon2006; Cawley and Talbot, Reference Cawley and Talbot2010), overfitting from model selection will be most severe when the sample of data is small and the number of hyperparameters is large. For small data sets, Cawley and Talbot (Reference Cawley and Talbot2010) recommend avoiding hyperparameter tuning altogether by using an ensemble approach, such as random forests, that performs well without tuning.

While random forests have a few hyperparameters that could be tuned in principle, the default values for these typically work very well in practice: (1) the number of trees was set to 500. As shown by Breiman (Reference Breiman2001), random forests do not overfit as more trees are added, but rather the generalization error converges to a limiting value. Thus, for this parameter, we only needed to verify that the default value was sufficient for the error to reach its plateau (James et al., Reference James, Witten, Hastie and Tibshirani2013). For more detail, see online Supplementary materials, section 4.0. (2) The depth to which trees are grown was set to terminate if the additional split resulted in fewer than five observations in the terminal node. While using fully grown trees can overfit the data, Hastie et al. (Reference Hastie, Tibshirani and Friedman2009) recommend not tuning this parameter because it seldom makes a substantive difference. (3) The number of variables that are randomly sampled when determining a split was set to the recommended value for regression problems, which is 1/3 of the candidate predictors. Tuning this parameter can make a substantive difference in performance, conditional on the prevalence of relevant v. irrelevant variables: this value should be large enough that any subset of variables of this size contains at least one relevant predictor.

The elastic net, on the other hand, has one parameter, λ, the magnitude of the shrinkage penalty that must be tuned with cross-validation, and an additional parameter, α, the proportion of L1 ‘lasso’ penalty (sum of absolute values of all coefficients) v. L2 ‘ridge’ penalty (sum of squared values of all coefficients), that often benefits from a small amount of tuning. For each outcome, we searched over 100 possible values of λ (autogenerated by the model fitting program) and three possible values of α: 0.01 (favoring the inclusion of most variables), 0.99 (favoring sparsity), and 0.5 (an equal mix of both L1 and L2). For reasons already discussed, cross-validation errors used for model selection are prone to optimistic bias. In the case of the elastic net, the models’ linear constraints might be expected to offset any optimistic bias introduced by this minimal amount of tuning. Nonetheless, to obtain an unbiased estimate of test error, we used a nested cross-validation procedure (Varma and Simon, Reference Varma and Simon2006), which will be explained further in the Prediction metrics section. For the HRSD outcome, the value of α that minimized cross-validation error tended to be either 0.01 or 0.5 (mean = 0.31), with optimal λ ranging from 3.9 to 7.6 (mean = 5.7). Sparser models were selected for the SDS and IDAS outcomes, with optimal α tending to be 0.5 but sometimes 0.99 (mean = 0.66 for SDS and 0.63 for IDAS well-being). Shrinkage penalties were stronger for SDS (mean λ = 3.7) than IDAS well-being (mean λ = 0.73), indicating there were fewer relevant predictors of SDS.

Comparing elastic net and random forest

The major appeal of the elastic net is that it offers a regularized version of the familiar linear model, whereas the major appeal of the random forest is that it offers an automated method of detecting non-linear and interaction effects. Importantly, both techniques allow all variables to ‘have their say’ (Hastie et al., Reference Hastie, Tibshirani and Friedman2009, p. 601), i.e. they are able to accommodate multiple weak influences and weed out useless predictors, albeit through different mechanisms. The elastic net does this by directly fitting all predictors to the outcome simultaneously, while an L2 penalty shrinks the coefficients of useful but redundant predictors toward 0 and each other, and an L1 penalty shrinks the coefficients of useless predictors all the way to 0 (Zou and Hastie, Reference Zou and Hastie2005). The random forest achieves a similar end result because each regression tree in the forest ‘sees’ a different one-third of the predictors every time it chooses the best variable with which to recursively partition the data. Thus, the strongest predictors will not always be available, giving weaker predictors a chance to contribute. Since the predictions of the individual trees are averaged to yield an ensemble prediction, the contribution of any one predictor is effectively reduced. Other advantages of the random forest over the elastic net include less sensitivity to the effects of skewed distributions and outliers and an ability to work with the predictors in their natural units; for the elastic net to work, all variables, including dummy codes for factors, must be scaled to have the same mean and variance. On the other hand, if there are true linear relationships, the elastic net will be better at modeling them; the random forest can only approximate linear relationships as an average of step functions.

Our initial plan was simply to evaluate how each of these approaches performed on the Deprexis data individually. However, we later reasoned that averaging the predictions of the random forest with those of the elastic net (which are highly correlated – see online Supplementary materials, section 6.0–6.2) might retain the ‘best of both worlds’ and result in a superior prediction than either approach alone. This is not a novel concept; there are many examples of ‘blending’ or ‘stacking’ machine learners, and this is the basis of the so-called ‘super learner’ algorithm (van der Laan et al., Reference van der Laan, Polley and Hubbard2007).

