Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-g8jcs Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-22T18:27:17.613Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Common Genetic Influence on the Relationship Between Gaming Addiction and Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder in Young Adults: A Twin Study

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 October 2024

Seol-Ah Lee
Affiliation:
Kookmin Twin Research Institute, Kookmin University, Seoul, South Korea
Yoon-Mi Hur*
Affiliation:
Kookmin Twin Research Institute, Kookmin University, Seoul, South Korea General College of Education, Kookmin University, Seoul, South Korea
*
Corresponding author: Yoon-Mi Hur; Email: [email protected]

Abstract

Although the relationship between gaming addiction (GA) and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is well established, the causal mechanism of this relationship remains ambiguous. We aimed to investigate whether common genetic and/or environmental factors explain the GA-ADHD relationship. We recruited 1413 South Korean adult twins (837 monozygotic [MZ], 326 same-sex dizygotic [DZ], and 250 opposite-sex DZ twins; mean age = 23.1 ± 2.8 years) who completed an online survey on GA and related traits. Correlational analysis and bivariate model-fitting analysis were conducted. Phenotypic correlation between GA and ADHD in the present sample was 0.55 (95% CI [0.51, 0.59]). Bivariate model-fitting analysis revealed that genetic variances were 69% (95% CI [64%, 73%]) and 68% (95% CI [63%, 72%]) for ADHD and GA respectively. The remaining variances (ADHD: 31%; GA: 32%) were associated with nonshared environmental variances, including measurement error. Genetic and nonshared environmental correlations between ADHD and GA were 0.68 (95% CI [0.62, 0.74]) and 0.22 (95% CI [0.13, 0.30]) respectively, which indicates that shared genes can explain 82% of the phenotypic correlation between ADHD and GA. Our study demonstrated that the ADHD-GA association was largely due to shared genetic vulnerability.

Type
Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BYCreative Common License - NCCreative Common License - ND
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives licence (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/), which permits non-commercial re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided that no alterations are made and the original article is properly cited. The written permission of Cambridge University Press must be obtained prior to any commercial use and/or adaptation of the article.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of International Society for Twin Studies

Gaming addiction (GA) is an increasing public concern for its negative effects on health. The prevalence of internet gaming disorder (IGD) ranges from 0.7% to 25.5% worldwide and is generally high in Asian countries (Mihara & Higuchi, Reference Mihara and Higuchi2017). Although debate is ongoing on the nosology of IGD (Fergusson et al., Reference Ferguson, Coulson and Barnett2011), it is now included in the fifth edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5; American Psychiatric Association, 2013) Appendix as a condition needing further research. The present study defined GA as preoccupation with gaming, tolerance, withdrawal, and inability to control participation in gaming despite problems. This definition followed the description of IGD in the DSM-5. In the literature, however, GA has been used interchangeably with internet and other technology-related disorders such as pathological video gaming, internet addiction, problematic internet use, compulsive internet use (King et al., Reference King, Haagsma, Delfabbro, Gradisar and Griffiths2013). The mixed use of these terms may be due to high correlations among them (Andreassen et al., Reference Andreassen, Billieux, Griffiths, Kuss, Demetrovics, Mazzoni and Pallesen2016; Chiu et al., Reference Chiu, Hong and Chiu2013), but it also reflects a lack of consistent definitions among researchers (Dong & Potenza, Reference Dong and Potenza2014).

Prior research demonstrated a strong relationship between GA and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD; Andreassen et al., Reference Andreassen, Billieux, Griffiths, Kuss, Demetrovics, Mazzoni and Pallesen2016; Kuss et al., Reference Kuss, Griffiths, Karila and Billieux2014). A meta-analysis showed that patients with internet addiction were 2.51 times more likely to be diagnosed with ADHD compared with patients without internet addiction (Wang et al., Reference Wang, Yao, Zhou, Liu and Lv2017). To explain the causal direction of the ADHD-GA relationship, Ko et al. (Reference Ko, Yen, Chen, Yeh and Yen2009) conducted a 2-year longitudinal study on more than 2000 adolescents and found that ADHD was the most significant predictor of the development of internet addiction. Those with attention problem and impulsivity tend to pursue immediate reward (Barkley, Reference Barkley1997), which increases the risk for developing GA. Also, a lack of self-control in those with ADHD may cause them to experience difficulty in controlling gaming, which may make them progress to GA (Ko et al., Reference Ko, Yen, Chen, Yeh and Yen2009). However, other studies suggested that gaming preceded the diagnosis of ADHD. In a longitudinal study on adolescents without ADHD symptoms, Ra et al. (Reference Ra, Cho, Stone, De La Cerda, Goldenson, Moroney, Tung, Lee and Leventhal2018) found that the frequent use of digital media was associated with later development of ADHD symptoms. Additionally, ADHD children with pathological gaming behavior exhibited more severe ADHD symptoms than those without (Shuai et al., Reference Shuai, He, Zheng, Wang, Qiu, Xia, Cao, Lu and Zhang2021). Adolescents who played games reported an increase in attention problems after playing games (Gentile et al., Reference Gentile, Swing, Lim and Khoo2012). These studies reported that gaming may, at least, exacerbate ADHD symptoms. Digital games are fast-paced and frequently offer immediate rewards. People can adapt to the intense cognitive stimulation in games through elevated levels of arousal; thus, gaming may lead people to perceive low-stimuli environments as deprivation, which fosters restlessness or inattention, a key feature of ADHD (Lang et al., Reference Lang, Zhou, Schwartz, Bolls and Potter2000).

