Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-mkpzs Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-22T23:35:29.413Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Impaired conscious and preserved unconscious inhibitory processing in recent onset schizophrenia

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  16 September 2008

V. C. Huddy
Affiliation:
Division of Neuroscience and Mental Health, Imperial College London, UK
A. R. Aron
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of California San Diego, USA
M. Harrison
Affiliation:
Division of Neuroscience and Mental Health, Imperial College London, UK
T. R. E. Barnes
Affiliation:
Division of Neuroscience and Mental Health, Imperial College London, UK
T. W. Robbins
Affiliation:
Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Cambridge, UK
E. M. Joyce*
Affiliation:
Institute of Neurology, University College London, UK
*
*Address for correspondence: Professor E. M. Joyce, Institute of Neurology, Box 19, The National Hospital for Neurology and Neurosurgery, Queen Square, London WC1N 3BG, UK. (Email: [email protected])
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Impairments in inhibitory function have been found in studies of cognition in schizophrenia. These have been linked to a failure to adequately maintain the task demands in working memory. As response inhibition is known to occur in both voluntary and involuntary processes, an important question is whether both aspects of response inhibition are specifically impaired in people with schizophrenia.

Method

The subjects were 33 patients presenting with a first episode of psychosis (27 with schizophrenia and six with schizo-affective disorder) and 24 healthy controls. We administered two motor response tasks: voluntary response inhibition was indexed by the stop-signal task and involuntary response inhibition by the masked priming task. We also administered neuropsychological measures of IQ and executive function to explore their associations with response inhibition.

Results

Patients with schizophrenia compared to healthy controls showed significantly increased duration of the voluntary response inhibition process, as indexed by the stop-signal reaction time (SSRT). By contrast, there were no group differences on the pattern of priming on the masked priming task, indicative of intact involuntary response inhibition. Neuropsychological measures revealed that voluntary response inhibition is not necessarily dependent on working memory.

Conclusions

These data provide evidence for a specific impairment of voluntary response inhibition in schizophrenia.

Type
Original Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2008. The online version of this article is published within an Open Access environment subject to the conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-ShareAlike licence <http<//creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-sa/2.5/>. The written permission of Cambridge University Press must be obtained for commercial re-use.

Introduction

A fundamental problem in schizophrenia research is to determine which of the many cognitive deficits apparent on neuropsychological testing reflect abnormalities in specific cognitive processes rather than general performance decrements (see MacDonald & Carter, Reference MacDonald and Carter2002; Joyce & Huddy, Reference Joyce and Huddy2004). This is important for the future identification of dysfunctional neural processes and their genetic underpinnings as aetiological factors in the development of the disorder (Gottesman & Gould, Reference Gottesman and Gould2003).

Patients with schizophrenia show pronounced deficits in executive function at all stages of the illness (e.g. Pantelis et al. Reference Pantelis, Barnes, Nelson, Tanner, Weatherley, Owen and Robbins1997; Hutton et al. Reference Hutton, Puri, Duncan, Robbins, Barnes and Joyce1998; see also Elvevåg & Goldberg, Reference Elvevåg and Goldberg2000; Nuechterlein et al. Reference Nuechterlein, Barch, Gold, Goldberg, Green and Heaton2004 for reviews). The term executive function encompasses several more discrete cognitive functions, notably working memory and response inhibition, which are thought to interact to optimize performance under changing or novel conditions. It is well established that patients with schizophrenia have impaired working memory, which holds true when the study design or the data analysis controls for general performance decrements (see Barch, Reference Barch2006). More recently, the working memory deficit has taken on added importance in also understanding inhibitory task deficits in schizophrenia. In the Stroop task, the apparent inhibitory effects have been attributed to a failure of context processing (Barch et al. Reference Barch, Carter and Cohen2004); working memory impairments have also been linked to performance on the antisaccade task (Hutton et al. Reference Hutton, Joyce, Barnes and Kennard2002, Reference Hutton, Huddy, Barnes, Robbins, Crawford, Kennard and Joyce2004) and the Wisconsin Card Sorting Task (WCST; Hartman et al. Reference Hartman, Steketee, Silva, Lanning and Andersson2003; Li, Reference Li2004). These studies have highlighted the importance of maintaining complex task demands in working memory while performing tasks thought to index the inhibitory dimension of executive function.

The stop-signal task provides a means to assess the specificity of inhibitory deficits in schizophrenia as inhibitory performance is clearly operationalized as a volitional act to prevent responding. This is a choice reaction time task in which the goal is to perform a speeded response at the onset of a GO stimulus, which is typically a directional arrow. On a minority of trials the procedure is interrupted by a signal to stop after the onset of the GO stimulus and the subject must attempt to prevent any response. Logan & Cowan (Reference Logan and Cowan1984) have described a ‘race’ model of stopping based on the assumption that two processes are operating (a GO process initiating action and a STOP process preventing the action) and that success or failure to stop is determined by whether the GO or STOP process completes first. Within this model the speed of the STOP process may be determined and this is commonly taken as a good indicator of stopping proficiency. The speed of the STOP process can be estimated by manipulating the delay between the onset of the GO stimulus and the STOP signal, termed the stop-signal delay (SSD). One method for manipulating the SSD is to actively move the STOP signal closer or further from the GO stimulus, according to the subject's performance. If the subject fails to stop the delay is decreased on the subsequent trial and vice versa for successful stops. By using this ‘dynamic tracking’ method, performance is intentionally stabilized at a critical criterion set at 50%, when it can be assumed that the STOP and GO processes are completing at about the same time, that is the race between GO and STOP processes is tied. The stop-signal reaction time (SSRT) is then estimated by subtracting the SSD at this point from the GO reaction time (Band et al. Reference Band, van der Molen and Logan2003). Thus, the SSRT is never directly observed; it can only be measured by manipulation of the subject's stopping proficiency so that stopping occurs at chance levels.