In this approach, a meta-learner is trained to optimize how models are combined, essentially resulting in a weighted average of model predictions. We would have liked to have used this approach here, but this would have required additional hold-out data or an independent test sample to obtain accurate estimates of model generalization, which our modest sample size could not support. Therefore, we adopted the simpler committee method (Hastie et al., Reference Hastie, Tibshirani and Friedman2009, p. 289) of taking the unweighted average of the predictions from each model. Notably, the random forest itself is an ensemble of very complicated trees (each of which individually overfits the data such that its predictions will not generalize well to the population as a whole), and it already uses the committee method to average these unbiased but variable predictions into a stable, generalizable prediction. Essentially, we are simply adding the elastic net to the random forest ensemble – the final ensemble includes 500 regression trees + 1 elastic net – but with the elastic net's predictions weighted 500 times greater than those of an individual regression tree.

Benchmark models

There is a growing recognition that many prior machine learning studies predicting health-related outcomes have compared machine learning models to relatively weak baseline models, such as the null or no information model (DeMasi et al., Reference DeMasi, Kording and Recht2017). This is a very low threshold for the machine learning models to improve upon, thus producing falsely optimistic conclusions. Rather than a null model, the current study examined whether machine learning ensembles explained additional variance beyond a linear regression ‘benchmark’ model that predicted the clinical outcome with the same assessment at pre-treatment. Thus, for each outcome, we first report variance explained by the benchmark model and then examine whether the ensemble predicts variance not already explained by the benchmark model (i.e. model gain).

Prediction metrics and cross-validation

An important aspect of model performance is how well it performs on cases that it was not trained on. We used 10-fold cross-validation to estimate a predictive R 2 $(R_{{\rm pred}}^2 )$, which is the fraction of variance in previously unseen data that the model is expected to explain. The 10-fold cross-validation was repeated 10 times using different randomized partitions of the data.Footnote 1Footnote Within each repetition, 10 models were fit, each trained to 90% of the data and used to predict the outcomes for the 10% of cases that were withheld. An $R_{{\rm pred}}^2 $ was then calculated based on the residual errors of the holdout predictions and then averaged across the 10 repetitions. This procedure was applied to both the benchmark model (see above) and the ensemble model. For the elastic net model, an additional 10-fold cross-validation procedure was nested within each 90% partition of data used for training and used to select values for the tuning parameters as described in the above section Learning algorithms and tuning parameters. The mean $R_{{\rm pred}}^2 $ of the benchmark model was subtracted from the mean $R_{{\rm pred}}^2 $ of the ensemble model to yield an estimate of predictive gainR 2pred). A 95% CI for predictive gain was estimated as the 0.025 and 0.975 quantiles of the distribution of ΔR 2pred recomputed over 10 000 bootstrap replicates. For the elastic net, these numbers reflect the outer cross-validation, not the nested cross-validation, which tended to show a 1% (range = 0–3%) greater $R_{{\rm pred}}^2 $, indicating that the estimates of error from the tuning procedure incur a slight optimistic bias.

Cross-validation of models was expedited by fitting models to resampled data in parallel on the Wrangler computing cluster provided by the Texas Advanced Computing Center (TACC) at the University of Texas at Austin (https://www.tacc.utexas.edu/). All analyses were implemented in R (version 3.4). Our code made extensive use of the tidyverse (Wickham, Reference Wickham2018) packages dplyr, purrr, and tidyr for general data extraction and transformation. Figures were generated using the packages ggplot2 (Wickham, Reference Wickham2009), gridExtra (Auguie, Reference Auguie2017), and ggmap (Kahle and Wickham, Reference Kahle and Wickham2013). The randomForest (Liaw and Wiener, Reference Liaw and Wiener2002) and glmnet (Friedman et al., Reference Friedman, Hastie and Tibshirani2010) packages were used to implement the machine learning ensembles.

Calculation of variable importance scores

We provide a variable importance metric that was obtained by selecting a sequence of values between the minimum and maximum of each predictor while holding all other predictors constant (at the mean of numeric variables or at the mode of factors), using the final model to make predictions for each of these selected values, and then obtaining the difference between the minimum and maximum prediction. This corresponds to the amount of partial influence that each variable had on the model prediction, in terms of the difference in outcome units that it was able to effect on its own. These predictions are for the final ensemble, thus the prediction for each variable is an average of linear and non-linear associations (from the elastic net and random forests models, respectively) between the predictor variable and outcome. Partial influence for each important predictor is illustrated in the partial dependence plots presented in the Results section.

In the online Supplementary material (sections 6.0–6.2), we also provide variable importance for the elastic net and random forest models separately. For the elastic net, variable importance was quantified as the absolute value of the standardized regression coefficient for each predictor. For the random forest, predictor importance was quantified as the percent mean increase in the residual squared prediction error on the out-of-bag samplesFootnote 2 when a given variable was randomly permuted. In other words, if permuting a variable substantially worsens the quality of the prediction, then it is important; if permutation has no impact on the prediction, then it is not important. We provide both sets of importance metrics because variable importance is not a well-defined concept (Grömping, Reference Grömping2009) and there are a large number of ways of computing variable importance (Wei et al., Reference Wei, Lu and Song2015). To facilitate comparison between the two metrics, both were scaled so that the importance scores of all variables sum to 1. For the most part, the importance metrics identify similar sets of predictors as important.