Although the direction of causation between ADHD and GA remains unclear, Hygen et al. (Reference Hygen, Skalická, Stenseng, Belsky, Steinsbekk and Wichstrøm2020) showed that a common underlying factor caused the correlations between IGD and mental disorders (e.g., ADHD, depression, and anxiety). However, the authors failed to address the common factor because they employed nontwins in their longitudinal cross-lagged panel analysis. Interestingly, pharmacological studies found that medicine to treat ADHD symptoms was effective in mitigating GA (Han et al., Reference Han, Lee, Na, Ahn, Chung, Daniels, Haws and Renshaw2009; Park et al., Reference Park, Lee, Sohn and Han2016), which indicates that common biological mechanisms may underlie the GA-ADHD association. Using summary statistics from genomewide association studies (GWAS), Vink et al. (Reference Vink, Treur, Pasman and Schellekens2021) found a significant genetic correlation (r = .53) between ADHD and nicotine dependence. Similarly, Koller et al. (Reference Koller, Mitjans, Kouakou, Friligkou, Cabrera-Mendoza and Deak2024) found several common genetic variants between ADHD and substance use disorders (SUDs), confirming genetic contribution to the comorbidity. Given the evidence of the strong relationship between SUDs and GA (Ko et al., Reference Ko, Yen, Chen, Yeh and Yen2009), common genetic factors may mediate the relationship between GA and ADHD.

Twin studies are useful for determining the common genetic and environmental etiology of comorbid disorders such as ADHD and GA. Twin studies of GA are scanty. Nonetheless, extant twin studies consistently demonstrated that internet-related disorders were strongly heritable in adolescents and young adults, with heritability estimates ranging from 48% to 66% (Deryakulu & Ursavas, Reference Deryakulu and Ursavaş2014; Li et al., Reference Li, Chen, Li and Li2014; Vink et al., Reference Vink, Van Beijsterveldt, Huppertz, Bartels and Boomsma2016). These studies found that shared environmental influences were not significant, and that environmental influences were those not shared by family members (hereafter, nonshared environmental influences). In support of twin studies, GWAS identified 72 single nucleotide polymorphisms associated with internet addiction disorder (Haghighatfard et al., Reference Haghighatfard, Ghaderi, Mostajabi, Kashfi, Shahrani, Mehrasa, Haghighat, Farhadi, Momayez Sefat, Shiryazdi, Ezzati, Qazvini, Alizadenik and Moghadam2023).

In contrast to GA, numerous twin studies and GWAS have been conducted to reveal genetic influences on ADHD. A review of twin studies of ADHD suggested a mean heritability of 74% (Faraone & Larsson, Reference Faraone and Larsson2019). In addition, recent GWAS meta-analyses discovered multiple gene variants associated with ADHD (Demontis et al., Reference Demontis, Walters, Athanasiadis, Walters, Therrien, Nielsen, Farajzadeh, Voloudakis, Bendl, Zeng, Zhang, Grove, Als, Duan, Satterstrom, Bybjerg-Grauholm, Bækved-Hansen, Gudmundsson, Magnusson, Baldursson and … Børglum2023). Given the evidence of the strong genetic influences on GA and ADHD, the present study sought to examine common genetic factors that contribute to the GA-ADHD relationship in a sample of South Korean adult twins. To the best of our knowledge, this is the first twin study to report the genetic relationship between ADHD and GA.

Material and Methods

Sample

Twins were invited through a survey link posted on online communities in universities and the websites of the Kookmin Twin Research Institute and twin clubs throughout South Korea in 2022−2023. The survey included informed consent form, ADHD, GA and zygosity questionnaires, and gaming-related questions. A mobile gift coupon was sent to participants who completed the survey. The survey was originally developed for adolescents and young adults. However, only adult twin participants (age: 20 to 35 years old) were included for the present study. The zygosity questionnaire, which was adopted from Ooki et al. (Reference Ooki, Yamada and Asaka1993), included questions on the physical similarity of twins, frequency of confusion about twins, and the self-perception of zygosity. Eight twin pairs were removed from data analysis because their zygosity was ambiguous. The final sample included 1413 twins (mean age: 23.1 ± 2.8 years), consisting of 837 monozygotic (MZ) twins (416 complete pairs and 5 cotwin missing twins), 326 same-sex dizygotic (DZ) twins (162 complete pairs and 2 cotwin missing twins), and 250 opposite-sex DZ twins (125 complete pairs). Females exceeded males (64% vs. 36%), partially because military service is compulsory for South Korean young adult men. Moreover, females tend to participate in online surveys more frequently than males do (Wu et al., Reference Wu, Zhao and Fils-Aime2022). MZ twins outnumbered DZ twins (59% vs. 41%), which likely reflected the twin birth rates in South Korea in the 1990s and early 2000s (Hur, Reference Hur2021).