Relatively few studies have examined stopping performance in schizophrenia and the findings have been inconsistent. Rubia et al. (Reference Rubia, Russell, Bullmore, Soni, Brammer, Simmons, Taylor, Andrew, Giampietro and Sharma2001), using a simplified version of the stop-signal task in a functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) paradigm, found no performance decrements in patients with schizophrenia. Badcock et al. (Reference Badcock, Michie, Johnson and Combrinck2002), using a version of the task that featured a series of fixed SSDs, found that patients with schizophrenia failed to trigger an inhibitory response as often as controls but the stopping times of triggered responses were the same as controls. Another study using this method found the opposite pattern, with intact triggering of responses and slower stopping times (Enticott et al. Reference Enticott, Ogloff and Bradshaw2007). Only one study has used the more accurate dynamic tracking method (Bellgrove et al. Reference Bellgrove, Chambers, Vance, Hall, Karamitsios and Bradshaw2006) and this was in adolescent-onset schizophrenia. No differences were found between controls and the whole group of patients but SSRT was slower in a subgroup with the undifferentiated subtype of schizophrenia, and then only in left-handed responses. These variable findings contrast with those from other clinical groups tested on the stop-signal task. For example, in a review of the area, Aron & Poldrack (Reference Aron and Poldrack2005) report consistent evidence for abnormal performance in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD).

The stop-signal task indexes voluntary motor inhibition by systematic manipulation of overt auditory cues; the subject is explicitly instructed to inhibit responding. Motor inhibition that occurs at an unconscious level has also been described by Eimer & Schlaghecken (Reference Eimer and Schlaghecken2003) using a masked priming task. Like the stop-signal task, the masked priming task measures choice reaction times of a left or right response to directional arrows. However, response inhibition is demonstrated by covert priming instead of overt action inhibition. Thus there is no stop-signal tone and the subject responds to all stimuli, with both prime and target GO stimuli being directional arrows. A mask immediately follows this prime preventing any conscious processing of the direction of the arrow. This is important because although the primes are intended to produce partial response activation, the absence of conscious processing prevents an overt response. On different trials, the prime and GO arrows are either compatible or incompatible with each other and are presented with differing intervening delays. Compared with incompatible trials, performance benefits are found on compatible trials for a 0-ms delay between prime and target [the positive compatibility effect (PCE)] but performance costs are found when the delay between prime and target is ⩾96 ms [the negative compatibility effect (NCE)]. To elucidate this effect, Eimer & Schlaghecken (Reference Eimer and Schlaghecken1998) recorded the lateralized readiness potential (LRP) during responses. They found that the masked prime partially activated the corresponding response as predicted, but crucially this was followed by an inhibition of the response. These data suggest that a self-inhibition process automatically counters the covert response activation of the prime. When the delay is short, this process does not have time to operate, leading to speeded responses to the target when prime and target are in the same direction; but when the delay is longer, this process does have time to operate, leading to performance costs on compatible trials.

There have been no studies of unconscious response inhibition of this type in schizophrenia. However, it is becoming increasingly apparent that preconscious information processing can be disrupted in schizophrenia when other methods are used. Initial event-related potential (ERP) findings in both auditory (Shelley et al. Reference Shelley, Ward, Catts, Michie, Andrews and Mcconaghy1991) and visual modalities (Butler et al. Reference Butler, Schechter, Zemon, Swartz, Greenstein, Gordon, Schroeder and Javitt2001) have been well replicated. Other findings indicate impairment in the preconscious inhibition of irrelevant sensory information as exemplified by P50 gating (Adler et al. Reference Adler, Pachtman, Franks, Pecevich, Waldo and Freedman1982) and prepulse inhibition (PPI) paradigms (Braff et al. Reference Braff, Stone, Callaway, Geyer, Glick and Bali1978).

In the present study we sought to further clarify the nature of inhibitory function in schizophrenia by administering procedurally similar tasks of inhibition, stop-signal and masked priming, in a group of adults with verified schizophrenia or schizo-affective disorder following a first episode of psychosis. This enabled a direct comparison between the inhibitory control of the same manual response to a directional arrow occurring at the voluntary conscious level and at the involuntary unconscious level. Other neuropsychological measures of executive functioning were additionally obtained to provide novel data describing the relationship between both measures of response inhibition and other established indices of executive function, most importantly working memory.

Method

Participants

Thirty-three patients were recruited as part of a prospective, longitudinal study of first-episode psychosis in West London. Patients eligible were screened using the World Health Organization (WHO) Psychosis Screen (Jablensky et al. Reference Jablensky, Sartorius, Ernberg, Anker, Korten, Cooper, Day and Bertelsen1992) and were recruited if aged between 16 and 50 years, presenting with a psychotic illness for the first time and had received no more than 12 weeks of antipsychotic medication. The diagnosis was ascertained using a structured interview, the diagnostic module of the Diagnostic Interview for Psychosis (DIP; Jablensky et al. Reference Jablensky, McGrath, Herrman, Castle, Gureje, Evans, Carr, Morgan, Korten and Harvey2000), which includes items from the Operational Criteria Checklist for Psychosis (OPCRIT; McGuffin et al. Reference McGuffin, Farmer and Harvey1991) and the WHO Schedules for Clinical Assessment in Neuropsychiatry (SCAN; Wing et al. Reference Wing, Babor, Brugha, Burke, Cooper, Giel, Jablenski, Regier and Sartorius1990). Patients with mental and behavioural disorders due to psycho-active substance abuse or organic disorders were excluded from the study. As part of the longitudinal study, all participants are routinely contacted 1 year later, at which time the diagnosis is reviewed. Twenty-six patients agreed to undergo a repeat diagnostic interview. The diagnostic outcome of the remaining seven patients was established by two psychiatrists (T.R.E.B. and E.M.J.) using the OPCRIT to compile information from the responsible psychiatrists and community psychiatric nurses and the clinical notes. The final DSM-IV diagnoses at follow-up were schizophrenia in 27 patients and schizo-affective disorder in six patients.