Missing data

Imputation of missing predictor data was performed using the missForest method (Stekhoven and Bühlmann, Reference Stekhoven and Bühlmann2012), which regresses each variable against all other variables and predicts missing data using random forests. Several studies have demonstrated the superiority of missForest over alternative approaches, such as k-nearest neighbors, multivariate imputation using chained equation, and several other random forest missing data algorithms (van Buuren, Reference van Buuren2007; Waljee et al., Reference Waljee, Mukherjee, Singal, Zhang, Warren, Balis, Marrero, Zhu and Higgins2013; Shah et al., Reference Shah, Bartlett, Carpenter, Nicholas and Hemingway2014). Imputation was only applied to the missing predictors; none of the outcome data were used in the imputation, so there was no opportunity for information about the outcome to ‘leak’ into the predictor values and thus artificially improve prediction.

Results

HRSD

The benchmark model predicted 16.6% of the variance in post-treatment HRSD. The ensemble model predicted an additional 8% of the variance in post-treatment HRSD (see Table 1). The most important predictor variables were pre-treatment depression total score, psychiatric comorbidity, dysthymia, several depression symptom items, usage of several Deprexis modules, disability, treatment credibility, and availability of therapists (see Fig. 2).

Fig. 2. Partial dependence plots for the top 16 predictors of post-treatment interviewer-rated depression symptoms.

Table 1. Prediction of post-treatment depression by linear regression model including only pre-treatment assessment of outcome (benchmark), additional variance explained beyond benchmark model by ensemble model (model gain), and total variance explained.

95% CIs for prediction R 2 were based on the standard error formula applied to the 10 × 10 cross-validation estimates; 95% CIs for gain (the increase in predicted R 2 over benchmark) were estimated by bootstrap.

Not surprisingly, partial dependence plots indicated a fairly linear relationship for pre-treatment HRSD and dysthymia; as pre-treatment depression/dysthymia increased, so did predicted post-treatment HRSD. Psychiatric comorbidity had a more curvilinear relationship, as post-treatment HRSD gently increased with increasing comorbidity until relatively high levels of comorbidity where post-treatment HRSD increased much more quickly. Higher levels of specific symptoms of depression, including slowness, psychic anxiety, and weight loss, were associated with higher post-treatment depression; disability related to psychiatric symptoms had a similar association.

Notably, usage of the relationships, acceptance, and relaxation modules were identified as important predictors. Using these modules for at least 30 min was associated with a 1.2-point greater reduction in HRSD score (all other predictors being equal), which is approximately one-quarter of the mean outcome difference observed for Deprexis-treated v. wait-list groups. As can be seen in many of the partial dependence plots for the 16 highest impact predictors (Fig. 2), associations between predictors and post-treatment HRSD were often non-linear and effects were relatively small (with the exception of the first three variables).Footnote 3 Importance scores are also presented separately for the random forests and elastic net models in online Supplementary materials, section 6.0.

Symptom-related disability

Consistent with prior work, to create a disability outcome, the work, social, and family disability questions (three items in total) from the SDS were averaged to form a single index of symptom-related disability. The benchmark model with pre-treatment disability predicted 20.4% of the variance in post-treatment symptom-related disability. The ensemble model predicted an additional 5% of the variance in post-treatment disability (Table 1).

As can be seen in Fig. 3, the pre-treatment disability composite had the strongest importance score, which was approximately equivalent to the importance of disability in the family domain. Nevertheless, several other variables also contributed to the prediction of disability. Several QIDS-SR items were identified as important predictors, including disinterest, early insomnia, and fatigue. More time spent on both the relaxation and cognitive modules (the benefit tapered off after approximately 60 min on each module) were both associated with lower post-treatment disability. Higher percentage of zip code with Hispanic ethnicity and fewer years in therapy were also associated with lower disability. Importance scores are also presented separately for the random forests and elastic net models in online Supplementary materials, section 6.1.

Fig. 3. Partial dependence plots for the top 16 predictors of post-treatment disability.

Depression-related well-being

The benchmark model with pre-treatment well-being (positive affect) predicted 17.8% of the variance in post-treatment symptom-related well-being. The ensemble model explained an additional 11.6% of the variance in post-treatment well-being (Table 1). As can be seen in Fig. 4, not surprisingly, higher pre-treatment well-being was associated with higher post-treatment well-being.

Fig. 4. Partial dependence plots for the top 16 predictors of post-treatment well-being (low positive affect) symptoms.