Measures

Gaming addiction (GA). The Korean Game Addiction scale (Choi et al., Reference Choi, Ryong and Kim2013) was used to assess GA. The scale contains 20 items measuring tolerance, withdrawal, compulsive use of game, impairment of self-control, impairment of daily activities, excessive time consumption for gaming, and continued gaming despite problems in the past year. A sample item includes, ‘I tried to reduce or stop playing the game several times but failed’. The items were rated using a 4-point Likert-type scale (0: not at all true; 3: almost always true). Scores were calculated by summing the responses, with high scores indicating severe GA. The Cronbach’s alpha reliability of the scale in the present sample was .96.

Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD). The Korean version of the Conners’ Adult ADHD Rating Scale (K-CAARS; E. J. Kim, Reference Kim2003) was used to assess ADHD. The K-CAARS includes 26 items measuring inattention, hyperactivity, impulsivity, and self-concept problems. The items were rated using a 4-point Likert-type scale (0: not at all true; 3: almost always true). Scores were calculated by summing the responses, with high scores indicating severe symptoms. The Cronbach’s alpha reliability in the present sample was .94.

Statistical Analyses

We first computed MZ and DZ twin correlations for GA and ADHD, phenotypic correlation between GA and ADHD, and cross-twin cross-trait correlations between GA and ADHD for MZ and DZ twins. Then, we conducted bivariate Cholesky model-fitting analysis to estimate genetic and environmental influences on GA and ADHD, genetic and environmental correlations between GA and ADHD, and bivariate heritability and environmentality.

The twin method involves the decomposition of phenotypic variances and covariances into additive genetic (A), shared environmental (C), and nonshared environmental variances, including measurement error (E). MZ twins share 100% of their genes, while DZ twins share, on average, 50%. Thus, if MZ twin correlation is greater than DZ twin correlation, genetic effects are presumed existing. If DZ twin correlation is greater than half the MZ twin correlation, shared-environmental effects are likely present. MZ twin correlation less than 1 indicates non-shared environmental influences. Cross-twin cross-trait correlation involves correlating the score of twin 1 for one trait (e.g., ADHD) with the score of twin 2 for another trait (e.g., GA). Greater MZ than DZ cross-twin cross-trait correlation indicates genetic contribution to the covariance between two variables. DZ cross-twin cross-trait correlation greater than half the corresponding MZ correlation indicates shared environmental contribution to covariance.

The Cholesky model partitions the covariance of ADHD and GA into additive genetic (A1 and A2), and shared (C1 and C2), and nonshared environmental (E1 and E2) variance components. This model provides genetic, shared environmental, and nonshared environmental influences on each phenotype as well as genetic (r a), shared environmental (r c), and nonshared environmental (r e) correlations between two variables. These correlations indicate the extent to which the same set of genes or shared and non-shared environments influence two variables. Using these correlations, bivariate heritability (the proportion of phenotypic correlation due to additive genetic factors (i.e., [√a1 × r a × √a2]/phenotypic correlation), and bivariate environmentality (the proportion of phenotypic correlation due to environmental factors (i.e., [√e1 × r e × √e2]/phenotypic correlation) can be calculated.

Mx (Neale et al., Reference Neale, Boker, Xie and Maes2003) was used to conduct correlation and model-fitting analysis. Mx produces −2 log likelihood (−2LL). The difference between −2LL of two nested models is distributed as a chi-square (χ2) with degrees of freedom (df) equivalent to the difference in the number of parameters between the two models. The relative goodness-of-fit for nested models was compared with that of the full model, including additive genetic, shared environmental, and nonshared environmental variances and their covariances, to determine the best-fitting model for the data. Parameter estimates were then calculated with 95% confidence intervals using the maximum likelihood method. The best-fitting model was selected based on the log-likelihood ratio test and the Akaike’s information criteria (AIC). Models exhibiting lower AIC were considered more parsimonious and were thus preferred (Akaike, Reference Akaike1987).

Results

Descriptive Statistics

Table 1 presents means and standard deviations of raw scores of ADHD and GA by sex and zygosity. Age was not significantly correlated with ADHD or GA (r < .05). Women had significantly higher mean and variance of ADHD than men. This was consistent with other South Korean university samples (e.g., E. J. Kim, Reference Kim2003; H. Y. Kim et al., Reference Kim, Lee, Kim, Lee and Cho2005) but not with other ethnic groups where no significant sex difference was found (e.g., Stibbe et al., Reference Stibbe, Huang, Paucke, Ulke and Strauss2020). Although sex difference in variance of GA was not significant, men had significantly higher mean than women, consistent with the literature of GA (Mihara & Higuchi, Reference Mihara and Higuchi2017).