These patients were compared to 24 healthy volunteers recruited from the same catchment area as the patients by advertising in local colleges and hospitals. Exclusion criteria were a history of psychiatric illness in themselves or their first-degree relatives, previous head injury or other neurological illness or endocrine disorder affecting brain function, such as epilepsy and thyroid disease, and drug or alcohol abuse. Permission to conduct the study was obtained from Merton, Sutton and Wandsworth, Riverside, and Ealing Research Ethics Committees. All participants gave written informed consent and were paid an honorarium for their time.

Following recruitment, cognitive assessments were performed a mean of 13 (s.d.=17) days after clinical assessment. All patients were being prescribed antipsychotic medication at the time of testing: 31 patients were receiving second-generation antipsychotics (20 olanzapine, nine risperidone, two amisulpiride) and two were receiving a first-generation antipsychotic (haloperidol).

Stop-signal task

The subjects were seated approximately 80 cm in front of the monitor with responses made on a custom-made button box. Subjects used the index and middle finger of their dominant hand. Subjects were first familiarized with the simple left/right discrimination task by performing 16 trials with no stop-signal tone. Each trial began with a centrally presented pale blue circle for 500 ms, after this period the black directional arrow would appear within the circle, and the subjects' task was to make a left/right button press consistent with the arrow. The circle and arrow would remain on the screen until a response was made, or for 1000-ms duration in the absence of a response. Incorrect responses were indicated to subjects by the word ‘WRONG!’ presented in the centre of the screen for 2000 ms. Following each trial a period of 1500 ms elapsed before presentation of the next trial.

Following the practice trials, subjects performed stop-signal trials that were identical to the practice trials in all respects, except on 25% of trials a stop signal would be presented; the stop signal was a 100-ms, 300-Hz tone. Subjects were instructed to withhold responding whenever they heard the stop-signal tone, while still responding as fast as possible to the arrow. They were told that stopping and responding as fast as possible were equally important.

A tracking algorithm was used that adapted the response rating by continuously altering the delay (the SSD) between the target arrow and the stop signal. There were four interleaved staircases, each of which started at a different SSD on their first presentation: 100, 200, 400 and 500 ms. In a trial where a stop signal was presented and the subject failed to stop, the delay of that particular staircase was increased by 50 ms. Where the subject successfully stopped, the delay was decreased by 50 ms. As a result, subjects successfully inhibit on approximately 50% of stop trials. The majority of participants completed five blocks of 64 trials, with two control and four patients completing four blocks. Following each block, subjects were shown a graph depicting their response trial for each block and the number of errors made. They were once again encouraged to respond as quickly as possible while avoiding errors. The response time of no-signal trials was used to derive an estimate of the average response time. The SSRT was determined by calculating the difference between the median response time and the average SSD. The average SSD was computed as a grand average of the average of the last 10 movements of each staircase. The standard deviation of response times and the probability of responding on stop signal trials were also determined for each subject.

Masked motor priming task

The trial sequence is depicted in Fig. 1. Each trial started with a fixation dot at the centre of the screen for 300 ms. The screen was then cleared for 577 ms, followed by the prime stimulus presented at fixation for 32 ms, and then the mask for 100 ms. Stimuli were left- and right-pointing double arrows (<< and >>). Masks were constructed from a 6×5 matrix randomly filled with overlapping lines of differing length and orientation. A new random mask was constructed on each trial. Stimuli were black on a white background. In the zero stimulus onset asynchrony (SOA) condition, the target was presented concurrently with the mask, offset was above or below by 150 pixels. By contrast, in the 150 SOA condition, the mask remained on the screen for 100 ms, followed by the blank screen then the target, again offset either above or below the mask. In both conditions the response period was 1000 ms following target presentation. A period of 1700 ms elapsed before presentation of the next trial for the 150-ms condition and 1850 ms for the 0-ms condition. Subjects used the index and middle finger of their dominant hand and completed one block of 10 practice trials and three blocks of 15 experimental trials. Following each block, subjects were shown a graph depicting their response trial for each block and the number of errors made. Subjects were encouraged to respond as fast as possible while avoiding errors. Subjects' median response times and errors were recorded.

Fig. 1. Experimental trial sequence for the masked priming task for the 150-ms condition.

Neuropsychological assessments

Pre-morbid IQ was assessed using the Wechsler Test of Adult Reading (WTAR; Wechsler, Reference Wechsler2001). Current IQ was estimated from the four-subtest form of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale Third Edition (WAIS-III; Wechsler, Reference Wechsler1999), validated for use in schizophrenia (Blyler et al. Reference Blyler, Gold, Iannone and Buchanan2000). Executive and memory tests were taken from the Cambridge Automated Neuropsychological Test Battery (CANTAB) as follows:

Spatial span (Owen et al. Reference Owen, Downes, Sahakian, Polkey and Robbins1990)

This measures the ability to remember the order of sequences of squares presented on the screen in increasing number.

Spatial working memory (Owen et al. Reference Owen, Downes, Sahakian, Polkey and Robbins1990)

Patients are required to ‘open’ sets of boxes, varying between three and eight in number, to find tokens. Errors are recorded when boxes in which tokens have been found are reopened.

Tower of London planning (Owen et al. Reference Owen, Downes, Sahakian, Polkey and Robbins1990)

Subjects are instructed to move coloured ‘balls’ in an arrangement displayed on the screen to match a goal arrangement.

Attentional set shifting (Owen et al. Reference Owen, Roberts, Polkey, Sahakian and Robbins1991)

Subjects are required to learn a series of visual discriminations along two dimensions. The subject is guided through various learning stages, until the critical extra-dimensional shift (EDS) stage is reached where the previously irrelevant dimension becomes relevant, thus assessing ability to inhibit the previously reinforced dimension. This task may be taken as an analogue of the WCST.

Statistical methods

Comparisons between patient and control groups were analysed with separate ANOVAs, t tests and the χ2 test and Spearman's or Pearson's correlation coefficient where appropriate, using SPSS version 13.0 (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, USA).

Results

Table 1 shows both demographic and neuropsychological data.