Comorbid psychopathology was also an important predictor of post-treatment well-being. The most important forms of comorbidity included mania symptoms, dysthymia, disinterest, and ill temper. Higher perceived treatment credibility and greater confidence that treatment would help were both associated with greater improvements in well-being in a fairly linear fashion (see Fig. 4). Younger age was associated with a better outcome as was paternal anxiety and mental illness. Higher use of the relaxation module was also associated with better post-treatment well-being. Importance scores are also presented separately for the random forests and elastic net models in online Supplementary materials, section 6.2.

Deprexis module usage

A final plot highlights the relative impact of module usage for each of the three outcomes (see Fig. 5). To generate these scores, the module importance scores for each outcome were scaled to sum to 1. A few notable patterns emerge. First, the most important module appeared to be the relaxation module; greater usage was associated with fewer depression symptoms, less disability, and more well-being. In addition, usage of the acceptance and relationship modules were most important for the prediction of HRSD depression symptoms. The cognitive module was important for predicting reductions in disability, as was the diagnosis module. Time spent on most of the other modules was not strongly associated with symptom improvement, at least for the average user. It is important to note that usage of all modules did factor (weakly) into the final prediction, and modules that were not important for the average user might nonetheless be very important when predicting the outcomes for some individuals.

Fig. 5. Importance of Deprexis module usage for predicting post-treatment depression, disability, and well-being (positive affect).

Discussion

This study used an ensemble of random forests regression trees and elastic net machine learning approaches to predict symptom change in response to Deprexis, an Internet intervention for depression. In contrast to many prior studies (DeMasi et al., Reference DeMasi, Kording and Recht2017), we determined whether the ensemble predicted significantly more variance in post-treatment outcomes than a benchmark linear regression model that only included the baseline assessment of the outcome. Candidate predictors ranged from patient demographics, to comorbid psychopathology, past and current treatment utilization, to environmental characteristics based on the participants’ residence (i.e. postal code in the USA). In summary, the ensemble outperformed an autoregressive linear benchmark model for the prediction of interviewer-rated depression symptoms (8% gain in variance explained, 25% total variance explained), symptom-related disability (5% gain in variance explained, 25% total variance explained), and well-being (12% gain in variance explained, 29% total variance explained).

Results provide important new insight into who is most likely to respond to Internet interventions such as Deprexis. Not unexpectedly, pre-treatment assessments were the strongest predictors of each outcome. However, a number of additional predictors, all making relatively small contributions to the prediction, were associated with symptomatic improvement. The ensemble indicated that comorbid psychopathology was an especially important predictor of post-treatment HRSD, particularly total psychopathology and dysthymia (other psychopathology symptoms were also identified as important predictors). This may not be surprising, as past research has indicated that comorbid psychopathology is often associated with poor treatment response (Smits et al., Reference Smits, Minhajuddin, Thase and Jarrett2012). Also consistent with prior research, pre-treatment disability was an important predictor of symptom change (Kessler et al., Reference Kessler, van Loo, Wardenaar, Bossarte, Brenner, Ebert, de Jonge, Nierenberg, Rosellini, Sampson, Schoevers, Wilcox and Zaslavsky2017). High pre-treatment impairment has been shown to predict poor treatment response across both pharmacological and psychological treatment modalities (Trivedi et al., Reference Trivedi, Rush, Wisniewski, Nierenberg, Warden, Ritz, Norquist, Howland, Lebowitz, McGrath, Shores-Wilson, Biggs, Balasubramani and Fava2006; Frank et al., Reference Frank, Cassano, Rucci, Thompson, Kraemer, Fagiolini, Maggi, Kupfer, Shear, Houck, Calugi, Grochocinski, Scocco, Buttenfield and Forgione2011; Jarrett et al., Reference Jarrett, Minhajuddin, Kangas, Friedman, Callan and Thase2013), but does not appear to moderate response to any specific form of treatment (Kessler et al., Reference Kessler, van Loo, Wardenaar, Bossarte, Brenner, Ebert, de Jonge, Nierenberg, Rosellini, Sampson, Schoevers, Wilcox and Zaslavsky2017). Nevertheless, impairment appears to be an important, non-specific predictor of treatment response.

The ensemble was also able to predict additional variance in post-treatment well-being beyond the benchmark model. Comorbid dysthymia, disinterest, depression symptoms, and mania symptoms were all important predictors of well-being. Further, treatment credibility and treatment expectancies were relatively strong predictors of symptom change. Self-guided interventions should consider incorporating interventions that enhance patient expectations about treatment effectiveness and treatment credibility, as initial expectancies have predicted treatment engagement and outcome in psychotherapy and Internet treatments (Greenberg et al., Reference Greenberg, Constantino and Bruce2006). Although positive expectations are sometimes dismissed as mere ‘placebo’ processes, which need to be controlled in trials, we concur with others that such factors can fuel treatment engagement and improve outcomes (Kirsch et al., Reference Kirsch, Wampold and Kelley2016). As with the HRSD, disability across multiple domains was also an important predictor of post-treatment well-being.

