Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-7cvxr Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-23T06:06:00.441Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Third language acquisition in the Nordic context

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 November 2024

Guro Busterud*
Affiliation:
Department of Linguistics and Scandinavian Studies, University of Oslo, Postboks 1102, Blindern, 0317 Oslo, Norway
Anne Dahl
Affiliation:
Department of Language and Literature, NTNU, Postboks 8900, Torgarden, 7491 Trondheim, Norway
Kjersti Faldet Listhaug
Affiliation:
Department of Language and Literature, NTNU, Postboks 8900, Torgarden, 7491 Trondheim, Norway
*
Corresponding author: Guro Busterud; Email: [email protected]

Abstract

Type
Editorial
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of The Nordic Association of Linguists

The second issue of volume 49 (2026) of the Nordic Journal of Linguistics will be a special issue dedicated to theoretical, empirical, and methodological work on the acquisition of a third language (L3) in the Nordic context. The issue will be edited by Guro Busterud, Anne Dahl and Kjersti Faldet Listhaug.

A defining characteristic of the human species is our capacity for learning language, and, importantly, for learning more than one language in the course of our lives. There is growing awareness of the ubiquitousness of multilingualism, and in language acquisition research there is an increasing number of studies devoted to the study of how humans learn more than two languages. The study of L3 acquisition is concerned with the acquisition of a new linguistic system in an already bi- or multilingual mind. This includes situations of acquisition of a third language in speakers of two or more first languages (L1s) as well as acquisition of a second or more additional language (L3/Ln).

Traditionally, the distinction between a second language (L2) and an L3/Ln has not been at the forefront in research about the acquisition of the societal language for newcomers to a linguistic community. However, in a Nordic context, this distinction is particularly relevant in light of the ubiquitousness of English as a second language: many immigrants to the Nordic countries already speak at least a native language and English before learning the societal language. The same is the case for those learning a Nordic language as a foreign language. Moreover, the populations of most of the Nordic countries typically speak English in addition to their native language(s) before learning any other additional languages such as the traditional foreign languages. Finally, English is increasingly learned as an L3/Ln by individuals who already speak more than one (native or non-native) language. Finally, depending on country and specific geographical area, other languages may also be obligatory in education.

Main areas studied in L3 research have been cross-linguistic influence (CLI) or transfer from previously acquired languages, particularly in the field of syntax and morphology, in the lexicon, and also within phonology (Cabrelli Amaro et al. Reference Cabrelli Amaro, Flynn and Rothman2012, Angelovska & Hahn Reference Angelovska and Hahn2017, Cabrelli et al. Reference Cabrelli, Chaouch-Orozco, Miguel Pereira Soares, Puig-Mayenco and Rothman2023). Questions addressed include factors that affect CLI/transfer as well as the directionality of transfer. An important body of research has addressed theoretical questions pertaining to the source of transfer, such as typological similarity and language status (Hermas Reference Hermas2010, Bardel & Falk Reference Bardel and Falk2012, Rothman Reference Rothman2015), as well as the question of wholesale vs. property-based CLI/transfer (Slabakova Reference Slabakova2017, Westergaard et al. Reference Westergaard, Mitrofanova, Mykhaylyk and Rodina2017). Recent work has emphasized methodological approaches to the theoretical questions within the field, and the need for a more varied methodological approach to help further our understanding of L3/Ln acquisition (Puig-Mayenco et al. Reference Puig-Mayenco, González Alonso and Rothman2020, Westergaard et al. Reference Westergaard, Mitrofanova, Rodina and Slabakova2023). Moreover, questions addressing the dynamic nature of multilingual language learning have gained importance (de Bot & Jaensch Reference de Bot and Jaensch2015).

In this NJL special issue, we invite contributions on L3/Ln acquisition in the Nordic context and/or involving Nordic languages. We welcome studies of different linguistic domains, learning contexts, target languages, and learner populations, and with varied methodologies.

The deadline for submissions is 1 October 2025. Peer review will take place during winter 2025 and spring 2026. All submissions should be made through NJL’s ScholarOne/Manuscript Central site https://mc.manuscriptcentral.com/njl. Prior to submission, authors should consult the Journal’s Instructions for Contributors, at https://www.cambridge.org/core/journals/nordic-journal-of-linguistics/information/instructions-contributors, and follow those guidelines for manuscript formatting. Please contact Guro Busterud () if you have any questions about this special issue.

References

Angelovska, Tanja & Hahn, Angela (eds.). 2017. L3 syntactic transfer: Models, new developments and implications. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bardel, Camilla & Falk, Ylva. 2012. The L2 status factor and the declarative/procedural distinction. In Cabrelli Amaro et al. (eds.), 6178.Google Scholar
de Bot, Kees & Jaensch, Carol. 2015. What is special about L3 processing? Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 18(2). 130144.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cabrelli, Jennifer, Chaouch-Orozco, Adel, Jorge González Alonso, Miguel Pereira Soares, Sergio, Puig-Mayenco, Eloi & Rothman, Jason (eds.). 2023. The Cambridge handbook of third language acquisition, 219242. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cabrelli Amaro, Jennifer, Flynn, Suzanne & Rothman, Jason (eds.). 2012. Third language acquisition in adulthood, 6178. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hermas, Abdelkader. 2010. Language acquisition as computational resetting: Verb movement in L3 initial state. International Journal of Multilingualism 7(4). 343362.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Puig-Mayenco, Eloi, González Alonso, Jorge & Rothman, Jason. 2020. A systematic review of transfer studies in third language acquisition. Second Language Research 36(1). 3164.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rothman, Jason. 2015. Linguistic and cognitive motivations for the Typological Primacy Model (TPM) of third language (L3) transfer: Timing of acquisition and proficiency considered. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 18(2). 179190.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Slabakova, Roumyana. 2017. The scalpel model of third language acquisition. International Journal of Bilingualism 21(6). 651665.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Westergaard, Marit, Mitrofanova, Natalia, Mykhaylyk, Roksolana & Rodina, Yulia. 2017. Crosslinguistic influence in the acquisition of a third language: The Linguistic Proximity Model. International Journal of Bilingualism 21(6). 666682.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Westergaard, Marit, Mitrofanova, Natalia, Rodina, Yulia & Slabakova, Roumyana. 2023. Full Transfer Potential in L3/Ln acquisition: Crosslinguistic influence as a property-by-property process. In Cabrelli et al. (eds.), 219242.Google Scholar