Table 1. Demographic characteristics and neuropsychological performance of the patient and control groups. Differences in neuropsychological test performance are shown with pre-morbid IQ (WTAR) as a covariate

M, Male; F, female; WAIS-III, Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale Third Edition; WTAR, Wechsler Test of Adult Reading; EDS, extra-dimensional shift stage; s.d., standard deviation; df, degrees of freedom; n.s., not significant.

Neuropsychological measures

The patients showed lower current and pre-morbid IQ score than the control group. In addition, separate paired-sample t tests for each group revealed a significant fall in current IQ from pre-morbid levels for the patient group [t(32)=3.6, p=0.001] that was not present in controls [t(23)=0.36]. For this reason, to control for the group difference in expected intellectual function, pre-morbid IQ indexed by the WTAR was included as a covariate in subsequent analyses. In these analyses, patients performed significantly worse on spatial span and Tower of London perfect solutions. In addition, significantly more patients failed the EDS stage of the attentional set shifting task.

Stop-signal task

Table 2 shows stop-signal performance for patients and healthy control subjects. Error rates on the GO task were very low in both groups, although patients did make more left/right discrimination errors. The probability of inhibition was very close to 50% in both groups, with no significant difference in their overall probability of inhibition. There was a trend for slower GO responses in the patient group. To control for the influence of generalized slowing or IQ effects on SSRT, an analysis of covariance was carried out with pre-morbid IQ and GO reaction time as covariates. This analysis revealed a robust group effect on SSRT [F(4, 57)=9.2, p<0.01, partial η2=0.15], with patients showing significantly longer SSRT in comparison to controls, with none of the other factors significantly contributing to SSRT.

Table 2. Performance characteristics of the patient and control groups on the stop-signal task. Differences in performance are shown with pre-morbid IQ (WTAR) as a covariate. Median scores for SSRT are given in square brackets

WTAR, Wechsler Test of Adult Reading; RT, reaction time; SSRT, stop-signal reaction time; s.d., standard deviation; df, degrees of freedom; n.s., not significant.

Masked priming task

The data for three control participants were corrupted, therefore the following analysis is for 21 of the control participants. Response times and accuracy data are given in Fig. 2. Four-way mixed ANOVAs were conducted for both mean response times and accuracy, with SOA and compatibility as within-group factors, patients/controls as the between-group factor and pre-morbid IQ as a covariate. The analysis revealed no significant effects for any factor for either response times or accuracy. Subsequent analysis was therefore performed to establish the presence of compatibility and SOA effects for the patient and control groups separately. These analyses revealed a robust SOA×compatibility effect for both patients [F(1, 23)=12.4, p<0.01, partial η2=0.28] and controls [F(1, 23)=14.9, p<0.01, partial η2=0.42]. Further analysis of simple main effects indicated that responses were faster for compatible trials at the short SOA for patients [F(32)=5.6, p<0.05] and controls [F(20)=11.5, p<0.01]. By contrast, at the long SOA, responses were faster for incompatible trials for patients [F(32)=11.2, p<0.01] and controls [F(20)=9.6, p<0.01]. Pearson's correlations between SSRT and NCE were not significant (controls: r=0.01; patients: r=0.05).

Fig. 2. Response time (ms) for the patient group (○) and control group (•) on the right axes and accuracy (% errors) for patients (■) and controls (□) on the left axes: (a) for the 0-ms stimulus onset asynchrony (SOA) trials and (b) for the 150-ms SOA trials.

Relationships between NCE, SSRT, neuropsychological and clinical variables

There were no significant correlations between NCE, SSRT and age in controls or patients. In the patients, neither NCE nor SSRT showed a significant correlation with age at onset of psychosis or length of illness. To explore the relationship between NCE, SSRT and neuropsychological measures, correlation coefficients were calculated for SSRT and NCE with current IQ, planning performance, spatial span, spatial working memory errors and errors on the EDS stage for those subjects who reached this stage of the task (19 controls, 16 patients). As multiple test adjustments were not used for these analysis, they are interpreted as exploratory (see Bender & Lange, Reference Bender and Lange2001). In the controls, there were no significant correlations between either NCE or SSRT and any of the neuropsychological measures. In the patients, there was a significant correlation between SSRT and EDS errors (r=0.38, p<0.05). The distribution of EDS errors is non-linear because of ceiling error scores if the subjects fail this stage. Repeating the correlation analysis using Spearman's ρ gave the same result for this measure (patients: ρ=0.40, p<0.05). All other correlations were not significant.

Discussion

One of the main findings of this study was that patients with schizophrenia were impaired at inhibiting a planned act. Using the dynamic tracking method to match the probability of inhibition in groups of patients and healthy controls, prolonged SSRTs were evident in patients even when group differences in GO reaction times and IQ were taken into account. Previous studies using a method of fixed SSDs have been inconsistent in their findings (Badcock et al. Reference Badcock, Michie, Johnson and Combrinck2002; Enticott et al. Reference Enticott, Ogloff and Bradshaw2007) and a study of adolescent-onset psychosis using the dynamic tracking method found slower stopping proficiency only in a subgroup of patients (Bellgrove et al. Reference Bellgrove, Chambers, Vance, Hall, Karamitsios and Bradshaw2006). Our findings clarify these different results by showing that the stopping process, having been initiated, was significantly slower in a representative group of patients who were adults very early in the course of their illness.

In the same patients, the finding of abnormally prolonged SSRT contrasted with the normal pattern of effects on the masked priming task. This was strikingly demonstrated by a specific pattern of facilitation of reaction time to compatible prime target pairs (the PCE) when the delay between the prime and the target was very short, and inhibition of reaction time to compatible pairs (the NCE) at a longer delay. The NCE is considered to reflect a self-inhibition process generated automatically from within the motor effector system to counter the external response activating effect of priming stimuli (see Eimer & Schlaghecken, Reference Eimer and Schlaghecken2003). Thus, the results of our study indicate that patients with schizophrenia show intact automatic inhibition but impaired voluntary inhibition of activated motor responses.