A final aim was to examine the differential association of Deprexis module usage to the three post-treatment outcomes. Usage of the relationships, acceptance, and relaxation modules appeared to be the most closely associated with the depression-related outcomes. Notably, this study is among the first to identify specific module dosages that are associated with symptom improvement. For each of the modules, symptom reduction appeared to taper off after approximately 30–50 min of usage. Prior work has found that module completion is modestly associated with improvements in depression (Spek et al., Reference Spek, Cuijpers, Nyklícek, Riper, Keyzer and Pop2007). Much of this prior work has examined a linear relationship between usage and symptom improvement, even though there is evidence that this association may be non-linear (Donkin et al., Reference Donkin, Hickie, Christensen, Naismith, Neal, Cockayne and Glozier2013). One benefit of using random forests together with elastic nets is that random forests can detect non-linear associations without having to specify the form of the association in advance. The use of standardized interventions (as opposed to face-to-face interventions that can have variable implementations) also facilitates these dose–response analyses.

There are several limitations of this study that should be noted. First, future work predicting treatment response would benefit from using larger samples, which would allow for more complex models without overfitting. Although it would be ideal for future work to also include highly dimensional data, such as neuroimaging, hormones, genetics, and microbiota, it would not be desirable to include those data at the cost of large sample size and population diversity. In addition, without comparable data from an alternative clinical intervention, we have no way of gauging the extent to which this model is predicting response to Deprexis specifically v. a response to interventions more generally. We do not yet know whether the results of this model can be used to recommend whether an individual should try Deprexis or not. Finally, although Deprexis has been studied in other countries, this was the first trial of the English version of Deprexis, and many of the important predictors here were either not collected or not applicable to prior European trials. Thus, testing our algorithm with other Deprexis-treated samples was not possible.

Given that the goal of this project was to develop an algorithm that could be used to predict response to a single treatment, one might wonder how this work could be used in real-world clinical settings. Imagine a clinical setting with a large waiting list (not an unusual circumstance in some settings, such as treatment centers at large medical centers or universities). Patient information could be obtained, via an online survey or other methods, and fed into the Deprexis treatment response algorithm. The algorithm may predict several of these new patients will show significant improvement in symptoms following Deprexis, whereas other individuals are predicted to show relatively little symptom change.

As a result of these analyses, Deprexis could be provided right away to the first set of individuals, perhaps allowing for the patients predicted not to respond to Deprexis to be scheduled sooner for more intensive, in-person treatment. Treatment centers would need to decide the threshold for an acceptable amount of predicted change in order to receive Deprexis, and this could potentially dynamically fluctuate depending on demand for services, therapist availability, and other constraints. In addition, while a patient's future usage of the various modules is obviously unknown prior to treatment, this aspect of the model could be used to explore different usage scenarios and recommend which modules are likely to yield the greatest return on an individual patient's time investment. To make this hypothetical application more concrete, we provide case examples of predicted response to treatment, using the machine learning algorithm developed in this study, for two hypothetical patients under two different usage scenarios in online Supplementary materials, section 7.0.

This exercise demonstrates how some patients could be identified as good candidates for this low-intensity, Internet-based treatment, whereas others may not be. Of course, because we have only one treatment, we do not know how these participants might respond to other treatments (or no treatment at all). Ideally, algorithms could be developed for predicting response to different treatments. These algorithms could then be used to determine the optimal treatment(s) for a patient with a given set of attributes. We believe methods used in the current study provide a small but important step toward developing these algorithms and, given the emphasis of machine learning methods for reducing overfitting and increasing generalization to new samples, this may be a better alternative than traditional statistical approaches typically used to detect treatment predictors (for a review, see Cohen and DeRubeis, Reference Cohen and DeRubeis2018). With further refinement, ensemble machine learning methods may facilitate a more efficient mental health care system by helping clinicians optimize treatment delivery so that patients initially receive the treatment with the best likelihood of a positive response for that specific individual.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S003329171800315X.

Author ORCIDs

Christopher G. Beevers https://orcid.org/0000-0002-4480-0902.

Acknowledgements

This study was supported by Gaia AG, the developer and owner of Deprexis, by providing access to the intervention at no cost to the researchers or participants, as well as providing technical support to the research participants as needed. All research data were collected and analyzed independently by researchers affiliated with the University of Texas at Austin. Financial support for this study was also provided to CGB by an NIH award (R21MH110758), an Independent Investigator award from the Brain and Behavior Foundation, and an unrestricted grant from the KCL Foundation, a 501(c)(3) not-for-profit philanthropic foundation that supports mental health programs in Austin, TX, USA. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health. The Texas Advanced Computing Center (TACC) at the University of Texas at Austin provided access to the Wrangler computing cluster to facilitate high-performance computing for this project (https://www.tacc.utexas.edu/). We thank James Hoffman for his help with study co-ordination.