Table 1. Means and standard deviations of raw scores of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and gaming addiction (GA) by sex and zygosity

Note: SDs are enclosed in parenthesis. MZ, monozygotic twins; DZ, dizygotic twins. DZ twins include 250 opposite-sex twins.

a Significant mean and variance differences between sexes and between the two zygosity groups at p < .01.

b Significant mean difference between sexes and between the two zygosity groups at p < .01.

c Significant variance difference between the two zygosity groups at p < .01.

Skewness indices were 0.6 for ADHD and 1.7 for GA. Thus, the scores of GA were log transformed, which resulted in a skewness of 0.4. To increase sample size and, thus, statistical power, we performed correlation and model-fitting analyses using data combined across males and females. Because twins are the same age and gender (except opposite-sex pairs), failing to correct for the effects of age and sex when they exist will lead to biased estimation of twin correlation and model parameters (McGue & Bouchard, Reference McGue and Bouchard1984). Prior to correlation and model-fitting analyses, raw scores of ADHD and log-transformed scores of GA were corrected for sex, age, age2, and age × sex effects using multiple regression analysis. The standardized residuals were used in subsequent analysis. Variances of standardized residual scores of ADHD and GA were not significantly different between two zygosity groups.

Correlational Analysis

Figure 1 depicts the results of correlational analysis. MZ and DZ twin correlations were 0.70 (95% CI [0.65, 0.75]) and 0.35 (95% CI [0.24, 0.44]), respectively, for GA and 0.71 (95% CI [0.66, 0.76]) and 0.39 (95% CI [0.29, 0.49]), respectively, for ADHD. For both traits, MZ twin correlations were significantly higher than DZ twin correlations, which indicated that genetic influences were large, whereas shared environmental influences were negligible. The phenotypic correlation between GA and ADHD was substantial (r: 0.55, 95% CI [0.51, 0.59]), which confirmed the significant relationship between the two traits. Cross-twin cross-trait correlations were 0.50 (95% CI [0.42, 0.57]) for MZ and 0.27 (95% CI [0.16, 0.37]) for DZ twins, which suggested that common genetic factors mediated the relationship between GA and ADHD.

Figure 1. Results of correlational analysis.

Note: GA, gaming addiction; ADHD, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder; MZ, monozygotic twins; DZ, dizygotic twins. Error bars represent 95% confidence intervals.

Bivariate Cholesky Model-Fitting Analysis

As means of ADHD and GA were significantly different between MZ and DZ twins, zygosity-specific means were implemented in the Cholesky model. Table 2 presents the results of model-fitting analysis. Although omitting all genetic variances/covariances yielded a significant change in chi-square (model 1: Δχ2: 60.1, Δdf 3, p < .00), omitting all shared environmental variances/covariances did not (model 2: Δχ2: 5.8, Δdf 3, p = .12). These results indicated that shared environmental variances/covariances for ADHD and GA were negligible, consistent with the results of correlational analysis. We individually omitted genetic covariance and nonshared environmental covariance from model 2 (models 3 and 4). The resulting chi-square differences were significant for both models, which indicates that common genetic and nonshared environmental influences contribute to the relationship between ADHD and GA.

Table 2. Results of bivariate Cholesky decomposition model-fitting analysis for gaming addiction (GA) and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD)

Note: -2LL, −2 log likelihood; AIC, Akaike information criterion; df, degrees of freedom. The best-fitting model is indicated in bold.

Figure 2 shows the path coefficients in the best-fitting bivariate model, which should be squared to estimate additive genetic and nonshared environmental influences. Additive genetic variances were 69% (95% CI [64%, 73%]) for ADHD and 68% (95% CI [63%, 72%]) for GA. The remaining variances (ADHD = 31%; GA = 32%) were attributable to nonshared environmental variances, including measurement error. Genetic and nonshared environmental correlations between ADHD and GA reached .68 (95% CI [.62, .74]) and 0.22 (95% CI [.13, .30]), respectively. Bivariate heritability for ADHD and GA was 82% [(√.68 × √.69 × .68)/.55], which indicates that shared genes can explain 82% of the correlation between ADHD and GA. Bivariate environmentality for ADHD and GA was 18% [(√.31 × √.32 × .22)/.55], which suggests that 18% of the correlation was due to common nonshared environmental factors.

Figure 2. Parameter estimates in the best-fitting bivariate Cholesky model. 95% confidence intervals are in parenthesis. A: additive genetic influences, E: nonshared environmental influences. Path coefficients should be squared to estimate additive genetic and nonshared environmental influences.

Note: GA, gaming addiction; ADHD, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder.