Our findings also suggest that the locus of motor inhibition abnormalities in schizophrenia lies in executive function, that is in those processes exerting attentional control on performance. Functional neuroimaging studies find that tasks requiring high attentional control, such as working memory, conflict monitoring and response inhibition, activate a network of prefrontal areas involving dorsolateral prefrontal, anterior cingulate and inferior frontal cortex (Duncan & Owen, Reference Duncan and Owen2000) and that the right inferior frontal cortex (IFC) is specifically involved in the inhibitory component of executive processes (Aron et al. Reference Aron, Monsell, Sahakian and Robbins2004). Aron et al. (Reference Aron, Fletcher, Bullmore, Sahakian and Robbins2003a ) showed that the amount of right IFC damage correlated with the SSRT, and Chambers et al. (Reference Chambers, Bellgrove, Stokes, Henderson, Garavan, Robertson, Morris and Mattingley2006) found that transcranial magnetic stimulation of the right IFC caused slowing of the SSRT in healthy subjects. The implication of these findings for our study is that the right IFC is dysfunctional in schizophrenia, a suggestion compatible with a recent neuroimaging study of response inhibition in schizophrenia (Kaladjian et al. Reference Kaladjian, Jeanningros, Azorin, Grimault, Anton and Mazzola-Pomietto2007). As a large number of studies have already shown that dorsolateral prefrontal and cingulate cortex function is impaired in this disorder, our results suggest that all three nodes of the frontal network shown by Duncan & Owen (Reference Duncan and Owen2000) to be active during executive tasks are abnormal in schizophrenia.

By contrast, studies have found that the NCE is abolished in the striatal disorders Huntington's disease (Aron et al. Reference Aron, Schlaghecken, Fletcher, Bullmore, Eimer, Barker, Sahakian and Robbins2003b ) and Parkinson's disease (Seiss & Praamstra, Reference Seiss and Praamstra2004). Sumner et al. (Reference Sumner, Nachev, Morris, Peters, Jackson, Kennard and Husain2007) found that the NCE was also abolished in a patient with a small focal lesion of the supplementary motor area (SMA) but not in patients with larger frontal lesions excluding the SMA. These findings suggest that the SMA fronto-striatal output pathway mediating the NCE is intact in schizophrenia.

Our findings are compatible with other studies of schizophrenia revealing intact priming effects in memory (Barch et al. Reference Barch, Cohen, Servan-Schreiber, Steingard, Cohen, Steinhauer and van Kammen1996; Kazes et al. Reference Kazes, Berthet, Danion, Amado, Willard, Robert and Poirier1999; Perry et al. Reference Perry, Light, Davis and Braff2000). Of particular relevance are studies of the effect of masking on response times in a digit-matching task that found intact subliminal but impaired conscious processing of the same visual material in schizophrenia (Dehaene et al. Reference Dehaene, Artiges, Naccache, Martelli, Viard, Schurhoff, Recasens, Martinot, Leboyer and Martinot2003; Del Cul et al. Reference Del Cul, Dehaene and Leboyer2006). However, our results are at odds with findings in schizophrenia of attenuated PPI, a measure thought to reflect subliminal inhibitory processing (Braff et al. Reference Braff, Stone, Callaway, Geyer, Glick and Bali1978; and see Turetsky et al. Reference Turetsky, Calkins, Light, Olincy, Radant and Swerdlow2007). An increasing number of studies have shown that PPI is under some degree of attentional control at medium to long prepulse to pulse intervals (i.e. around 60 ms) (Dawson et al. Reference Dawson, Hazlett, Filion, Nuechterlein and Schell1993, Reference Dawson, Schell, Hazlett, Nuechterlein and Filion2000; Hazlett et al. Reference Hazlett, Buchsbaum, Haznedar, Singer, Germans, Schnur, Jimenez, Buchsbaum and Troyer1998; Kedzior & Martin-Iverson, Reference Kedzior and Martin-Iverson2007; Neumann, Reference Neumann2007). In addition, an fMRI study found specific activation of the right IFC by the PPI procedure as well as activation of striatum and thalamus (Kumari et al. Reference Kumari, Gray, Geyer, ffytche, Soni, Mitterschiffthaler, Vythelingum, Simmons, Williams and Sharma2003). One explanation is that the NCE and PPI reflect different inhibitory processes, with the NCE being unconscious and mediated by an SMA-striato-thalamic output system, and PPI being more attention demanding and mediated by striato-thalamic outputs emanating from the right IFC, an area already known to be involved in the inhibition of voluntary responses. Other findings of a lack of correlation in the same patients between PPI and another index of inhibition known to be impaired in schizophrenia, P50 suppression (Braff et al. Reference Braff, Light and Swerdlow2007), support the view that there are several forms of inhibition mediated by separate neural processes differentially affected in this disorder.

When we explored the association between the stop-signal and other executive tasks, we found a significant correlation between the SSRT and the number of errors at the EDS stage of the attentional set shifting task. There are several distinct cognitive operations contributing to successful EDS performance, most notably inhibition of a prepotent response set and switching attention. Aron et al. (Reference Aron, Fletcher, Bullmore, Sahakian and Robbins2003a ), in a study of patients with frontal cortex lesions, found a significant and specific positive correlation between the degree of damage to the right IFC and the residual switch cost, a measure of the effect of the previous response set on current performance, thus supporting the role of this area in mediating inhibition when switching between responses. Our finding of a link between the SSRT and attentional set shifting errors in schizophrenia patients implicates a common higher-order inhibitory impairment governing poor performance on both tasks, possibly due to right IFC dysfunction.