Author contributions

All authors contributed to the research design. JS and DP take responsibility for data analysis and JS and CGB take responsibility for data integrity and interpretation of the results. CGB obtained funding. RP held responsibility for data collection under the supervision of CGB. CGB and RP drafted the paper, and JS, DP, and BM provided critical revisions. All authors approved the final version of the paper for submission.

Conflict of interest

BM is employed at Gaia AG, the developer and owner of Deprexis, the Internet intervention examined in this study. None of the other authors are employed by Gaia AG or have received remuneration for participating in this project or have any other conflicts of interest to declare.

Financial support

National Institute for Mental Health (MH110758) and Brain and Behavior Foundation.

Footnotes

The notes appear after the main text.

1 One may choose the number of folds to be anywhere from two (equivalent to splitting the data in half) to the number of observations (equivalent to ‘leave-one-out’ cross-validation). This choice incurs a bias-variance tradeoff. Fewer folds mean smaller sample sizes for fitting the models and therefore worse fits (greater bias) but more similar performance between different samples (less variance); more folds mean larger sample sizes and therefore better fits (less bias) but more inconsistent performance between different samples (greater variance). Empirically, 10-fold cross-validation seems to offer the best compromise between these two extremes. Specifically, the 10 × 10 cross-validation tests performed here follow the work and recommendations of Bouckaert (Reference Bouckaert, Fawcett and Mishra2003; Reference Bouckaert2004) for reliably comparing the performance of different learning algorithms.

2 The term ‘out of bag’ derives from the term ‘bagging’, which is a portmanteau of ‘bootstrap aggregating’. Because each split in each regression tree in the random forest is based on a bootstrap sample of the original data, there are always a number of samples (about 1/3 on average) that are randomly left out of each ‘bagged’ sample. Hence, for the random forest, aggregating the prediction errors for these ‘out-of-bag’ observations provides an additional valid estimate of how well the machine learner can predict new cases.

3 Note that the self-reported depression symptoms were also assessed with the QIDS-SR. Results for the QIDS-SR are presented in online Supplemental materials, section 5.0.