We also found genetic and nonshared environmental variances unique to GA. Of 68% of genetic variance for GA, 31% were variance shared with ADHD, and the remaining 37% were the genetic variance unique to GA. Of 32% of the nonshared environmental variance for GA, only 1% were variance shared with ADHD, and the remaining 31% were the nonshared environmental variance unique to GA.

Discussion

Using South Korean adult twins, the present study demonstrated that the common genetic factors mainly influenced the ADHD-GA association. Although common nonshared environmental influence attained statistical significance, it was much smaller than common genetic influence (.68 vs. .22).

The finding of genetic overlap between GA and ADHD indicates that the horizontal pleiotropic effects of genes may be partly responsible for the relationship between the two traits. Horizontal pleiotropic effects of genes occur when the same genetic factors influence multiple traits independently (Paaby & Rockman, Reference Paaby and Rockman2013). ADHD and GA are characterized by a constant need for stimulation and an aversion to delayed rewards (Hinshaw, Reference Hinshaw2018). Neurobiological studies propose that these characteristics are related to low dopaminergic functioning (Weinstein & Lejoyeux, Reference Weinstein and Lejoyeux2020). Thus, genes involved in dopamine regulation likely influence ADHD and GA, simultaneously. Indeed, a recent GWAS (Haghighatfard et al., Reference Haghighatfard, Ghaderi, Mostajabi, Kashfi, Shahrani, Mehrasa, Haghighat, Farhadi, Momayez Sefat, Shiryazdi, Ezzati, Qazvini, Alizadenik and Moghadam2023) illustrated that genes for dopamine pathways, such as DRD4, COMT, and MAOB, which were identified for GA, were shared with ADHD. Furthermore, because ADHD and GA are comorbid with depression, SUDs, and other mental disorders (Ko et al., Reference Ko, Yen, Chen, Yeh and Yen2009; Yen et al., Reference Yen, Ko, Yen, Wu and Yang2007), genes for serotonergic activity may also influence ADHD and GA, leading to genetic correlation.

The genetic correlation found in the present study may result from vertical pleiotropic effects of genes as well. Vertical pleiotropic effects of genes occur when a trait influenced by genetic factors in turn influences another trait by acting as a mediator (Paaby & Rockman, Reference Paaby and Rockman2013). Namely, genetic variants may affect ADHD through GA and vice versa. Koller et al. (Reference Koller, Mitjans, Kouakou, Friligkou, Cabrera-Mendoza and Deak2024) explored the presence of vertical pleiotropy in the relationship between ADHD and SUDs using Mendelian randomization (MR) analysis. Their results suggested bidirectional causality, but genetic effects of SUDs on ADHD were stronger than the reverse. However, in a similar study, Vink et al. (Reference Vink, Treur, Pasman and Schellekens2021) failed to detect genetic effects of nicotine dependence on ADHD.

Interestingly, recent evidence shows that game-based tools that provide cognitive training are effective in decreasing ADHD symptoms (especially, inattention problems) (Peñuelas-Calvo et al., Reference Peñuelas-Calvo, Jiang-Lin, Girela-Serrano, Delgado-Gomez, Navarro-Jimenez, Baca-Garcia and Porras-Segovia2022). Cognitive training can reduce executive function deficits by strengthening neural networks, leading to improved attentional performance in children with ADHD (Peñuelas-Calvo et al., Reference Peñuelas-Calvo, Jiang-Lin, Girela-Serrano, Delgado-Gomez, Navarro-Jimenez, Baca-Garcia and Porras-Segovia2022). However, a meta-analysis suggested that long-term effects of game-based cognitive training were limited (Caselles-Pina et al., Reference Caselles-Pina, Sújar, Quesada-López and Delgado-Gómez2023). Also, because most studies of game-based interventions to date employed patients with ADHD without other psychopathology (Caselles-Pina et al., Reference Caselles-Pina, Sújar, Quesada-López and Delgado-Gómez2023), how these interventions influence patients with GA-ADHD comorbidity is largely unknown. More studies are necessary to resolve the contrasting results between the shared genetic etiology of GA-ADHD comorbidity and game-based interventions to reduce symptoms of ADHD.

Whether GA is a separate clinical entity or a manifestation of underlying psychiatric disorders is controversial (Fergusson et al., Reference Ferguson, Coulson and Barnett2011). Although this study showed high genetic overlap between ADHD and GA, it identified a substantial amount of genetic variance unique to GA. This finding supports the notion that GA may be a separate clinical entity. However, given the comorbidity of GA with many forms of psychopathology (Ko et al., Reference Ko, Yen, Chen, Yeh and Yen2009; Yen et al., Reference Yen, Ko, Yen, Wu and Yang2007), further GWAS and twin studies are necessary to elucidate the genetic architecture of GA and mental disorders.