We found no correlation between the SSRT and working memory errors in patients or controls. Clark et al. (Reference Clark, Blackwell, Aron, Turner, Dowson, Robbins and Sahakian2007), using the same tasks as us, found significant correlations between the SSRT and spatial working memory errors in groups of patients with adult ADHD and right IFC damage. They argued that the basis of their correlations lies in the inhibitory requirements of both tasks mediated by the right IFC. The lack of correlation between the SSRT and working memory in our patients, together with strong evidence for an impairment in the ability to manipulate information in working memory being a reflection of dorsolateral prefrontal cortex dysfunction in schizophrenia (see Barch, Reference Barch2006), suggests that increased working memory errors and prolonged SSRT reflect independent impairments mediated by separate dysfunctional frontal areas in this disorder. Aside from this issue of specific cortical regions driving distinct cognitive abnormalities in schizophrenia, the current study provides evidence for a wider distinction between impaired cortical function (demonstrated by prolonged SSRT) and intact subcortical function (demonstrated by normal NCE). This suggestion chimes with the conclusion of a recent study that found impaired rapid reward learning but intact learning over a longer time period (Waltz et al. Reference Waltz, Frank, Robinson and Gold2007). Waltz et al. (Reference Waltz, Frank, Robinson and Gold2007) argue that this demonstrates a dissociation between cortical and subcortically mediated cognitive functions because the prefrontal cortex mediates rapid learning of reinforcement contingencies, whereas the striatum plays a key role in gradual learning of stimulus response pairings.

Our patients were all being prescribed atypical antipsychotic medications that antagonize dopamine receptors. Whether this medication influenced performance on these tasks is unclear. Existing evidence predicts that antipsychotic medication should not impair the stopping process. Models of striatal dopamine modulation of learning show that increased dopamine release facilitates appetitive learning (GO) via the direct output pathway whereas dopamine underactivity facilitates suppression of responding following negative reinforcement (NO GO) via the indirect output pathway (Frank et al. Reference Frank, Seeberger and O'Reilly2004). Relevant to stop-signal performance, Aron et al. (Reference Aron, Behrens, Smith, Frank and Poldrack2007) have shown that the stopping process is mediated by a hyperdirect pathway from the right IFC to the subthalamic nucleus that ‘bypasses’ the striatum, and this proposed mechanism is compatible with animal models of stop-signal performance that suggest that dopamine is involved in the GO but not the STOP process (Eagle et al. Reference Eagle, Tufft, Goodchild and Robbins2007). There have been no studies addressing the contribution of dopaminergic mechanisms to the NCE. We cannot therefore rule out a possible normalizing effect of medication on striatal output pathways mediating the intact negative compatibility effect and studies of unmedicated patients are required to clarify this.

In summary, we have found evidence for impaired conscious and preserved unconscious inhibition of activated motor responses in schizophrenia. The observation that task performance and response times were largely equivalent between patients and controls and the lack of association between SSRT and IQ or spatial working memory impairment in the schizophrenia group suggest that this is a specific effect and not due to either a generalized cognitive abnormality or a working memory deficit. These findings further suggest that unconscious inhibitory motor processing is intact in schizophrenia and does not contribute to the slowing of activated volitional motor inhibitory responses. Finally, on the basis of previous work, these findings implicate an abnormality of right IFC function in schizophrenia, thus adding to the evidence that executive impairments in this disorder are mediated by a dysfunctional frontal network, involving dorsolateral prefrontal, cingulate and inferior frontal cortices, that is normally activated during performance of tasks requiring executive function or attentional control.

Acknowledgements

This study was supported by Wellcome Trust programme grant 064607. We are grateful to the consultants and nurses of West London and South West London and St George's Mental Health National Health Service (NHS) Trusts for greatly facilitating the study and to Isobel Harrison and Stan Mutsatsa for patient assessments. We thank Martin Eimer and Friederike Schlaghecken for initial assistance with the masked prime paradigm.

Declaration of Interest

None.

Footnotes

Portions of this study were presented at the International Congress on Schizophrenia Research, Savannah, USA, April 2005.