References

Auguie, B (2017) Miscellaneous Functions for ‘Grid’ Graphics [R package gridExtra version 2.3]. Comprehensive R Archive Network (CRAN). Available at https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/gridExtra/index.html (Accessed 1 October 2018).Google Scholar
Beevers, CG, Pearson, R, Hoffman, JS, Foulser, AA, Shumake, J and Meyer, B (2017) Effectiveness of an internet intervention (Deprexis) for depression in a United States adult sample: a parallel-group pragmatic randomized controlled trial. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 85, 367380.Google Scholar
Bouckaert, RR (2003) Choosing between two learning algorithms based on calibrated tests. In Fawcett, T and Mishra, N (eds), Proceedings of the 20th International Conference on Machine Learning. Menlo Park, CA: aaai.org, pp. 5158.Google Scholar
Bouckaert, RR (2004) Estimating replicability of classifier learning experiments. Paper presented at Twenty-first international conference, Menlo Park, CA Ed. C Brodley, p15. ACM Press.Google Scholar
Breiman, L (2001) Random Forests. Machine Learning 45, 532.Google Scholar
Cawley, GC and Talbot, NLC (2010) On over-fitting in model selection and subsequent selection bias in performance evaluation. Journal of Machine Learning Research 11, 20792107.Google Scholar
Chekroud, AM, Zotti, RJ, Shehzad, Z, Gueorguieva, R, Johnson, MK, Trivedi, MH, Cannon, TD, Krystal, JH and Corlett, PR (2016) Cross-trial prediction of treatment outcome in depression: a machine learning approach. The Lancet Psychiatry 3, 243250.Google Scholar
Cohen, ZD and DeRubeis, RJ (2018) Treatment selection in depression. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology 14, 209236.Google Scholar
Cruz, JA and Wishart, DS (2007) Applications of machine learning in cancer prediction and prognosis. Cancer Informatics 2, 5977.Google Scholar
Cuijpers, P, Ebert, DD, Acarturk, C, Andersson, G and Cristea, IA (2016) Personalized psychotherapy for adult depression: a meta-analytic review. Behavior Therapy 47, 966980.Google Scholar
Dawes, RM, Faust, D and Meehl, PE (1989) Clinical versus actuarial judgment. Science 243, 16681674.Google Scholar
DeMasi, O, Kording, K and Recht, B (2017) Meaningless comparisons lead to false optimism in medical machine learning. Ed. Y-K Jan. Public Library of Science PLoS ONE 12, e018460415.Google Scholar
Devilly, GJ and Borkovec, TD (2000) Psychometric properties of the credibility/expectancy questionnaire. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry 31, 7386.Google Scholar
Donkin, L, Hickie, IB, Christensen, H, Naismith, SL, Neal, B, Cockayne, NL and Glozier, N (2013) Rethinking the dose-response relationship between usage and outcome in an online intervention for depression: randomized controlled trial. Journal of Medical Internet Research 15, e231.Google Scholar
Fernandez, KC, Fisher, AJ and Chi, C (2017) Development and initial implementation of the Dynamic Assessment Treatment Algorithm (DATA). PLoS ONE 12, e017880616.Google Scholar
Fisher, AJ (2015) Toward a dynamic model of psychological assessment: implications for personalized care. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 83, 825836.Google Scholar
Frank, E, Cassano, GB, Rucci, P, Thompson, WK, Kraemer, HC, Fagiolini, A, Maggi, L, Kupfer, DJ, Shear, MK, Houck, PR, Calugi, S, Grochocinski, VJ, Scocco, P, Buttenfield, J and Forgione, RN (2011) Predictors and moderators of time to remission of major depression with interpersonal psychotherapy and SSRI pharmacotherapy. Psychological Medicine 41, 151162.Google Scholar
Friedman, J, Hastie, T and Tibshirani, R (2010) Regularization paths for generalized linear models via coordinate descent. Journal of Statistical Software 33, 122.Google Scholar
Greenberg, RP, Constantino, MJ and Bruce, N (2006) Are patient expectations still relevant for psychotherapy process and outcome? Clinical Psychology Review 26, 657678.Google Scholar
Grömping, U (2009) Variable importance assessment in regression: linear regression versus random forest. The American Statistician 63, 308319.Google Scholar
Hamburg, MA and Collins, FS (2010) The path to personalized medicine. The New England Journal of Medicine 363, 301304.Google Scholar
Hamilton, M (1960) A rating scale for depression. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry 23, 5662.Google Scholar
Hastie, T, Tibshirani, R and Friedman, J (2009) The Elements of Statistical Learning. New York, NY: Springer Science & Business Media.Google Scholar
Huang, SH, LePendu, P, Iyer, SV, Tai-Seale, M, Carrell, D and Shah, NH (2014) Toward personalizing treatment for depression: predicting diagnosis and severity. Journal of the American Medical Informatics Association: JAMIA 21, 10691075.Google Scholar
James, G, Witten, D, Hastie, T and Tibshirani, R (2013) An Introduction to Statistical Learning, vol. 103. New York, NY: Springer New York.Google Scholar
Jarrett, RB, Minhajuddin, A, Kangas, JL, Friedman, ES, Callan, JA and Thase, ME (2013) Acute phase cognitive therapy for recurrent major depressive disorder: who drops out and how much do patient skills influence response? Behaviour Research and Therapy 51, 221230.Google Scholar
Kahle, D and Wickham, H (2013) Ggmap: spatial visualization with ggplot2. The R Journal 5, 144161.Google Scholar
Karyotaki, E, Riper, H, Twisk, J, Hoogendoorn, A, Kleiboer, A, Mira, A, Mackinnon, A, Meyer, B, Botella, C, Littlewood, E, Andersson, G, Christensen, H, Klein, JP, Schröder, J, Bretón-López, J, Scheider, J, Griffiths, K, Farrer, L, Huibers, MJH, Phillips, R, Gilbody, S, Moritz, S, Berger, T, Pop, V, Spek, V and Cuijpers, P (2017) Efficacy of self-guided internet-based cognitive behavioral therapy in the treatment of depressive symptoms: a meta-analysis of individual participant data. JAMA Psychiatry 4, 351359.Google Scholar
Kessler, RC, van Loo, HM, Wardenaar, KJ, Bossarte, RM, Brenner, LA, Ebert, DD, de Jonge, P, Nierenberg, AA, Rosellini, AJ, Sampson, NA, Schoevers, RA, Wilcox, MA and Zaslavsky, AM (2017) Using patient self-reports to study heterogeneity of treatment effects in major depressive disorder. Epidemiology and Psychiatric Sciences 26, 2236.Google Scholar