The present findings have Implications for prevention and intervention strategies. The high genetic correlation between ADHD and GA emphasizes that family members, especially siblings of children with ADHD or GA, are also at risk, and therefore should be targeted for prevention. Common genetic mechanisms also suggest that treating ADHD symptoms may help treat GA or, conversely, treating GA may reduce the severity of ADHD in those with comorbidity.

Our findings should be interpreted with several limitations. First, due to insufficient sample size, we were not able to evaluate sex differences in the genetic and environmental correlations between ADHD and GA. Yen et al. (Reference Yen, Yen, Chen, Tang and Ko2009) reported that women showed higher ADHD-IGD association than men. Thus, future studies should increase sample size and explore sex differences in genetic and environmental correlations. Second, we determined twins’ zygosity using a questionnaire method. A previous study indicated that the misclassification of zygosity can affect genetic and environmental influences (Odintsova et al., Reference Odintsova, Willemsen, Dolan, Hottenga, Martin, Slagboom, Ordoñana and Boomsma2018). Notably, however, twins with ambiguous zygosity were excluded from data analysis to increase the validity of zygosity diagnosis. Third, this study employed self-reported GA and ADHD instead of clinical diagnosis or observational measures. Thus, future studies should replicate the findings with objective measures. Fourth, because GA and ADHD were measured in a single online survey, common method variance (CMV) could have occurred. In the twin modeling, if CMV had occurred, it would have increased nonshared environmental correlation. Thus, the true estimate of nonshared environmental correlation between GA and ADHD is likely lower than the estimate found in the present study. Finally, participants were South Korean adults, which primarily comprised university students. Given the difference in prevalence of GA across populations (H. S. Kim et al., Reference Kim, Son, Roh, Ahn, Kim, Shin, Chey and Choi2022), whether the present results can be generalized to children, clinical samples, or other ethnic groups remains unclear.

In conclusion, the study demonstrated that the ADHD-GA association was largely due to shared genetic vulnerability. This finding could be used to support multivariate genetic approaches, like genomic structural equation modeling or multitrait analysis of GWAS, which could be used to increase power to detect genetic effects for GA.

Acknowledgments

This study was supported by the Korean National Research Foundation grant (NRF2011371B00047) and the Australian Government National Health and Medical Council grant (INV #1172990-RSP). We are grateful to twins for their participation.

Footnotes

*

Director, Associate professor, Kookmin Twin Research Institute, Kookmin University, Seoul, South Korea