References

Adler, LE, Pachtman, E, Franks, RD, Pecevich, M, Waldo, MC, Freedman, R (1982). Neurophysiological evidence for a defect in neuronal mechanisms involved in sensory gating in schizophrenia. Biological Psychiatry 17, 639654.Google ScholarPubMed
Aron, AR, Behrens, TE, Smith, S, Frank, MJ, Poldrack, RA (2007). Triangulating a cognitive control network using diffusion-weighted magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) and functional MRI. Journal of Neuroscience 27, 37433752.Google Scholar
Aron, AR, Fletcher, PC, Bullmore, ET, Sahakian, BJ, Robbins, TW (2003 a). Stop-signal inhibition disrupted by damage to right inferior frontal gyrus in humans. Nature Neuroscience 6, 115116.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aron, AR, Monsell, S, Sahakian, BJ, Robbins, TW (2004). A componential analysis of task-switching deficits associated with lesions of left and right frontal cortex. Brain 127, 15611573.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aron, AR, Poldrack, RA (2005). Cognitive neuroscience of response inhibition: relevance for genetic research in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biological Psychiatry 57, 12851291.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aron, AR, Schlaghecken, F, Fletcher, PC, Bullmore, ET, Eimer, M, Barker, R, Sahakian, BJ, Robbins, TW (2003 b). Inhibition of subliminally primed responses is mediated by the caudate and thalamus: evidence from functional MRI and Huntington's disease. Brain 126, 713723.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Badcock, JC, Michie, PT, Johnson, L, Combrinck, J (2002). Acts of control in schizophrenia: dissociating the components of inhibition. Psychological Medicine 32, 287297.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Band, GPH, van der Molen, MW, Logan, GD (2003). Horse-race model simulations of the stop-signal procedure. Acta Psychologica 112, 105142.Google Scholar
Barch, DM (2006). What can research on schizophrenia tell us about the cognitive neuroscience of working memory? Neuroscience 139, 7384.Google Scholar
Barch, DM, Carter, CS, Cohen, JD (2004). Factors influencing Stroop performance in schizophrenia. Neuropsychology 18, 477483.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barch, DM, Cohen, JD, Servan-Schreiber, D, Steingard, S, Cohen, JD, Steinhauer, SS, van Kammen, DP (1996). Semantic priming in schizophrenia: an examination of spreading activation using word pronunciation and multiple SOAs. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 105, 592601.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bellgrove, MA, Chambers, CD, Vance, A, Hall, N, Karamitsios, M, Bradshaw, JL (2006). Lateralized deficit of response inhibition in early-onset schizophrenia. Psychological Medicine 36, 495505.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bender, R, Lange, S (2001). Adjusting for multiple testing – when and how? Journal of Clinical Epidemiology 54, 343349.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blyler, CR, Gold, JM, Iannone, VN, Buchanan, RW (2000). Short form of the WAIS-III for use with patients with schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research 46, 209215.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Braff, D, Stone, C, Callaway, E, Geyer, M, Glick, I, Bali, L (1978). Pre-stimulus effects on human startle reflex in normals and schizophrenics. Psychophysiology 15, 339343.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Braff, DL, Light, GA, Swerdlow, NR (2007). Prepulse inhibition and P50 suppression are both deficient but not correlated in schizophrenia patients. Biological Psychiatry 61, 12041207.Google Scholar
Butler, PD, Schechter, I, Zemon, V, Swartz, SG, Greenstein, VC, Gordon, J, Schroeder, CE, Javitt, DC (2001). Dysfunction of early-stage visual processing in schizophrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry 158, 11261133.Google Scholar
Chambers, CD, Bellgrove, MA, Stokes, MG, Henderson, TR, Garavan, H, Robertson, IH, Morris, AP, Mattingley, JB (2006). Executive ‘brake failure’ following deactivation of human frontal lobe. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 18, 444455.Google Scholar
Clark, L, Blackwell, AD, Aron, AR, Turner, DC, Dowson, J, Robbins, TW, Sahakian, BJ (2007). Association between response inhibition and working memory in adult ADHD: a link to right frontal cortex pathology? Biological Psychiatry 61, 13951401.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dawson, ME, Hazlett, EA, Filion, DL, Nuechterlein, KH, Schell, AM (1993). Attention and schizophrenia: impaired modulation of the startle reflex. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 102, 633641.Google Scholar
Dawson, ME, Schell, AM, Hazlett, EA, Nuechterlein, KH, Filion, DL (2000). On the clinical and cognitive meaning of impaired sensorimotor gating in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Research 96, 187197.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dehaene, S, Artiges, E, Naccache, L, Martelli, C, Viard, A, Schurhoff, F, Recasens, C, Martinot, ML, Leboyer, M, Martinot, JL (2003). Conscious and subliminal conflicts in normal subjects and patients with schizophrenia: the role of the anterior cingulate. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 100, 1372213727.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Del Cul, A, Dehaene, S, Leboyer, M (2006). Preserved subliminal processing and impaired conscious access in schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry 63, 13131323.Google Scholar
Duncan, J, Owen, AM (2000). Common regions of the human frontal lobe recruited by diverse cognitive demands. Trends in Neurosciences 23, 475483.Google Scholar
Eagle, DM, Tufft, MR, Goodchild, HL, Robbins, TWR (2007). Differential effects of modafinil and methylphenidate on stop-signal reaction time task performance in the rat, and interactions with the dopamine receptor antagonist cis-flupenthixol. Psychopharmacology 192, 193206.Google Scholar
Eimer, M, Schlaghecken, F (1998). Effects of masked stimuli on motor activation: behavioral and electrophysiological evidence. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance 24, 17371747.Google Scholar
Eimer, M, Schlaghecken, F (2003). Response facilitation and inhibition in subliminal priming. Biological Psychology 64, 726.Google Scholar
Elvevåg, B, Goldberg, TE (2000). Cognitive impairment in schizophrenia is the core of the disorder. Critical Reviews in Neurobiology 14, 121.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Enticott, PG, Ogloff, JR, Bradshaw, JL (2007). Response inhibition and impulsivity in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Research 157, 251254.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frank, MJ, Seeberger, LC, O'Reilly, RC (2004). By carrot or by stick: cognitive reinforcement learning in parkinsonism. Science 306, 19401943.Google Scholar
Gottesman, II, Gould, TD (2003). The endophenotype concept in psychiatry: etymology and strategic intentions. American Journal of Psychiatry 160, 636645.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hartman, M, Steketee, MC, Silva, S, Lanning, K, Andersson, C (2003). Wisconsin Card Sorting Test performance in schizophrenia: the role of working memory. Schizophrenia Research 63, 201217.Google Scholar
Hazlett, EA, Buchsbaum, MS, Haznedar, MM, Singer, MB, Germans, MK, Schnur, DB, Jimenez, EA, Buchsbaum, BR, Troyer, BT (1998). Prefrontal cortex glucose metabolism and startle eyeblink modification abnormalities in unmedicated schizophrenia patients. Psychophysiology 35, 186198.Google Scholar