Khodayari-Rostamabad, A, Reilly, JP, Hasey, GM, de Bruin, H and Maccrimmon, DJ (2013) A machine learning approach using EEG data to predict response to SSRI treatment for major depressive disorder. Clinical Neurophysiology 124, 19751985.Google Scholar
Kirsch, I, Wampold, B and Kelley, JM (2016) Controlling for the placebo effect in psychotherapy: noble quest or tilting at windmills? Psychology of Consciousness: Theory, Research, and Practice 3, 121131.Google Scholar
Liaw, A and Wiener, M (2002) Classification and regression by Random Forest. R News 2, 1822.Google Scholar
Ng, AY (1997) Preventing ‘overfitting’ of cross-validation data. Proceedings of the Fourteenth International Conference on Machine Learning, 245253.Google Scholar
Redlich, R, Opel, N, Grotegerd, D, Dohm, K, Zaremba, D, Bürger, C, Münker, S, Mühlmann, L, Wahl, P, Heindel, W, Arolt, V, Alferink, J, Zwanzger, P, Zavorotnyy, M, Kugel, H and Dannlowski, U (2016) Prediction of individual response to electroconvulsive therapy via machine learning on structural magnetic resonance imaging data. JAMA Psychiatry 73, 557564.Google Scholar
Shah, AD, Bartlett, JW, Carpenter, J, Nicholas, O and Hemingway, H (2014) Comparison of random forest and parametric imputation models for imputing missing data using MICE: a CALIBER study. American Journal of Epidemiology 179, 764774.Google Scholar
Sheehan, DV, Harnett-Sheehan, K and Raj, BA (1996) The measurement of disability. International Clinical Psychopharmacology 11, 89.Google Scholar
Smits, JAJ, Minhajuddin, A, Thase, ME and Jarrett, RB (2012) Outcomes of acute phase cognitive therapy in outpatients with anxious versus nonanxious depression. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics 81, 153160.Google Scholar
Spek, V, Cuijpers, P, Nyklícek, I, Riper, H, Keyzer, J and Pop, V (2007) Internet-based cognitive behaviour therapy for symptoms of depression and anxiety: a meta-analysis. Psychological Medicine 37, 319328.Google Scholar
Stekhoven, DJ and Bühlmann, P (2012) Missforest – non-parametric missing value imputation for mixed-type data. Bioinformatics (Oxford, England) 28, 112118.Google Scholar
Taylor, SE, Lerner, JS, Sage, RM, Lehman, BJ and Seeman, TE (2004) Early environment, emotions, responses to stress, and health. Blackwell Publishing Journal of Personality 72, 13651393.Google Scholar
Trivedi, MH, Rush, AJ, Wisniewski, SR, Nierenberg, AA, Warden, D, Ritz, L, Norquist, G, Howland, RH, Lebowitz, B, McGrath, PJ, Shores-Wilson, K, Biggs, MM, Balasubramani, GK, Fava, M and STAR*D Study Team (2006) Evaluation of outcomes with citalopram for depression using measurement-based care in STAR*D: implications for clinical practice. American Journal of Psychiatry 163, 2840.Google Scholar
Twomey, C, O'Reilly, G and Meyer, B (2017) Effectiveness of an individually-tailored computerised CBT programme (Deprexis) for depression: a meta-analysis. Psychiatry Research 256, 371377.Google Scholar
van Buuren, S (2007) Multiple imputation of discrete and continuous data by fully conditional specification. Statistical Methods in Medical Research 16, 219242.Google Scholar
van der Laan, MJ, Polley, EC and Hubbard, AE (2007) Super learner. Statistical Applications in Genetics and Molecular Biology 6, 123.Google Scholar
Varma, S and Simon, R (2006) Bias in error estimation when using cross-validation for model selection. BMC Bioinformatics 7, 91.Google Scholar
Waljee, AK, Mukherjee, A, Singal, AG, Zhang, Y, Warren, J, Balis, U, Marrero, J, Zhu, J and Higgins, PD (2013) Comparison of imputation methods for missing laboratory data in medicine. BMJ Open 3, e002847.Google Scholar
Watson, D, O'Hara, MW, Chmielewski, M, McDade-Montez, EA, Koffel, E, Naragon, K and Stuart, S (2008) Further validation of the IDAS: evidence of convergent, discriminant, criterion, and incremental validity. Psychological Assessment 20, 248259.Google Scholar
Wei, P, Lu, Z and Song, J (2015) Variable importance analysis: a comprehensive review. Reliability Engineering & System Safety 142, 399432.Google Scholar
Wickham, H (2009) ggplot2: Elegant Graphics for Data Analysis. New York: Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar
Wickham, H (2018) tidyverse: Easily install and load the ‘Tidyverse’. CRAN.R-project.org. Available at https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/tidyverse/index.html (Accessed 1 October 2018).Google Scholar
Zimmerman, M and Mattia, JI (2001) The Psychiatric Diagnostic Screening Questionnaire: development, reliability and validity. Comprehensive Psychiatry 42, 175189.Google Scholar
Zou, H and Hastie, T (2005) Regularization and variable selection via the elastic net. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society: Series B (Statistical Methodology) 67, 301320.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Fig. 1. Geographical distribution of study participants. Note that most participants (approximately 50%) were recruited from the state of Texas.

Figure 1

Fig. 2. Partial dependence plots for the top 16 predictors of post-treatment interviewer-rated depression symptoms.

Figure 2

Table 1. Prediction of post-treatment depression by linear regression model including only pre-treatment assessment of outcome (benchmark), additional variance explained beyond benchmark model by ensemble model (model gain), and total variance explained.

Figure 3

Fig. 3. Partial dependence plots for the top 16 predictors of post-treatment disability.

Figure 4

Fig. 4. Partial dependence plots for the top 16 predictors of post-treatment well-being (low positive affect) symptoms.

Figure 5

Fig. 5. Importance of Deprexis module usage for predicting post-treatment depression, disability, and well-being (positive affect).

Supplementary material: File

Pearson et al. supplementary material

Pearson et al. supplementary material
Download Pearson et al. supplementary material(File)
File 1.7 MB