References

Akaike, H. (1987). Factor analysis and AIC. Psychometrika, 52, 317332. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02294359 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual for mental disorders (5th ed.). American Psychiatric Association.Google Scholar
Andreassen, C. S., Billieux, J., Griffiths, M. D., Kuss, D. J., Demetrovics, Z., Mazzoni, E., & Pallesen, S. (2016). The relationship between addictive use of social media and video games and symptoms of psychiatric disorders: A large-scale cross-sectional study. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 30, 252262. https://doi.org/10.1037/adb0000160 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barkley, R. A. (1997). Behavioral inhibition, sustained attention, and executive functions: Constructing a unifying theory of ADHD. Psychological Bulletin, 121, 6594. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.121.1.65.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Caselles-Pina, L., Sújar, A., Quesada-López, A., & Delgado-Gómez, D. (2023). Adherence, frequency, and long-term follow-up of video game-based treatments in patients with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A systematic review. Brain and Behavior, 13, e3265. https://doi.org/10.1002/brb3.3265 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chiu, S.-I., Hong, F.-Y., & Chiu, S.-L. (2013). An analysis on the correlation and gender difference between college students´ Internet addiction and mobile phone addiction in Taiwan. International Scholarly Research Notices Addiction, Article 360607. https://doi.org/10.1155/2013/360607 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Choi, H. S., Ryong, J. S., & Kim, K. H. (2013). Development and validation of the Korean Game Addiction Scale for Adults. Korean Journal of Psychological Health, 18, 709726. https://doi.org/10.17315/kjhp.2013.18.4.007 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Demontis, D., Walters, G. B., Athanasiadis, G., Walters, R., Therrien, K., Nielsen, T. T., Farajzadeh, L., Voloudakis, G., Bendl, J., Zeng, B., Zhang, W., Grove, J., Als, T. D., Duan, J., Satterstrom, F. K., Bybjerg-Grauholm, J., Bækved-Hansen, M., Gudmundsson, O. O., Magnusson, S. H., Baldursson, G. … Børglum, A. D. (2023). Genome-wide analyses of ADHD identify 27 risk loci, refine the genetic architecture and implicate several cognitive domains. Nature Genetics, 55, 198208. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41588-022-01285-8 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Deryakulu, D., & Ursavaş, Ö. F. (2014). Genetic and environmental influences on problematic internet use: A twin study. Computers in Human Behavior, 39, 331338. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2014.07.038 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dong, G., & Potenza, M. N. (2014). A cognitive-behavioral model of Internet gaming disorder: Theoretical underpinnings and clinical implications. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 58, 711. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2014.07.005 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Faraone, S. V., & Larsson, H. (2019). Genetics of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Molecular Psychiatry, 24, 562575. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41380-018-0070-0 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ferguson, C. J., Coulson, M., & Barnett, J. (2011). A meta-analysis of pathological gaming prevalence and comorbidity with mental health, academic and social problems. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 45, 15731578. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2011.09.005 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gentile, D. A., Swing, E. L., Lim, C. G., & Khoo, A. (2012). Video game playing, attention problems, and impulsiveness: Evidence of bidirectional causality. Psychology of Popular Media Culture, 1, 6270. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0026969 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haghighatfard, A., Ghaderi, A. H., Mostajabi, P., Kashfi, S. S., Shahrani, M., Mehrasa, M., Haghighat, S., Farhadi, M., Momayez Sefat, M., Shiryazdi, A. A., Ezzati, N., Qazvini, M. G., Alizadenik, A., & Moghadam, E. R. (2023). The first genome-wide association study of internet addiction; revealed substantial shared risk factors with neurodevelopmental psychiatric disorders. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 133, 104393. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2022.104393 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Han, D. H., Lee, Y. S., Na, C., Ahn, J. Y., Chung, U. S., Daniels, M. A, Haws, C. A., & Renshaw, P. F. (2009). The effect of methylphenidate on internet video game play in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 50, 251256. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.comppsych.2008.08.011 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hinshaw, S. P. (2018). Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): Controversy, developmental mechanisms, and multiple levels of analysis. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 14, 291316. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-050817-084917 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hur, Y. M. (2021). Changes in multiple birth rates and parental demographic factors in South Korea during the last four decades: 1981–2019. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 24,163167. https://doi.org/10.1017/thg.2021.23 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hygen, B. W., Skalická, V., Stenseng, F., Belsky, J., Steinsbekk, S., & Wichstrøm, L. (2020). The co-occurrence between symptoms of internet gaming disorder and psychiatric disorders in childhood and adolescence: Prospective relations or common causes? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 61, 890898. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.13289 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kim, E. J. (2003). The validation of Korean adult ADHD scale (K-AADHDS). Korean Journal of Clinical Psychology, 22, 897911.Google Scholar
Kim, H. S., Son, G., Roh, E., Ahn, W., Kim, J., Shin, S., Chey, J., & Choi, K. (2022). Prevalence of gaming disorder: A meta-analysis. Addictive Behaviors, 126, 107183. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2021.107183 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kim, H. Y., Lee, I. S., Kim, J. H., Lee, J. Y., & Cho, S. S. (2005). A preliminary study on reliability and validity of the Conners Adult ADHD Rating Scales-Korean version in college students. Korean Journal of Clinical Psychology, 24, 171185.Google Scholar