Hutton, SB, Huddy, V, Barnes, TRE, Robbins, TW, Crawford, TJ, Kennard, C, Joyce, EM (2004). The relationship between antisaccades, smooth pursuit, and executive dysfunction in first-episode schizophrenia. Biological Psychiatry 56, 553559.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hutton, SB, Joyce, EM, Barnes, TRE, Kennard, C (2002). Saccadic distractibility in first-episode schizophrenia. Neuropsychologia 40, 17291736.Google Scholar
Hutton, SB, Puri, BK, Duncan, LJ, Robbins, TW, Barnes, TRE, Joyce, EM (1998). Executive function in first-episode schizophrenia. Psychological Medicine 28, 463473.Google Scholar
Jablensky, A, McGrath, J, Herrman, H, Castle, D, Gureje, O, Evans, M, Carr, V, Morgan, V, Korten, A, Harvey, C (2000). Psychotic disorders in urban areas: an overview of the Study on Low Prevalence Disorders. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry 34, 221236.Google Scholar
Jablensky, A, Sartorius, N, Ernberg, G, Anker, M, Korten, A, Cooper, JE, Day, R, Bertelsen, A (1992). Schizophrenia: manifestations, incidence and course in different cultures. A World Health Organization ten-country study. Psychological Medicine. Monograph Supplement 20, 197.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Joyce, E, Huddy, V (2004). Defining the cognitive impairment in schizophrenia. Psychological Medicine 34, 11511155.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaladjian, A, Jeanningros, R, Azorin, JM, Grimault, S, Anton, LC, Mazzola-Pomietto, P (2007). Blunted activation in right ventrolateral prefrontal cortex during motor response inhibition in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research 97, 184193.Google Scholar
Kazes, M, Berthet, L, Danion, JM, Amado, I, Willard, D, Robert, P, Poirier, MF (1999). Impairment of consciously controlled use of memory in schizophrenia. Neuropsychology 13, 5461.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kedzior, KK, Martin-Iverson, MT (2007). Attention-dependent reduction in prepulse inhibition of the startle reflex in cannabis users and schizophrenia patients – a pilot study. European Journal of Pharmacology 560, 176182.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kumari, V, Gray, JA, Geyer, MA, ffytche, D, Soni, W, Mitterschiffthaler, MT, Vythelingum, GN, Simmons, A, Williams, SCR, Sharma, T (2003). Neural correlates of tactile prepulse inhibition: a functional MRI study in normal and schizophrenic subjects. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging 122, 99113.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Li, CSR (2004). Do schizophrenia patients make more perseverative than non-perseverative errors on the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test? A meta-analytic study. Psychiatry Research 129, 179190.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Logan, G, Cowan, WB (1984). On the ability to inhibit thought and action: a theory of an act of control. Psychological Review 91, 295327.Google Scholar
MacDonald, AW, Carter, CS (2002). Cognitive experimental approaches to investigating impaired cognition in schizophrenia: a paradigm shift. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology 24, 873882.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McGuffin, P, Farmer, A, Harvey, I (1991). A polydiagnostic application of operational criteria in studies of psychotic illness. Development and reliability of the OPCRIT system. Archives of General Psychiatry 48, 764770.Google Scholar
Neumann, DL (2007). Prepulse inhibition of the startle blink reflex is modulated during a memory task requiring prepulses to be encoded for later report. International Journal of Psychophysiology 63, 5563.Google Scholar
Nuechterlein, KH, Barch, DM, Gold, JM, Goldberg, TE, Green, MF, Heaton, RK (2004). Identification of separable cognitive factors in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research 72, 2939.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Owen, AM, Downes, JJ, Sahakian, BJ, Polkey, CE, Robbins, TW (1990). Planning and spatial working memory following frontal-lobe lesions in man. Neuropsychologia 28, 10211034.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Owen, AM, Roberts, AC, Polkey, CE, Sahakian, BJ, Robbins, TW (1991). Extra-dimensional versus intra-dimensional set shifting performance following frontal-lobe excisions, temporal-lobe excisions or amygdalo-hippocampectomy in man. Neuropsychologia 29, 9931006.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pantelis, C, Barnes, TRE, Nelson, HE, Tanner, S, Weatherley, L, Owen, AM, Robbins, TW (1997). Frontal-striatal cognitive deficits in patients with chronic schizophrenia. Brain 120, 18231843.Google Scholar
Perry, W, Light, GA, Davis, H, Braff, DL (2000). Schizophrenia patients demonstrate a dissociation on declarative and non-declarative memory tests. Schizophrenia Research 46, 167174.Google Scholar
Rubia, K, Russell, T, Bullmore, ET, Soni, W, Brammer, MJ, Simmons, A, Taylor, E, Andrew, C, Giampietro, V, Sharma, T (2001). An fMRI study of reduced left prefrontal activation in schizophrenia during normal inhibitory function. Schizophrenia Research 52, 4755.Google Scholar
Seiss, E, Praamstra, P (2004). The basal ganglia and inhibitory mechanisms in response selection: evidence from subliminal priming of motor responses in Parkinson's disease. Brain 127, 330339.Google Scholar
Shelley, AM, Ward, PB, Catts, SV, Michie, PT, Andrews, S, Mcconaghy, N (1991). Mismatch negativity: an index of a preattentive processing deficit in schizophrenia. Biological Psychiatry 30, 10591062.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sumner, P, Nachev, P, Morris, P, Peters, AM, Jackson, SR, Kennard, C, Husain, M (2007). Human medial frontal cortex mediates unconscious inhibition of voluntary action. Neuron 54, 697711.Google Scholar
Turetsky, BI, Calkins, ME, Light, GA, Olincy, A, Radant, AD, Swerdlow, NR (2007). Neurophysiological endophenotypes of schizophrenia: the viability of selected candidate measures. Schizophrenia Bulletin 33, 6994.Google Scholar
Waltz, JA, Frank, MJ, Robinson, BJ, Gold, JM (2007). Selective reinforcement learning deficits in schizophrenia support predictions from computational models of striatal-cortical dysfunction. Biological Psychiatry 62, 756764.Google Scholar
Wechsler, D (1999). Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale – Third Edition. Psychological Corporation: London.Google Scholar
Wechsler, D (2001). Wechsler Test of Adult Reading. Psychological Corporation: London.Google Scholar
Wing, JK, Babor, T, Brugha, T, Burke, J, Cooper, JE, Giel, R, Jablenski, A, Regier, D, Sartorius, N (1990). SCAN. Schedules for Clinical Assessment in Neuropsychiatry. Archives of General Psychiatry 47, 589593.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Fig. 1. Experimental trial sequence for the masked priming task for the 150-ms condition.

Figure 1

Table 1. Demographic characteristics and neuropsychological performance of the patient and control groups. Differences in neuropsychological test performance are shown with pre-morbid IQ (WTAR) as a covariate

Figure 2

Table 2. Performance characteristics of the patient and control groups on the stop-signal task. Differences in performance are shown with pre-morbid IQ (WTAR) as a covariate. Median scores for SSRT are given in square brackets

Figure 3

Fig. 2. Response time (ms) for the patient group (○) and control group (•) on the right axes and accuracy (% errors) for patients (■) and controls (□) on the left axes: (a) for the 0-ms stimulus onset asynchrony (SOA) trials and (b) for the 150-ms SOA trials.