King, D. L., Haagsma, M. C., Delfabbro, P. H., Gradisar, M., & Griffiths, M. D. (2013). Toward a consensus definition of pathological video-gaming: A systematic review of psychometric assessment tools. Clinical Psychology Review, 33, 331342. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2013.01.002 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ko, C. H., Yen, J. Y., Chen, C. S., Yeh, Y. C., & Yen, C. F. (2009). Predictive values of psychiatric symptoms for internet addiction in adolescents: A 2-year prospective study. Archives of Pediatrics & Adolescent Medicine, 163, 937943. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpediatrics.2009.159 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koller, D., Mitjans, M., Kouakou, M., Friligkou, E., Cabrera-Mendoza, B., & Deak, J. D. (2024). Genetic contribution to the comorbidity between attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and substance use disorders. Psychiatry Research, 333, 115758. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2024.115758 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuss, D., Griffiths, M., Karila, L., & Billieux, J. (2014). Internet addiction: A systematic review of epidemiological research for the last decade. Current Pharmaceutical Design, 20, 40264052. https://doi.org/10.2174/13816128113199990617 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lang, A., Zhou, S., Schwartz, N., Bolls, P. D., & Potter, R. F. (2000). The effects of edits on arousal, attention, and memory for television messages: When an edit is an edit can an edit be too much? Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media, 44, 94109. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15506878jobem4401_7 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Li, M., Chen, J., Li, N., & Li, X. (2014). A twin study of problematic internet use: Its heritability and genetic association with effortful control. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 17, 279287. https://doi.org/10.1017/thg.2014.32 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McGue, M., & Bouchard, T. J. (1984). Adjustment of twin data for the effects of age and sex. Behavior Genetics, 14, 325343. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01080045 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mihara, S., & Higuchi, S. (2017). Cross-sectional and longitudinal epidemiological studies of Internet gaming disorder: A systematic review of the literature. Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 71, 425444. https://doi.org/10.1111/pcn.12532 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Neale, M. C., Boker, S. M., Xie, G., & Maes, H. H. (2003). Mx: Statistical modeling (6th ed.). Department of Psychiatry, Virginia Commonwealth University.Google Scholar
Odintsova, V. V., Willemsen, G., Dolan, C. V., Hottenga, J., Martin, N. G., Slagboom, P. E., Ordoñana, J. R., & Boomsma, D. I. (2018). Establishing a twin register: An invaluable resource for (behavior) genetic, epidemiological, biomarker, and ‘omics’ studies. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 21, 239252. https://doi.org/10.1017/thg.2018.23 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ooki, S., Yamada, K., & Asaka, A. (1993). Zygosity diagnosis of twins by questionnaire for twins’ mothers. Acta Geneticae Medicae et Gemellologiae, 42, 109115. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0515283600042244 Google ScholarPubMed
Paaby, A., & Rockman, M. V. (2013). The many faces of pleiotropy. Trends in Genetics, 29, 6673.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Park, J. H., Lee, Y. S., Sohn, J. H., & Han, D. H. (2016). Effectiveness of atomoxetine and methylphenidate for problematic online gaming in adolescents with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Human Psychopharmacology Clinical and Experimental, 31, 427432. https://doi.org/10.1002/hup.2559.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peñuelas-Calvo, I., Jiang-Lin, L. K., Girela-Serrano, B., Delgado-Gomez, D., Navarro-Jimenez, R., Baca-Garcia, E., & Porras-Segovia, A. (2022). Video games for the assessment and treatment of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A systematic review. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 31, 520. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00787-020-01557-w CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ra, C. K., Cho, J., Stone, M. D., De La Cerda, J., Goldenson, N. I., Moroney, E, Tung, I., Lee, S. S., & Leventhal, A. M. (2018). Association of digital media use with subsequent symptoms of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder among adolescents. JAMA, 320, 255263. https://doi.org/10.1001/jama.2018.8931 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shuai, L., He, S., Zheng, H., Wang, Z., Qiu, M., Xia, W., Cao, X., Lu, L., & Zhang, J. (2021). Influences of digital media use on children and adolescents with ADHD during COVID-19 pandemic. Global Health, 17, 19. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12992-021-00699-z Google ScholarPubMed
Stibbe, T., Huang, J., Paucke, M., Ulke, C., & Strauss, M. (2020). Gender differences in adult ADHD: Cognitive function assessed by the test of attentional performance. PLoS One. 15, e0240810. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0240810 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vink, J. M., Van Beijsterveldt, T. C., Huppertz, C., Bartels, M, & Boomsma, D. I. (2016). Heritability of compulsive Internet use in adolescents. Addiction Biology, 21, 460468. https://doi.org/10.1111/adb.12218 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vink, J. M., Treur, J. L., Pasman, J. A., Schellekens, A. (2021). Investigating genetic correlation and causality between nicotine dependence and ADHD in a broader psychiatric context. American Journal of Medical Genetics B Neuropsychiatric Genetics, 186, 423429. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajmg.b.32822 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wang, B. Q., Yao, N. Q., Zhou, X., Liu, J., & Lv, Z. (2017). The association between attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder and internet addiction: A systematic review and meta-analysis. BMC Psychiatry, 17, 112. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12888-017-1408-x Google ScholarPubMed
Weinstein, A., & Lejoyeux, M. (2020). Neurobiological mechanisms underlying internet gaming disorder. Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience, 22, 113126. https://doi.org/10.31887/DCNS.2020.22.2/aweinstein CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wu, M. J., Zhao, K., & Fils-Aime, F. (2022). Response rates of online surveys in published research: A meta-analysis. Computers in Human Behavior Reports, 7, 100206. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chbr.2022.100206 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yen, J. Y., Ko, C. H., Yen, C. F., Wu, H. Y., & Yang, M. J. (2007). The comorbid psychiatric symptoms of internet addiction: Attention deficit and hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), depression, social phobia, and hostility. Journal of Adolescent Health, 41, 9398. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jadohealth.2007.02.002 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yen, J. Y., Yen, C. F., Chen, C. S., Tang, T. C., & Ko, C. H. (2009). The association between adult ADHD symptoms and internet addiction among college students: The gender difference. Cyberpsychology & Behavior, 12, 187191. https://doi.org/10.1089/cpb.2008.0113 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Means and standard deviations of raw scores of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and gaming addiction (GA) by sex and zygosity

Figure 1

Figure 1. Results of correlational analysis.Note: GA, gaming addiction; ADHD, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder; MZ, monozygotic twins; DZ, dizygotic twins. Error bars represent 95% confidence intervals.

Figure 2

Table 2. Results of bivariate Cholesky decomposition model-fitting analysis for gaming addiction (GA) and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD)

Figure 3

Figure 2. Parameter estimates in the best-fitting bivariate Cholesky model. 95% confidence intervals are in parenthesis. A: additive genetic influences, E: nonshared environmental influences. Path coefficients should be squared to estimate additive genetic and nonshared environmental influences.Note: GA, gaming addiction; ADHD, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder.